Introduction

Since the time of Alzheimer himself [2], two proteins, beta-amyloid (Aβ) deposited in senile plaques, and tau deposited in neurofibrillary tangles, have become tantamount to Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [9]. Neurofibrillary tangles progress throughout the brain in a stereotypic pattern defined by the Braak staging scheme [9]. This staging scheme correlates with cognitive impairment and brain atrophy in AD [18, 20, 44]. However, a significant proportion of patients with AD pathology remain clinically normal up to the time of death despite the presence of both neurofibrillary tangles and senile plaques [15]. Little is known about this phenomenon and whether other proteins may also be playing a role.

A third protein, the TAR DNA-binding protein of 43 kDa (TDP-43), has, however, recently been found to be present in the brains of subjects with pathologically diagnosed AD [3, 4, 8, 14, 24, 26, 43]. TDP-43 is an RNA-binding protein that functions in exon skipping and is identified in an abnormal phosphorylated state in cellular inclusions [11]. TDP-43 is associated with neurodegeneration and cognitive impairment [34] yet it is unknown whether TDP-43 plays any role in what has been considered “the AD neurodegenerative process” or whether TDP-43 could help explain why some patients remain clinically normal, while others do not, despite both having similar degrees of AD pathology.

The primary aim of this study was to determine whether TDP-43 is independently associated with cognitive impairment and brain atrophy in AD, and hence an important contributor to the AD phenotype. A secondary aim was to determine whether cognitive impairment and atrophy would correlate with TDP-43 burden or the distribution of TDP-43 (number of brain regions showing TDP-43 immunoreactivity).

Materials and methods

Study design and participants

All cases were identified from the Mayo Clinic neuropathological database, Rochester, MN and fulfilled the following inclusion criteria: (1) intermediate–high probability AD diagnosis according to the National Institute on Aging and the Reagan Institute criteria (NIA-Reagan) [47], (2) Braak neurofibrillary tangle stage IV–VI [9] and (3) available formalin-fixed paraffin blocks of brain tissue regions. 346 cases were identified: 4 were excluded for not having available paraffin blocks, resulting in 342 cases being included in this study. All subjects were prospectively recruited and followed in the Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center or Patient Registry between 1992 and 2010.

All subjects had undergone a clinical evaluation by a dementia specialist, completed neuropsychological testing and were determined to be cognitively normal or cognitively impaired before death. The determination of cognitive status was based on consensus of a team of scientists utilizing data generated from detailed clinical and neuropsychological evaluations. For this study, we analyzed the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [17] as a measure of general cognitive impairment, Clinical Dementia Rating Scale Sum-of-Boxes (CDR-SB) [31] as a measure of functional impairment, Boston Naming Test (BNT) [27] as a measure of confrontational naming, memory subscale of the Dementia Rating Scale (mDRS) [29] as a measure of loss of episodic memory, brief questionnaire version of the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI-Q) [28] as a measure of behavioral impairment, and cognitive status, at the final evaluation before death. Apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotyping was performed, as previously described [13, 23].

Neuropathological examinations were performed according to recommendations of the Consortium to Establish a Registry for AD (CERAD) [30]. Every specimen was assigned a Braak stage [9] using modified Bielschowsky silver stain, on the basis of the earliest appearance of neurofibrillary tangles. Lewy bodies and infarcts were documented, and Aβ burden measured using CERAD recommendations [30]. Hippocampal sclerosis (HpScl) was diagnosed if neuronal loss in the subiculum and CA1 regions of the hippocampus was out of proportion to the burden of neurofibrillary tangles [16].

This study was approved by the Mayo Clinic IRB. All patients or their proxies provided written informed consent before participating in any research activity.

Pathological analysis

Amygdala blocks were sectioned and immunostained for TDP-43 (polyclonal antibody MC2085 that recognizes a peptide sequence in the 25-kDa C-terminal fragment [48]) with a DAKO-Autostainer (DAKO-Cytomaton, Carpinteria, CA) and 3, 3′-diaminobenzidine as the chromogen. Sections were lightly counterstained with Hematoxylin. Amygdala sections were screened (by DWD) to assess for the presence of TDP-43 immunoreactive neuronal cytoplasmic inclusions, dystrophic neurites, or neuronal intranuclear inclusions (Fig. 1). We screened the amygdala as the amygdala has been shown to be the first region affected in AD by TDP-43 pathology [19, 26]. Any AD case not showing TDP-43 immunoreactivity in the amygdala was considered TDP-negative (Fig. 2a), while any AD case showing any amount of TDP-43 immunoreactivity in the amygdala was considered TDP-positive (Fig. 2b–d). Hence, amygdaloid positivity was all that was necessary to call an individual AD case TDP-43 positive. For TDP-positive cases, we sectioned additional paraffin blocks of the middle frontal, superior temporal, and inferior parietal cortices, nucleus basalis, hippocampus, midbrain and medulla using the same protocol as described above for the amygdala. The following 14 distinct brain regions per case were reviewed simultaneously with a multi-headed microscope (by DWD and KAJ) for TDP-43 immunoreactivity: amygdala, entorhinal cortex, subiculum, hippocampal dentate fascia, occipitotemporal cortex, inferior temporal cortex, basal forebrain, insula, ventral striatum, frontal lobe, basal ganglia, substantia nigra, midbrain tegmentum and inferior olive. A region was considered positive if TDP-43 immunoreactive lesions were observed at 20× magnification screening the entire region, with subsequent confirmation at 40× magnification. The number of cases with TDP-43 immunoreactivity for each of the 14 regions is shown in Fig. 3.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Pathological assessment of TDP-43 immunoreactive inclusions. TDP-43 immunoreactive inclusions identified in the subjects with Alzheimer disease include neuronal cytoplasmic inclusions that were variable in size with some being asterisk-like and small (a), while others were larger, round and more Pick-body like (b). Dystrophic neurites were predominantly thin and thread-like (c) although in some instances there were large thick dystrophic neurites (d). In the CA1 region of the hippocampus, particularly in the cases with widespread TDP-43 immunoreactivity, there were fine neurites (e). Many of these cases with widespread TDP-43 immunoreactivity also had neuronal intranuclear inclusions that were either cat-eye like in appearance (f) or were more rounded (g). TDP-43 immunoreactivity, high-power mag ×40. Dentate fascia of the hippocampus (a–b), frontotemporal neocortices (cd), CA1 region of the hippocampus (e), entorhinal cortex (f) and occipitotemporal cortex (g)

Fig. 2
figure 2

The range of TDP-43 immunoreactivity observed in the amygdala. Cases without TDP-43 immunoreactivity in the amygdala (a) were classified as TDP-negative, while cases showing any amount of TDP-43 immunoreactivity (bd) were classified as TDP-positive. Amygdala TDP-43 immunoreactivity varied and included cases with scant (b), moderate (c) and severe (d) immunoreactivity. TDP-43 immunoreactivity at mag ×20

Fig. 3
figure 3

Histograms showing the prevalence of TDP-43 by region (a) and quantitative TDP-43 burden in the dentate gyrus (b). Only TDP-positive subjects are shown for both histograms. Therefore, as would be expected based on our inclusion criteria, 100 % of the subjects had amygdala involvement (a). Involvement in the other 13 regions was variable with the frontal lobe and basal ganglia being involved in the least number of subjects. In the bottom histogram (b), TDP-43 burden was highly skewed to the right. As reported, there was a strong correlation between TDP-43 burden in the dentate gyrus and number of regions affected

To ensure antibody sensitivity, we additionally screened amygdala sections from 10 % of the TDP-negative cases using a different antibody against phosphorylated TDP-43 peptide (1:5,000 rabbit polyclonal anti-human phosphoserine 409/410). None of the cases that had initially screened negative with the polyclonal antibody MC2085 showed TDP-43 immunoreactivity with the phosphorylated antibody, ensuring excellent sensitivity of MC2085.

TDP-43 burden was assessed in the hippocampal dentate fascia using the Aperio slide scanner and a customized color deconvolution algorithm enabling the detection of any abnormal TDP-43 (Fig. 4). The dentate fascia was selected for the burden analysis since the dentate fascia has been demonstrated to be most strongly associated with memory loss [36]. TDP-43 immunostained sections of the posterior hippocampus at the level of the lateral geniculate were scanned at ultra-resolution on the ScanScope XT (Aperio Technologies, Vista, CA). This instrument permits scanning of the entire slide from which large areas of interest can be annotated using ImageScope version 11.2 (Aperio Technologies, Vista, CA). The method greatly increases the sampling frame compared to some other image analysis systems that are limited to the field of view of the microscope or require image tiling [38]. The entirety of the dentate fascia was assessed to quantitatively determine TDP-43 immunohistochemical burden. To operationalize annotation of the dentate fascia, the ruler tool was used to measure 125 μm across the granule cell layer to molecular layer to avoid quantification variability resulting from tissue sectioning differences. Any dust or dirt particles or tissue folds were excluded using the negative trace tool. Annotated layers were analyzed in Spectrum version 11.2 (Aperio Technologies, Vista, CA) using a custom-designed color deconvolution algorithm, as previously described [22]. After applying the color deconvolution algorithm, each high-resolution image was reviewed independently by two investigators (MM & AL) to ensure that only abnormal TDP-43 was being measured. Cases where the algorithm was unable to separate abnormal TDP-43 from normal nuclear TDP-43, were removed from further analysis of TDP-43 burden (n = 20). TDP-43 burden was then expressed as the area of immunoreactive pixels to the total area of the annotated region.

Fig. 4
figure 4

TDP-43 burden assessment. TDP-43 immunoreactive inclusions in the dentate fascia were assessed quantitatively to determine TDP-43 burden. High-resolution images created with the Aperio slide scanner were utilized to detect abnormal TDP-43 using a custom-designed color deconvolution algorithm. Top panel shows TDP-43 immunoreactive inclusions, while bottom panel shows the results of the color deconvolution algorithm in which abnormal TDP-43 is depicted in red

MRI analysis

Two-hundred and forty-eight subjects had an antemortem volumetric MRI performed with a standardized protocol [21]. The MRI closest to death was selected for each subject to correspond to the clinical data. Two-hundred and twenty subjects were scanned at 1.5 T. A reference group for volumetric analysis included 46 healthy controls that had antemortem MRI and normal pathological diagnosis (Braak 0-III, NIA-Reagan no/low) and were TDP-negative (mean age = 82 ± 6.46 % female). Gradient non-linearity and intensity non-uniformity corrections were applied to all MRI.

Gray matter volumes of specific regions of interest were calculated using atlas-based parcellation in SPM5 and the automated anatomic labeling atlas [42], as previously described [45]. Volumes were calculated for medial temporal regions (hippocampus, amygdala and entorhinal cortex) and association cortices (fusiform gyrus, medial and lateral frontal and parietal lobes and lateral temporal lobe). Left and right volumes were averaged. Regional volumes were scaled by total intracranial volume [46] to correct for head size. Gray matter atrophy was also assessed at the voxel level using voxel-based morphometry (VBM) [6]. Standard preprocessing steps were employed, including normalization to a customized template, unified segmentation [7], modulation and smoothing at 8 mm full width at half-maximum. A VBM “full-factorial” (i.e., ANCOVA) model was used to compare TDP-negative and TDP-positive subjects to controls (p < 0.05 using family-wise error correction), and to each other (uncorrected p < 0.001). Comparisons were performed separately at Braak IV, V and VI. Age, gender and total intracranial volume were included as covariates in all analyses.

Statistical analysis

We used linear, binary logistic and ordinal logistic regression models to estimate associations between outcome variables and TDP-43. In our first modeling analysis, we treated TDP-43 status as a binary predictor.

To better understand the relationship between TDP-43 and cognition and brain atrophy while taking into account the Braak neurofibrillary tangle stage, we stratified by Braak stage and tested for additive associations and interactions between TDP-43 and Braak using linear or logistic regression adjusted for age at death. This analysis allowed us to investigate whether the association between TDP-43 and cognition/brain atrophy differed across Braak stage IV, V and VI. We extended this analysis in two ways to account for any potential genetic or other pathological confounders: (1) to account for confounding effects due to APOE, infarctions, Lewy bodies, neuritic plaques (CERAD) we included these variables as covariates in our models; (2) To address the issue of whether the TDP-43 effects were wholly or partly mediated by HpScl we added this factor to the models and evaluated the resulting HpScl-adjusted TDP-43 associations. We encountered the problem of logistic regression separation in models involving the cognitive impairment outcome. This was because all 104 TDP-positive subjects at Braak stage VI were cognitively impaired and thus in this subgroup there were no “non-events”. To address this problem, we used weakly informative variance stabilizing data augmentation priors for the coefficient corresponding to the Braak VI by TDP-43 interaction [40]. This method is very closely related to the popular approach of adding one success and one failure when estimating a proportion [1]. Our priors specified that on the odds ratio scale 95 % of the prior probability was in the range 1/16–16 which serves to conservatively shrink estimates toward the null.

For our secondary analyses we modeled the outcome variables as a function of TDP-43 burden and as a function of the number of brain regions positive for TDP-43 (i.e., distribution). These analyses were limited only to subjects who were TDP-positive. Quantitative TDP-43 burden was expressed as a percentage of pixels showing immunoreactivity. We rank transformed the numeric value because of extreme right skewness (Fig. 3). So that a 1-unit increase was more interpretable, we divided the rank-transformed value by 40 to obtain a uniformly distributed predictor with a max (and range) of ~4. In these analyses we assessed three models that included: age at death alone; age at death and Braak stage given our interest in Braak stage; and age at death, APOE, Braak stage, CERAD, Lewy bodies, infarcts and HpScl as adjustment covariates to remove the potential confounding effect of all these variables.

Results

One-hundred and ninety-five cases (57 %) were TDP-positive. After accounting for age at death, the TDP-positive subjects had a higher proportion of APOE ε4 carriers, and worse performance on MMSE, CDR-SB, BNT and mDRS than the TDP-negative subjects (Table 1). The TDP-positive group had a lower proportion of subjects who were cognitively normal at death (2 vs 19 %). The TDP-positive group had higher Braak stage, and higher proportions of Lewy bodies and HpScl. Volumes of all medial temporal structures and fusiform gyrus were smaller in the TDP-positive group compared to the TDP-negative group, with no differences observed in other association cortices.

Table 1 Characteristics of TDP-negative and TDP-positive subjects

Group differences adjusting for potential confounders

After accounting for age at death, CERAD, APOE ε4, infarcts, Braak stage and Lewy bodies, TDP-43 had an additive effect on cognitive impairment, MMSE, mDRS, CDR-SB and BNT (Fig. 5). When we further account for HpScl in our mediation analysis, the additive effect of TDP-43 on cognitive impairment and BNT remained significant with similar trends persisting for MMSE, mDRS and CDR-SB (Fig. 5). An interaction, in addition to an additive effect beyond Braak, was observed for MMSE and CDR-SB. Specifically, a greater difference was observed between TDP-positive and TDP-negative groups at Braak IV and V. For example, a greater than 4-point difference on MMSE was observed at Braak stages IV and V between TDP-positive and TDP-negative subjects, while less than a 2-point difference was observed for MMSE at Braak stage VI.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Models for clinical variables. Estimates and 95 % CIs for the mean difference for each term in the primary model along with the estimates and 95 % CIs for the hippocampal sclerosis and TDP terms from the mediation model. P values for the primary models summarize a test of an additive effect of TDP after accounting for Braak stage, age, CERAD, APOE ε4, infarctions, and Lewy bodies (P add) and a test of an interaction between TDP and Braak stage versus the additive model (P int). The p values for the mediation model additionally account for HpScl. The cognitive impairment outcome was modeled with logistic regression and the estimates were transformed to the percentage scale with age set to the mean (85). All other outcomes were modeled using linear regression

Similar to the clinical outcomes, after accounting for age at death, CERAD, APOE ε4, infarcts, Braak stage and Lewy bodies, TDP-43 had an additive effect on hippocampal, amygdala and entorhinal cortex volumes (Fig. 6). When we further account for HpScl in our mediation analysis, the additive effect of TDP-43 on hippocampal volume remained significant with similar trends persisting for the amygdala and entorhinal cortex volumes (Fig. 6). No added interactions were observed for these medial temporal volumes. Conversely, interactions, but no additive effects of TDP-43 were observed on the association cortex volumes.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Models for neuroimaging variables. Estimates and 95 % CIs for the mean volume difference for each term in the primary model along with the estimates and 95 % CIs for the hippocampal sclerosis and TDP terms from the mediation model. The p values for the primary models summarize the test of an additive effect of TDP after accounting for Braak stage, age, CERAD, APOE ε4, infarctions, and Lewy bodies (P add) and a test of an interaction between TDP and Braak stage versus the additive model (P int). The p values for the mediation model additionally account for HpScl. All outcomes were modeled using linear regression

On VBM, the TDP-positive AD subjects showed progressive worsening of volume loss with increasing Braak, when compared to the 46 healthy control subjects (Fig. 7). The TDP-negative AD subjects showed no volume loss at Braak IV and only a tiny area of focal loss at Braak V, when compared to the healthy controls. When the TDP-positive and TDP-negative subjects were directly compared, the TDP-positive subjects showed greater medial temporal loss than the TDP-negative subjects at all Braak stages. Conversely, the TDP-negative subjects showed no regions of greater loss than the TDP-positive subjects, except at Braak VI, where more parieto-occipital loss was observed.

Fig. 7
figure 7

Regional patterns of gray matter volume loss in TDP-negative and TDP-positive AD subjects within Braak stages IV, V and VI. The TDP-negative and TDP-positive subjects are compared to pathologically normal controls (corrected using family-wise error at p < 0.05) and compared to each other (uncorrected p < 0.001). Results are shown on three-dimensional renderings of the brain

TDP-43 burden and distribution

A strong correlation was observed between TDP-43 burden and distribution (Spearman correlation = 0.74, p < 0.001). After accounting for age at death, CERAD, APOE ε4, infarcts, Braak stage, Lewy bodies and HpScl, TDP-43 burden and TDP-43 distribution were associated with many clinical and neuroimaging variables (Tables 2, 3). For all these associations, clinical performance worsened and volume loss increased with higher TDP-43 burden in the dentate gyrus, and as more regions were affected by TDP-43.

Table 2 Summarizing the association between quantitative TDP-43 burden and outcome variables
Table 3 Summarizing the association between number of TDP-43 regions (distribution) and outcome variables

Discussion

In this large clinico-imaging pathological study, we demonstrate that TDP-43 is associated with the major features of AD: memory loss and medial temporal atrophy. In fact, when AD pathology was less severe (i.e. Braak Stages IV and V), the absence of TDP-43 was strongly associated with normal cognition. Consequently, TDP-43 appears to play an important role in the cognitive and neuroimaging characteristics that have been linked to AD.

The findings from this study challenge the perception that Aβ and tau are the only important proteins accounting for the clinical features of AD by demonstrating that TDP-43 also has an effect on the core clinical and neuroimaging features considered pathognomonic for AD. Importantly, the effects of TDP-43 on cognition and atrophy occurred after taking into account AD-related pathogenetic potential confounders, including Braak stage, and hence tau, the presence of Lewy bodies, Aβ, infarctions and APOE ε4. The effects of TDP-43 also persisted after taking into account HpScl. Therefore, although HpScl likely influences cognition and brain atrophy when present in AD [10, 33, 35], the effects of TDP-43 on the clinical and imaging variables assessed in our cohort were not mediated by HpScl. This suggests that TDP-43 itself is an important factor. The fact that TDP-43 had an effect on these outcome variables at Braak IV, which represents mild AD, is evidence that TDP-43 is not just a feature of severe pathology. In fact, the differences between TDP-positive and TDP-negative groups were most striking at Braak IV and V. One explanation for this finding is that TDP-43 plays a more important role in the early stages of AD, but once tau deposition is widespread, i.e., Braak VI, the importance of TDP-43 becomes somewhat overshadowed. Alternatively, it is also possible that the effects of TDP-43 at Braak VI are just as strong as they are at Braak IV and V, but are more difficult to detect since the range of cognitive values at Braak VI are likely to be truncated given that most subjects are severely affected at this stage.

One of the most important findings of this study was that the absence of TDP-43 was strongly associated with normal cognition despite subjects with and without TDP-43 having similar degrees of AD pathology. In our sample, approximately one-third of the TDP-negative Braak IV and V subjects were cognitively normal, compared to only 4 % of the TDP-positive Braak IV and V subjects. Given that the TDP-positive Braak IV and V groups showed greater medial temporal atrophy than the TDP-negative Braak IV and V subjects, it appears that the presence of TDP-43 creates a synergistic effect with the AD pathology. A similar effect has also been previously reported to occur with vascular pathology [39] and with argyrophilic grain disease [25, 41]. It therefore appears that the resilience of some subjects to AD pathology can be overpowered by the presence of TDP-43. These findings help further shed light on those subjects that remain cognitively unimpaired despite having AD pathology [5, 15, 37]. Previous studies have suggested that cognitively resilient subjects may have lower burdens of Aβ fibrillar plaques, oligomeric Aβ deposits or hyperphosphorylated tau in synapses [37], or preserved densities of presynaptic terminals and dentritic spines [5], although none of these studies assessed TDP-43. We now know that such subjects are also less likely to have TDP-43.

An association was also identified between APOE ε4, a major risk factor for AD [12], and TDP-43. The proportion of subjects with the APOE ε4 allele was higher in the presence of TDP-43 across all Braak stages. At Braak VI, for example, approximately three-quarters of the TDP-positive subjects had the APOE ε4 allele, a higher proportion than typically observed in AD [32]. It is therefore probable that APOE ε4 increases the risk of TDP-43 pathology, an association not previously recognized. However, APOE ε4, like Lewy body disease, Aβ deposition, infarctions and HpScl, was not a confounder of the observed associations with TDP-43.

An intriguing observation was the atypical characteristics of the Braak VI TDP-negative group. This group showed widespread atrophy, yet the atrophy pattern was unusual when compared to the Braak VI TDP-positive group, with more atrophy in cortical association regions and less atrophy in medial temporal regions. This group of subjects was approximately 10 years younger than all other groups and is reminiscent of the hippocampal sparing variant of AD, which typically is associated with younger age, relatively greater cortical atrophy, an atypical distribution of neurofibrillary tangles [32, 46] and absence of TDP-43 [8]. It is unlikely that TDP-43 is protective against involvement of the cortex and more likely that the greater cortical atrophy in the TDP-negative subjects is being driven by a greater burden of cortical tau in these younger subjects [32, 46]. Conversely, the greater hippocampal atrophy observed in the older TDP-positive subjects is likely associated with the presence of TDP-43. It therefore appears that tau and TDP-43 may have distinct effects in AD as shown in these models from our cohort that presumably generalizes to the general population (Fig. 8).

Fig. 8
figure 8

Summaries illustrating the relationship between normal cognition, MMSE and hippocampal volume loss, and Braak stage, and TDP-43 status in AD. In the absence of TDP-43, there is a progressively higher proportion of subjects with cognitive impairment across Braak stages with the steepest changes in proportion occurring between Braak stages V and VI. On the contrary, TDP-43 is consistently associated with a high proportion of impaired subjects regardless of Braak stage. Similar trends are seen with MMSE and hippocampal volume. While we illustrate this phenomenon using cognition, MMSE and hippocampus, similar trends were observed for other clinical and imaging outcome variables

The co-existence of the hallmark AD pathologies of Aβ and tau, along with TDP-43, could be interpreted in two ways. First, TDP-43 is simply a pathological feature of AD. Arguments supporting this hypothesis are (1) the fact that the TDP-negative subjects were more likely to be cognitively normal and show atypical patterns of atrophy and (2) that important factors that are associated with Braak stage, and hence tau pathology, were also associated with TDP-43, including APOE ε4, memory loss and medial temporal atrophy. One strong argument against this interpretation, however, is the fact that there were many AD subjects without TDP-43 that were cognitively impaired. The second more likely possibility is that the presence of TDP-43 represents a secondary or independent pathology that shares overlapping features with AD by targeting the medial temporal lobe. If this latter interpretation is correct then TDP-43 may have obscured our view of the true AD clinico-imaging phenotype, given that such a high proportion of AD cases have TDP-43.

We acknowledge that our outcome measures were not all independent. However, since our aim was not to determine which outcome variable is most affected by TDP-43, and the fact that all the variables we assessed for the study are strongly associated with AD, our approach was very reasonable. We also note that our data for TDP-43 deposition in the dentate gyrus was highly right skewed. However, we addressed this potential problem by rank transforming the burden data prior to our modeling analyses. We did not address the issue of the spatial distribution of TDP-43 in AD given that we have previously done so and describe five stages for TDP-43 deposition in AD for this exact cohort [26]. In fact, all five stages were roughly equally represented. Hence our data was not biased by the overrepresentation of early or late stages.

In this study, we demonstrate that TDP-43 is an important player in the AD field, particularly during the early phases of neurodegeneration. Our findings suggest that in order to be cognitively resilient TDP-43 must be absent. That is, the synergistic effect of having TDP-43 results in cognitive impairment. TDP-43 therefore should be considered a potential therapeutic target for the future treatment of AD.