Introduction

Many animals produce long-distance acoustic signals to mediate a variety of social interactions (Ryan and Kime 2003). The efficacy of acoustic signal transmission depends on the environment (Marten and Marler 1977). Atmospheric absorption, ground attenuation, and deflection of sound by vegetation contribute to signal attenuation (Marten and Marler 1977; Waser and Brown 1986). Coping with environmental attenuation is therefore a major factor shaping the evolution of long-distance acoustic signaling (Römer and Lewald 1992; Boncoraglio and Saino 2007). Two key solutions to minimizing attenuation involve producing signals that are resistant to degradation (i.e. sensory drive or acoustic adaptation and associated receiver adaptations; Obrist et al. 1993; Ey and Fischer 2009; Römer 2020), or signaling at times and/or positions that optimize transmission (Waser and Waser 1977; Barker and Mennill 2009). Although studies on acoustic adaptation are numerous (e.g. Boncoraglio and Saino 2007; Goutte et al. 2018), comparatively fewer studies have explored the behavioral mechanisms used to minimize attenuation.

Animals can minimize signal degradation by advertising during times when climatic conditions are optimal for sound propagation (Wiley and Richards 1978; Hayes and Huntly 2005). For example, the dawn chorus of birds and primates and the evening chorus of insects and frogs are hypothesized to occur due to favorable atmospheric conditions; low temperatures and high relative humidity during these periods are presumed to reduce energy loss via absorption (Marten and Marler 1977; Wiley and Richards 1978). Similarly, animals may position themselves in a manner that increases signal propagation (Blumenrath and Dabelsteen 2004). Among taxa as diverse as crickets (Arak and Eiriksson 1992), treefrogs (Schwartz et al. 2015), and monkeys (Riondato et al. 2021), senders advertise from elevated positions to minimize attenuation from the ground and understory vegetation. Receivers may also benefit from adjusting their position relative to senders to improve signal detection. Indeed, playback of birdsong indicates that receivers benefit from selection of high perches to optimize signal reception (Dabelsteen et al. 1993; Holland et al. 1998; Mathevon et al. 2005). However, few studies have quantified the magnitude of signal efficacy sender and receiver position in tandem, especially in species that produce directional signals that characterize high-frequency vocalizations of many rodents (Richards and Wiley 1980; Fernández-Vargas et al. 2022).

Deer mice (genus Peromyscus) are a widespread and diverse group of rodents that exhibit a wide range of social behaviors, mating systems, and habitat use (Smartt 1978; Kalcounis-Rueppell et al. 2018a, c). Pinyon mice (P. truei) of the southwestern United States are among the most specialized species in the genus due to their reliance on pinyon (Pinus spp.) and juniper (Juniperus spp.) trees for food (Ribble and Samson 1987) and nest sites (Hall and Morrison 1997). Of particular importance, pinyon mice possess morphological adaptations thought to be suited for a semi-arboreal lifestyle, including long tails to facilitate balance (Horner 1954; Smartt and Lemen 1980; Kingsley et al. 2017; Hager and Hoekstra 2021) and larger brains to navigate spatially complex environments (Lemen 1980; Camargo et al. 2019; but see Mace and Eisenberg 1982). Similar to other congeners, pinyon mice produce a variety of vocalizations for social communication, including ca. 20 kHz sustained vocalizations (SVs) that function over long distances (> 1 m; Kalcounis-Rueppell et al. 2018b, c). SVs appear to function as general advertisement signals that help maintain territories (e.g. P. boylii; Petric and Kalcounis-Rueppell 2013) and/or spatial cohesion between mated pairs (P. californicus; Briggs and Kalcounis-Rueppell 2011). In pinyon mice, both sexes produce SVs in social isolation to advertise their presence to conspecifics, but females call more often (Kobrina et al. 2022a). Given that SVs operate over large spatial scales, quantifying how the geometry of senders and receivers influences signal transmission is fundamental to understanding the ecology and evolution of communication in rodents.

In this study, we investigated if signaling and receiving from trees confers benefits for acoustic communication in pinyon mice. Specifically, if arboreality confers an advantage for acoustic signal transmission, then SVs produced and received at heights within trees should experience less attenuation than vocalizations produced and received at ground level. We broadcast and recorded synthesized vocalizations in pinyon mouse habitat at different heights and distances to test these predictions and contextualized our findings by incorporating receiver hearing sensitivities at ecologically relevant distances.

Materials and methods

Animals

Twenty pinyon mice (9 females (F), 11 males (M); average mass: F = 30.90 ± 3.92 g, M = 32.85 ± 3.11 g) were captured near Deadman Flat, 28 km north of Flagstaff, AZ, using Sherman live traps baited with peanut butter and oats. Mice were transferred to standard mouse cages and maintained in animal facilities at Northern Arizona University, Flagstaff, AZ, USA. Mice were housed individually in the vivarium, maintained on a 14:10 dark:light cycle (21 ± 2 °C), and provided rodent chow and water ad libitum. Mice were used for both acoustic recording and hearing experiments (below).

Acoustic recording

Individually-housed mice were placed in a semi-anechoic sound cubicle lined with acoustic foam and recorded over 3–7 days within their home cage. Calibrated microphones (1/4′′ Type 40BE, G.R.A.S.) connected to preamplifiers (Type 26CB, G.R.A.S.) placed 33.3 cm above the center of the cage were used to record mice. Microphone response was flat within ± 1.5 dB from 10 Hz—50 kHz, and pre-amplifier response was flat within ± 0.2 dB from 2 Hz—200 kHz. Microphones were connected to a National Instruments DAQ (USB 4431) sampling at 102.4 kHz to a desktop computer running MATLAB (v. 2018a).

Acoustic signal generation

To generate a signal for use in sound transmission experiments, all SV vocalizations (mean ± SD = 193 ± 427, range: 1–1310; n = 5 F, 3 M; see Kobrina et al. 2022a) were analyzed for average fundamental frequency (F0; kHz), average duration (s), and average amplitude (dB SPL re: 20 μPa). Frequency and duration measures were extracted using the automated parameter measurements function in Avisoft SASLab Pro (v. 4.2.27, Avisoft Bioacoustics, Germany; 1024-point Fast Fourier Transform, Hann window, 75% frame size, 94 Hz bandwidth, 47 Hz frequency resolution, 1.333 ms with 93.75% overlap temporal resolution). Amplitude measures from calibrated microphones were extracted using the Sound Pressure Level Calculator (Greene 2021) in MATLAB (v. 2018a). To correct amplitude values for the distance between the microphone and calling mouse (33.3 cm), we applied a standard equation that accounts for sound energy dissipation via spherical spreading (A.2 in Brumm and Zollinger 2011) to standardize playback SPL levels at 1 m (72.1 dB) for sound attenuation experiments (see below).

To avoid pseudo-replication (McGregor et al. 1992; Kroodsma et al. 2001), we calculated a species grand mean (F0 = 19.8 ± 1.5 kHz; duration = 0.14 ± 0.07 s; dB SPL = 81.6 ± 4 dB) from averaged parameters within individuals to synthesize a signal for use in sound transmission experiments using Avisoft SASLab Pro (48 kHz sampling rate, 16-bit resolution,.wav format). To improve signal to noise ratios, the file contained 50 replicates of the synthesized signal with 0.5 s silent gaps between each replicate.

Sound attenuation experiments

We conducted sound transmission experiments at Deadman Flat between March–May 2022 around dusk (18:00–22:00 h), coincident with times that mice become active. Twenty trees > 4 m (min. distance between trees 50 m) in height were selected randomly at the study site where mice were originally captured. At each site, we set a 4 m transect outward from the perimeter of the furthest live branch of the focal tree in a random direction. At 0 m, we placed a microphone (Sennheiser MKH 8060, 50 Hz – 25 kHz flat frequency response) 9 cm above the ground to mimic the height of a hypothetical mouse receiver. The microphone was connected to a field audio recorder (Sound Devices 702 T) to record stimuli at a sampling rate of 48 kHz and 16-bit resolution. We broadcast synthesized stimuli through a speaker (Ultrasonic Dynamic Speaker, ScanSpeak, Avisoft Bioacoustics, flat frequency response within ± 1.5 dB from 10–45 kHz) connected to an Avisoft Ultrasound Gate Player 416H connected to a laptop running Avisoft RECORDER (v. 5.2.14). We calibrated the amplitude of the playback file using a recorded 93.7 dB 1 kHz tone from a sound level calibrator (Brüel and Kjaer Type 4230) and input the resultant recording into Avisoft SASLab Pro to calibrate all subsequent recordings. We modeled the vertical position of senders (ground; 0 m, 1 m, 2 m) and receivers (ground; 0 m, 1 m, 2 m) in a full factorial design and recorded stimuli at 4 horizontal distances (1, 2, 4, and 8 m) per treatment. For example, to mimic a mouse calling from a 2 m height in a tree and a receiver at ground level, we mounted the speaker on a tripod 2 m above the ground and broadcast stimuli to a microphone placed on the ground iteratively at 1, 2, 4, and 8 m.

Prior to the analyses, all recordings were bandpass filtered between 18.73–20.73 kHz (Hamming window, 128 taps) in Avisoft SASLab Pro. The root mean square (rms) amplitude (20 ms averaging time) of each of the 50 replicates was then measured using automated detection in Avisoft SASLab Pro. We randomly selected a silent period between signal replicates to quantify the amplitude of background noise and subtracted noise values from signal amplitude values using a standard equation (equation A1 in Brumm and Zollinger 2011). Researchers were blind to treatment when attenuation data were analyzed to minimize observer bias.

Hearing sensitivity

Auditory brainstem responses (ABRs) (n = 9 F, 8 M; a subsample of mice from the acoustic recording experiment) were conducted in a semi-anechoic chamber (ETS Lindgren SD-1; internal dimensions 91.4 cm × 91.4 cm × 91.4 cm) lined with acoustic foam. We administered ketamine/dexmedetomidine (75/0.5 mg/kg) intraperitoneally to anesthetize animals. Occasionally, we injected an additional dose of ketamine (< 0.01 mL) 15 min after the initial dose to maintain an anesthetic plane. Anesthetized animals were transferred to a gel heating pad (32 ± 5 °C) to maintain a stable body temperature for the duration of the experiment. Monaural (randomly assigned ear; right n = 10, left n = 7) ABR measurements were obtained by placing three subdermal needle electrodes (27 gauge, 12 mm; Rochester Electro-Medical Inc., Lutz, FL, USA) on the mastoid of ear receiving the stimulus (reference), the vertex of the skull (active channel), and in the dorsum close to the base of the tail (ground). Electrodes were connected to a head stage (RA4LI, Tucker Davis Technologies (TDT), Alachua, FL, USA) and a preamplifier (RA4RA, TDT) attached to a processor (RZ6, TDT) via a fiber optic cable.

We generated and presented test stimuli, and collected responses using SigGenRZ and BioSigRZ (v. 5.7.0, TDT), a TDT Multi I/O processor RZ6, and a PC. ABR experiments were controlled by a PC Windows computer running an Optibit interface on a TDT driver using BioSigRZ. The stimuli were generated by the RZ6 and played through a speaker (MF1, TDT) located 10 cm away from the pinna of the pinyon mouse at 0-degree azimuth. Digitized data were recorded to the RZ6 processor through the RA4LI preamplifier. Stimuli consisted of clicks (0.1 ms square wave pulse of alternating polarity, obtained from the TDT root click file) and 5-ms single-channel cosine-squared gated tone bursts at frequencies of 4, 8, 16, 20, 24, 32, and 42 kHz (Cos2 (10%—90%) gating type, obtained from the TDT root tone file) presented 21 times per second for a total set of 512 repetitions. Test frequencies were selected to examine hearing abilities of both low and high-frequency communication signals previously recorded in P. truei (Kobrina et al. 2021) and typical of rodent audiograms (Dent et al. 2018). However, in this study, we only use sensitivities derived from the 20 kHz stimulus that was most similar to the F0 of SVs (19.8 kHz; above). The click stimulus has more spectral energy below 10 kHz and usually elicits a more robust ABR response than pure tones. Each stimulus was presented at descending levels starting at 90 dB until a threshold was reached. Step sizes were 10–15 dB at suprathreshold levels, and then decreased to 5 dB bracketing the threshold. The system was calibrated prior to each experiment using a microphone (Brüel and Kjær, Type 2670, Nærum, Denmark) connected to a microphone supply (Brüel and Kjær, Type 5935 L, Nærum, Denmark).

Attenuation statistical analyses

To assess how SVs were affected by distance and height, we used a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) with the lme4 package in R studio, v. 3.3.3 (Bates et al. 2015; R Core Team 2017). The GLMM included the log-transformed synthesized stimuli SPL as the response variable and microphone distance (1 m, 2 m, 4 m, 8 m), speaker height (0 m, 1 m, 2 m), and microphone height (0 m, 1 m, 2 m) as main effects, and all 2- and 3-way interactions. ANOVA and posthoc Tukey models revealed background noise was variable among sites (F1,19 = 3.29, p < 0.0001). Therefore, we included site as a random effect in the GLMM to control for background noise variation. We verified assumptions of normality and homoscedasticity by visual inspection of q-q and residual plots. Finally, we computed η2 and confidence intervals (CI) as effect sizes for significant results in the effectsize package for R (Ben-Shachar et al. 2020).

ABR statistical analyses

We used the visual detection method (Green et al. 2019; Kobrina et al. 2021, 2022b) to determine the lowest stimulus level (dB) per stimulus that evoked an ABR response. Thresholds were operationally defined as the dB level halfway (2.5 dB) between the last detectable ABR response and next lowest stimulus level (see Kobrina et al. 2022b for details). A two-way repeated-measures mixed ANOVA was used to determine whether hearing sensitivity varied across stimuli and between ears (lsr package in R; Navarro 2013). Paired Tukey’s t-test post hoc analyses were conducted to assess significance. We computed η2 and confidence intervals (CI) as measures of effect sizes for significant results (effectsize package in R; Ben-Shachar et al. 2020).

Audible distance estimation

Finally, we assessed the audible distance of pinyon mouse SVs produced from the ground (height = 0 m) and from a tree (height = 1 m). To do so, we first calculated sound attenuation via spherical spreading of SV amplitude at 0 m and 1 m heights, using the attenuation function in the Seewave package (Sueur et al. 2008). All factors held equal, sound is expected to attenuate 6 dB for every doubling of distance (Brumm and Zollinger 2011). We then modelled excess attenuation for our observed amplitude values at each height by fitting a logarithmic decay model in R at 1 m, 2 m, 4 m, and 8 m distances, and extrapolated amplitude values beyond 8 m using the line of best fit equation. Finally, to estimate the audible distance of pinyon mice SVs, we used the line of best fit equation to calculate the distance at which sound attenuation intersected receiver hearing sensitivity (41.7 dB at 20 kHz; see Results) measured from ABRs.

Results

Sound attenuation

Attenuation of synthesized stimuli were influenced by sender (speaker) height (β = -19.859, df = 1, 663, p < 0.0001, η2 = 0.36; CI [0.31, 0.41]), receiver (microphone) height (β = -24.483, df = 1, 663, p < 0.0001, η2 = 0.43; CI [0.38, 0.48]), receiver (microphone) distance (β = -4.230, df = 1, 663, p < 0.0001; η2 = 0.22; CI [0.17, 0.27]), and all 2- and 3-way interactions (Table 1). SVs produced and/or received from the ground were more attenuated than above ground. At all distances beyond 1 m, attenuation was greater when both the sender and receiver were on the ground than at any elevation (Fig. 1). On average, trials where both the sender and receiver were elevated 1 m experienced the least attenuation (Fig. 1). When the sender was on the ground, the 1 m receiver height trials experienced the least attenuation at longer distances (4 m and 8 m; Fig. 1). The interaction between speaker and microphone height was the strongest effect in our model (β = 20.97, df = 1.363, p < 0.0001; η2 = 0.50; CI [0.45, 0.54]), indicating amplitude was loudest when the sender and receiver height was matched (Fig. S1). When heights were mismatched, amplitude was optimized if the receiver height was within 1 m of the sender height (Fig. S1). Site, as a random effect, accounted for a relatively large amount of variation (σ2 = 2.83, SD = 1.68).

Table 1 The effects of speaker height, microphone height, and microphone distance on attenuation of pinyon mouse (P. truei) sustained vocalizations based on full factorial playback experiments. Results are from a generalized linear mixed model
Fig. 1
figure 1

Attenuation of pinyon mice sustained vocalization (SVs) amplitude (SPL) vary based on the position of the sender and receiver. Heat map shows relative amplitude values of SVs from relatively quiet (yellow hue) to relatively loud (red hue), produced from a speaker at 0 m, 1 m, and 2 m heights (“sender height” in gray), and recorded from a microphone height at 0 m, 1 m, and 2 m heights (“receiver height” secondary y-axis) at distances of 1 m, 2 m, 4 m, and 8 m. Amplitude values were interpolated between distances

Hearing sensitivity

In general, pinyon mice were able to detect all stimuli. ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of stimulus (F7,96 = 69.76, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.82; CI [0.75, 0.86]) and a non-significant main effect of ear (F1,96 = 0.07, p = 0.79). Paired Tukey’s post-hoc analyses indicated that pinyon mice had lower thresholds (i.e. were more sensitive) to click stimuli than to 4, 8, 20, 24, 32, and 42 kHz tones (p < 0.004). Pinyon mice were most sensitive to 16 kHz tones than to all other frequencies (p < 0.02) with the exception of 8 kHz (p > 0.05). Mice were least sensitive to 32 and 42 kHz tones than to all other frequencies (p < 0.001). Hearing sensitivity for 20 kHz tones were 41.67 ± 6.69 dB and were not significantly different from 4, 8, or 24 kHz tones (p > 0.05; Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Audiogram of pinyon mice (n = 17). Error bars represent ± SE

Audible distance estimation

We assessed the audible distance of SVs on the ground and in a tree by integrating our ABR results with sound attenuation models based on spherical spreading and excess attenuation. Attenuation curves indicated stimuli produced from the ground experienced greater excess attenuation relative to spherical spreading (Fig. 3). From this model, the audible distance, or active space, of SVs was 4.35 m (Fig. 3); beyond this distance, amplitude fell below the estimated 41.67 dB hearing threshold of pinyon mice. In contrast, stimuli produced from trees at 1 m height was predominantly affected by spherical spreading and had little excess attenuation. Under this scenario, the audible distance of SVs was 3 times farther than the ground (12.4 m; Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Attenuation curves based on spherical spreading (open circles) and excess attenuation (black circles) of synthesized pinyon mouse stimuli emitted and received on the ground (left panel) vs. emitted and received at 1 m height (right panel). Horizontal dashed lines represent the hearing sensitivity (41.67 dB) of pinyon mice at 20 kHz. Vertical dashed lines and the corresponding value represent the maximum audible distance (m) of vocalizations

Discussion

Our findings indicate both senders and receivers of acoustic signals benefit from positioning themselves at elevated heights. Vocalizations emitted and received at 1 m off the ground had an active space that was approximately 3 times larger compared to on the ground, in part due to ground and vegetation effects that increase acoustic attenuation, especially at high frequencies (Brenowitz 1986; Marten and Marler 1977). We discuss our findings in relation to the ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in animals in general and rodents in particular.

Our results concur with studies in other taxa that exploit elevated heights to minimize attenuation and thereby extend the communication range of acoustic signals (Arak and Eiriksson 1992; Mathevon et al 2005; Schwartz et al. 2015; Riondato et al. 2021). Benefits to elevated heights include increasing the probability of detection, reducing search costs associated with localizing mates and/or competitors, and/or increasing the number of receivers (Richards and Wiley 1980; Ryan and Kime 2003). In some birds, both senders and receivers exploit elevated perches to optimize production and reception (Dabelsteen et al. 1993). For example, in Rufous-and-white wrens (Thryophilus rufalbus) and Blackcaps (Sylvia atricapilla), certain perch locations extend communication range, especially if senders and receivers are at the same height (Holland et al. 1998; Mathevon et al 2005). Similarly, our results indicate that both senders and receivers benefit at elevated heights, particularly when each party is on a similar plane. However, benefits wane at higher heights (2 m), likely because most attenuation is caused by ground effects.

Although a growing body of literature exists on acoustic communication in mice (Kalcounis-Rueppell et al. 2018c; Rieger and Marler 2018; Fernández-Vargas et al. 2022; Kobrina et al. 2022a, b), few studies are contextualized ecologically. In closely related California mice (P. californicus), SVs appear to facilitate spatial contact and territorial defense between monogamous pairs (Briggs and Kalcounis-Rueppell 2011). Our estimate of audible distance of SVs at ground level (4.35 m) concur with values reported in P. californicus (3.12 m; Timonin et al. 2018) that occupies denser vegetation, adding support to their utility over long distances. In polygynous to promiscuous brush mice (P. boylii; Ribble and Stanley 1998; Kalcounis-Rueppell and Spoon 2009), SVs are implicated in territorial advertisement, especially among females (Petric and Kalcounis-Rueppell 2013). Similar to brush mice, pinyon mice are promiscuous (Ribble and Stanley 1998) and SVs are produced primarily by females (Kobrina et al. 2022a). Given their large home range sizes (0.4—1.6 ha; Ribble and Stanley 1998), female SVs likely facilitate advertisement of their position to neighbors throughout the year and roaming males during the mating season (Emlen and Oring 1977). Our findings suggest that elevated advertisement and reception increases the probability of signal efficacy to mediate intra- and inter-sexual interactions. Notably, varying levels of arboreality are reported in different species of Peromyscus (e.g. P. boylii, P. californicus, P. leucopus, P. maniculatus; Meserve 1977; Harney and Dueser 1987; Kalcounis-Rüppell and Millar 2002) that use SVs in similar long-distance contexts (Kalcounis-Rueppell et al. 2006; Petric and Kalcounis-Rueppell 2013; Rieger and Marler 2018). While the origins of arboreal behavior may be due to food availability (Holbrook 1978) and/or interspecific competition (Stah 1980), the communicative benefits realized from the reduction of acoustic attenuation at ground level may help maintain arboreality. Comparative studies that assess the geometry of vocal behavior across this diverse genus (Kalcounis- Rueppell et al. 2018a, c) would provide important insight and resolution.

The tendency for higher frequencies (≥ 20 kHz) that characterize SVs to be highly directional and attenuate rapidly in the environment requires novel adaptations in both senders and receivers to increase signal efficacy (Richards and Wiley 1980). In bats, active control (Yovel et al. 2011) of high-amplitude echolocation calls (Jakobsen et al. 2013) produced by unique laryngeal mechanisms (Metzner and Schuller 2010) are accompanied by morphologically diverse (Leiser-Miller and Santana 2020) and positionally dynamic (Müller et al. 2017) pinnae to facilitate reception of high frequencies. Peromyscus similarly have unique laryngeal anatomy to produce SVs (Fernández-Vargas et al. 2022), but mechanisms of reception are less studied (Dice and Barto 1952; Ralls 1967; Capshaw et al. 2022). In pinyon mice, large, conspicuous pinnae are hypothesized to facilitate detection of low-frequency predator cues (Hoffmeister 1951, 1981), not higher frequency conspecific vocalizations. Indeed, broader and larger pinnae are associated with lower frequency echolocation calls in bats (Obrist et al. 1993; Huihua et al. 2003). Furthermore, we did not find secondary peaks in pinyon mouse audiograms as is expected when pinnae morphology is involved in high frequency hearing (Heffner et al. 2001). Although more studies are needed on rodent hearing, such patterns further implicate the importance of studying relative positions of senders and receivers.

Our inferences come with minor caveats. Playback of synthesized signals highlight theoretical benefits gained by senders and receivers if they attended to spatial locations when producing or evaluating signals. Although pinyon mice are agile climbers and commonly observed in trees (Bailey 1931; Horner 1954), assessment of realized benefits will require simultaneous tracking and vocal recording of animals in three-dimensional space. In addition, we focused our study on quantifying signal attenuation of a single note of SVs, which represents only one aspect of signal degradation. Pinyon mouse SVs are typically composed of multiple rapidly repeated notes (Kobrina et al. 2021) that likely serve to facilitate detection and recognition (Bradbury and Vehrencamp 2011). Studies that integrate other measures of degradation (e.g. distortion of temporal and frequency patterns; Mathevon et al. 2005) can help clarify the robustness of our findings. Finally, our estimates of signal active space are conservative because electrophysiological methods of hearing sensitivity (e.g. ABRs) are typically less sensitive (10–30 dB) than behavioral paradigms (Heffner and Heffner 2003; Dent et al. 2018). Thus, the ecological validity of the spatial extent of acoustic interactions may be greater than reported herein.

From a basic perspective, our study extends the taxonomic scope of positional advertisement and contributes to our understanding of behavioral mechanisms used to facilitate animal communication. From an applied perspective, consideration of the environment where signaling behaviors occur is critical. For example, pinyon mice rely on pinyon-juniper woodlands (Hall and Morrison 1997) that are receding in their southern ranges due to drought (Clifford et al. 2011; Redmond et al. 2012; Whipple et al. 2019). Given the importance of elevated heights for sound propagation, reductions in tree densities may not only negatively impact food (Hall and Morrison 1997) and nest sites but indirectly influence demographic rates that rely on behaviors reliant on acoustic communication. Integrating signaling and sensory ecology will be key to understanding species-specific resilience (Srivathsa et al. 2019) in response to environmental change.