Introduction

Antagonist co-activation is defined as the “unintentional concurrent activation of antagonist muscles during the activation of agonist muscles” (Duchateau and Baudry 2014). Antagonist muscles produce significant torque around the joint in the direction opposite to net torque, which increases joint stiffness to improve movement accuracy (Miller et al. 2000; Gribble et al. 2003; Stokes and Gardner-Morse 2003) and protect joints [for review, see Remaud et al. (2007)]. Despite the crucial functional role of antagonist muscles during both isometric and dynamic actions, the cortical mechanisms underlying their regulation are not fully understood (Duchateau and Baudry 2014).

Based on early investigations that revealed common firing rate fluctuations between agonist and antagonist motor unit pairs (De Luca and Mambrito 1987; De Luca and Erim 2002), a large body of evidence suggests that cortical level regulates the excitation of agonist–antagonist muscle pairs as a single pool via a common descending oscillatory drive (Boonstra et al. 2009; Mohr et al. 2015). This hypothesis is supported by other electromyography (EMG) studies showing similar fatigue-related changes in agonist and antagonist muscles during a fatigue protocol (Psek and Cafarelli 1993; Levenez et al. 2005; Levenez et al. 2008) and significant intermuscular oscillatory coupling of agonist and antagonist muscles (Wang et al. 2015). The possible role of the cortex in the regulation of antagonist muscle co-activation has been inferred from analysis of motor cortex oscillations recorded by electroencephalography (EEG) (Dal Maso et al. 2012). Consequently, measurement of functional coupling between activities of the motor cortex and antagonist muscles is required to confirm direct motor cortex involvement in the regulation of antagonist co-activation.

Corticomuscular coherence (CMC) analysis directly assesses frequency coupling (Rosenberg et al. 1989) between brain and muscle electrophysiological oscillatory activities recorded by EEG and EMG, respectively. Since the past 2 decades, CMC between the primary motor cortex and agonist muscles has been well characterized (Conway et al. 1995; Kristeva et al. 2007; Gwin and Ferris 2012; Muthuraman et al. 2012; Ushiyama et al. 2012; Campfens et al. 2013; Enders and Nigg 2015; Poortvliet et al. 2015; Lai et al. 2016). Indeed, during isometric contractions, motor cortex oscillations, which are carried down to the alpha-motoneurons via the corticospinal tract (Baker et al. 2003; Lemon 2008; Negro and Farina 2011), are significantly coupled with muscle activity in the 13–31 Hz, ‘beta’ band (β-band) (Muthuraman et al. 2012; Campfens et al. 2013; Poortvliet et al. 2015). Although the underlying mechanisms are still being debated (Witham et al. 2011), CMC reveals large-scale communication between remote populations of neuronal networks (Buzsaki and Draguhn 2004; Schnitzler and Gross 2005; Joundi et al. 2012) and represents suitable analysis for characterizing the cortical regulation of muscle activation (Ushiyama et al. 2010; Boonstra 2013). Consequently, comparison of CMC magnitude between EEG from the motor cortex and EMG from muscles acting as agonists and antagonists may help to further elucidate cortical mechanisms controlling antagonist muscle co-activation.

Interestingly, training status alters antagonist co-activation. Indeed, studies have reported lower antagonist co-activation in strength-trained (ST) than in endurance-trained (ET) or untrained participants during isometric contractions (Tillin et al. 2011; Dal Maso et al. 2012; Amarantini and Bru 2015). Significant changes also occur at the cortical and corticospinal tract levels between ST and ET participants (Carroll et al. 2002; Griffin and Cafarelli 2005; Falvo et al. 2010; Dal Maso et al. 2012). For instance, event-related desynchronization of 13–21 Hz cortical oscillations was higher in ST than ET during isometric contractions. Furthermore, CMC magnitude is sensitive to neural adaptations—induced by extended motor practice (Ushiyama et al. 2010; Perez et al. 2012; Ushiyama et al. 2012; Larsen et al. 2016). Therefore, CMC comparison between individuals with different training status may provide an in-depth understanding of the mechanisms that participate in the control of antagonist muscles.

The aim of the present study was to compare CMC between the motor cortex and agonist muscles versus CMC between the motor cortex and antagonist muscles to provide further insights regarding cortical mechanisms underlying antagonist co-activation. The current work is based on the same sample as in Dal Maso et al. (2012) where the effects of training status on event-related desynchronization of motor cortex oscillations were investigated during isometric voluntary contractions. Following the method developed by Bigot et al. (2011) for accurate assessment of CMC, the first general hypothesis was that CMC would be significant in both agonist and antagonist muscles, but of different magnitudes according to muscle function. Moreover, based on previous results showing higher event-related desynchronization in ST than in ET in the lower β-band (13–21 Hz), it was hypothesized that CMC magnitude would differ between ST and ET in this frequency band. In the upper β-band (21–31 Hz), event-related desynchronization was found to be modulated according to torque level and training status during flexion exertions only. These results led to further postulation that CMC magnitude differs with torque level with specific modulation according to training status in the upper β-band during flexion exertion only.

Methods

Ethics approval

The study protocol was in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki for research on human subjects and followed local ethics guidelines of the Université de Toulouse (France). Study participants received explicit information about the experimental design and gave signed informed consent before the experimental procedures began.

Participants

Twenty-one right-footed men (assessed by inventory of foot preference (Chapman et al. 1987)) volunteered to participate in this study. They were assigned to 1 of the 2 following groups:

  • The ST group included ten participants engaged in regular lower limb strength training at least three times per week since at least 3 years (age 24.10 ± 4.31 years; height 1.77 ± 0.07 m; mass 79.60 ± 7.73 kg; mean ± SD). None of these participants was significantly involved in another physical activity.

  • The ET group included 11 participants engaged in endurance disciplines involving the lower limbs at least three times per week since at least 3 years (age 22.09 ± 2.30 years; height 1.80 ± 0.07 m; mass 77.45 ± 7.49 kg; mean ± SD). None of these participants was involved in an ST program.

The two groups were age-, height-, and mass-matched (p > 0.05). All participants had no lower limb injuries or neurological disorders.

Instrumentation

Net joint torque around the knee was recorded at 1000 Hz by calibrated dynamometer (System 4 Pro, Biodex Medical Systems, Shirley, NY, USA). Surface EMG signals were recorded at 1000 Hz by a Bagnoli-8 system and DE-2.1 single differential electrodes (Delsys, Boston, MA, USA). After suitable skin preparation (Hermens et al. 2000), EMG electrodes were placed over the bellies of representative right knee extensors, namely, the vastus medialis (VM) and rectus femoris (RF), and representative right knee flexors, namely, the biceps femoris (BF) and gastrocnemius (Ga) (Olney and Winter 1985; Amarantini et al. 2010). This electrode placement permitted to record EMG with no crosstalk contamination (Supplementary Figure 1). The reference electrode was placed on the left radial styloid. EEG signals were recorded at 1024 Hz by a 64-channel ActiveTwo system (BioSemi, Amsterdam, The Netherlands). The BioSemi system works in a “zero-Ref” set-up with ground and reference electrodes replaced by a so-called CMS/DRL circuit (for further information, refer to http://www.biosemi.com/faq/cms&drl.htm). EEG electrode locations followed the 10–20 international system. EMG and EEG electrode impedance was kept below 5 kΩ. Torque, EMG, and EEG data were time-synchronized offline with TTL pulse.

Experimental procedures

The experimental procedures have been described in detail elsewhere (Dal Maso et al. 2012). Briefly, participants were seated and secured to the dynamometer with the right knee flexed at 60°. They were then asked to perform:

  1. 1.

    Three 4-s knee isometric maximum voluntary contractions (MVC), followed by 1-min rest in flexion and extension.

  2. 2.

    Three 6-s so-called “relative MVC” (rMVC) in both directions of contraction separated by 1-min rest. During rMVC, participants were asked to achieve the greatest isometric knee torque while relaxing the upper body (for details, see Dal Maso et al. 2012 and Cremoux et al. 2013) to minimize contamination of EEG signals by contractions of muscles surrounding the head during MVC.

  3. 3.

    Knee flexion and extension isometric contractions at 20, 40, 60, and 80% of rMVC are in random order. Participants were not required to stiffen their joints, so that the co-activation recorded over antagonist muscles was of unintentional origin. Participants received custom-made visual feedback of the target torque level (for details, see Dal Maso et al. 2012 and Cremoux et al. 2013). Twenty contractions, each lasting 6 s with 6-s rest in between, were recorded in each condition. A 3-min rest period was allowed after every 16 contractions.

Data analysis

All filters mentioned hereafter are zero-lag, fourth-order Butterworth filters.

Net joint torque was low-pass filtered at 10 Hz. A period of interest was defined for each contraction as a 3-s consecutive window where the sum of absolute error and the variability between required and exerted torque was minimal. Mean squared error of torque production was computed according to the method of Divekar and John (2013). No between-group difference was seen in flexion and extension (p > 0.05).

Figure 1 illustrates the processing steps for computing CMC. Raw EEG and EMG signals were 3–100 Hz band-pass filtered and 45–55 Hz notch filtered (Fig. 1a, b). EEG signals were average referenced. The Cz EEG electrode was selected as the electrode of interest, since (1) event-related desynchronization of the β-band was maximum at this electrode during pre-testing with 50 right knee extension movements (Supplementary Figure 1) and (2) its location on the EEG cap matched the location of the primary motor cortex of the lower limb. Previous studies on lower limb muscle contractions also selected the Cz electrode as the electrode of interest (Perez et al. 2007; Masakado and Nielsen 2008; Dal Maso et al. 2012). Continuous EEG and EMG data were then reduced to concatenated trials from −0.5 to +8 s relative to the onset of visual feedback and zero-aligned. CMC was computed in the time–frequency domain as electrophysiological signals are non-stationary processes (Zhan et al. 2006; Allen and MacKinnon 2010). Time–frequency CMC between Cz EEG signal and each unrectified EMG signal was calculated with the WaveCrossSpec Matlab toolbox for wavelet coherence analysis (Bigot et al. 2011: http://www.math.u-bordeaux1.fr/~jbigot/Site/Software_files/WavCrossSpec.zip). The parameters ‘nvoice’ (scale resolution of wavelets), ‘J1’ (number of scales), and ‘wavenumber’ (Morlet mother wavelet parameter) were, respectively, set to 0.125, 864, and 7 to yield accurate identification of oscillatory activity in the [0.0021:0.9967:104.6565] Hz frequency range. Magnitude-squared coherence was computed as follows:

$$ R_{\text{EEG/EMG}}^{2} \left( {\omega , u} \right) = \left| {S_{\text{EEG/EMG}} \left( {\omega , u} \right)} \right|^{2} /\left( {S_{\text{EEG}} \left( {\omega , u} \right) S_{\text{EMG}} \left( {\omega , u} \right)^{'} } \right) $$
(1)

where \( S_{\text{EEG/EMG}} \left( {\omega , u} \right) \) is the wavelet cross-spectrum between EEG and EMG time series at frequency \( \omega \) and time \( u \) (Fig. 1e); \( S_{\text{EEG}} \left( {\omega , u} \right) \) and \( S_{\text{EMG}} \left( {\omega , u} \right) \) are wavelet auto-spectra of EEG (Fig. 1c) and EMG (Fig. 1d) time series, respectively, at frequency \( \omega \) and time \( u \). Refer to Bigot et al. (2011) for detailed equations.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Steps taken to compute corticomuscular coherence with data from Cz EEG and VM EMG electrodes in an ST participant during knee extension performed at 20% of rMVC. Signals of a Cz EEG and b VM EMG electrodes. Wavelet auto-spectra of c Cz EEG and d VM EMG signals. e Wavelet cross-spectrum between Cz EEG and VM EMG signals: red contours identify areas in the time–frequency plane where correlation between the two signals is significant. f Representation of significant areas of wavelet magnitude-squared coherence between Cz EEG and VM EMG signals. For each participant, experimental condition, and each muscle, the corticomuscular coherence magnitude was quantified as the volume under magnitude-squared coherence values in the low-β (13–21 Hz) and high-β (21–31 Hz) bands where a significant correlation between EEG and EMG time series was detected on the wavelet cross-spectrum

There is an ongoing debate challenging the conventional EMG rectification for CMC analysis (Neto and Christou 2010; McClelland et al. 2012; Negro et al. 2015). Rectification of a zero-mean oscillatory signal, as EMG in our case, is a non-linear process that distorts its power spectrum properties (Neto and Christou 2010; McClelland et al. 2012). Especially for steady-state force tasks, the previous studies highlighted that there is no significant difference in CMC magnitude computed with rectified or unrectified EMG signals (Yao et al. 2007; Bayraktaroglu et al. 2011; Yang et al. 2016). Consequently, to meet both theoretical support and practical justification for the computation of CMC (Bigot et al. 2011; McClelland et al. 2012), CMC was computed from zero-mean centered EEG signals and unrectified EMG signals. As cautioned by Yang et al. (2016), computing CMC with unrectified EMG is suitable provided that EMG signals are motion artefact-free, which was the case of our data (Supplementary Figure 2).

CMC magnitude was quantified as volume under the time–frequency plane where CMC was significant, as detected on cross-spectrum, using the statistical test introduced by Bigot et al. (2011). This magnitude quantification was previously introduced in CMC (Yoshida et al. 2017; Cremoux et al. 2017) and intermuscular coherence (Charissou et al. 2016) studies. The significant threshold of corticomuscular coherence was Bonferroni-corrected to 0.05/4 as CMC was computed with four muscles (Winslow et al. 2016). Finally, for each participant and experimental condition, CMC magnitude in knee extensor and flexor muscle groups was obtained by averaging CMC magnitude in Cz EEG–VM EMG and Cz EEG–RF EMG and in Cz EEG–BF EMG and Cz EEG–Ga EMG, respectively.

Statistics

All dependent variables were tested for normality (Shapiro–Wilk test, α = 0.05), and all data met the assumption of homogeneity of variance (Levene’s test, all p > 0.05).

Three-factor group (ST vs. ET) × torque level (20 vs. 40 vs. 60 vs. 80%) × muscle function (agonist vs. antagonist) analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted on CMC magnitude separately in flexion and extension with repeated measures on torque level and muscle function. CMC magnitude was compared statistically in both 13–21 Hz and 21–31 Hz frequency bands.

Results

CMC magnitude in the 13–21 Hz frequency band

In both flexion and extension, ANOVA revealed a torque level effect (F 3,17 = 8.34; p = 0.001 and F 3,17 = 10.34; p < 0.001, respectively), a muscle function effect (F 1,19 = 30.46; p < 0.001 and F 1,19 = 86.70; p < 0.001, respectively), and torque level × muscle function interaction (F 3,17 = 7.59; p = 0.002 and F 3,17 = 9.72; p = 0.001, respectively) on CMC magnitude in the 13–21 Hz frequency band. CMC magnitude was higher in antagonist than in agonist muscles. Low-β CMC magnitude in antagonist muscles decreased with torque level, while it tended to remain constant across torque levels in agonist muscles (Fig. 2a, c). Low-β CMC magnitude was significantly higher in ST than in ET participants in flexion and extension (F 1,19 = 5.64; p = 0.028; Fig. 2b and F 1,19 = 4.73; p = 0.042; Fig. 2d, respectively).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean (+SE) of corticomuscular coherence magnitude in 13–21 Hz (left part) and 21–31 Hz (right part) frequency bands in agonist and antagonist muscles during flexion (upper part) and extension (lower part) contractions at 20, 40, 60, and 80% of rMVC. a, c, e and g represent significant torque level and muscle function effects and torque level × muscle function interaction, respectively, indicated by arrow, hash symbol, and triangle when applicable. b, d, f, and h represent the group effect indicated by asterisk when applicable

CMC magnitude in the 21–31 Hz frequency band

In both flexion and extension, ANOVA revealed a torque level effect (F 3,17 = 3.98; p = 0.026 and F 3,17 = 7.46; p = 0.002, respectively). CMC magnitude with agonist and antagonist muscles decreased with torque level (Fig. 2e, g). In extension only, high-β CMC was significantly higher than in ET participants (F 1,19 = 7.26; p = 0.014, Fig. 2h). In flexion only, high-β CMC was significantly higher with antagonist than with agonist muscles (F 1,19 = 80.95; p < 0.001, Fig. 2e).

Discussion

To provide further insights into cortical involvement underlying antagonist co-activation, the present study compared CMC in agonist and antagonist muscles and in ST and ET participants during knee isometric contractions performed at different torque levels. First, these findings revealed that CMC was significant with antagonist muscles in the broad β-band, providing clear experimental evidence that the cortex participates directly in the regulation of antagonist co-activation. Second, CMC magnitude in antagonist muscles decreased more than CMC magnitude in agonist muscles as torque level increased, which suggests specific regulation of muscle activation according to their function. Finally, higher CMC magnitude in the lower β-band for ST in comparison to ET participants may result from training-induced adaptation.

The cortex regulates antagonist muscle co-activation

Coherence analysis measures the strength of the coupling between two oscillatory signals (Rosenberg et al. 1989). While the previous studies repeatedly investigated CMC in agonist muscles (Kristeva et al. 2007; Gwin and Ferris 2012; Muthuraman et al. 2012; Ushiyama et al. 2012; Campfens et al. 2013; Enders and Nigg 2015; Poortvliet et al. 2015; Lai et al. 2016), the present work focused on CMC in antagonist muscles, considering their major functional role in human voluntary muscular contraction. Our findings revealed a significant coupling between the motor cortex and antagonist muscles in the broad β-band, regardless of the group of participants, direction of contraction, and torque level. Considering that CMC is thought to reflect communication between distant neural networks (Buzsaki and Draguhn 2004; Schnitzler and Gross 2005; Joundi et al. 2012) via pyramidal pathways (Baker et al. 2003; Lemon 2008; Negro and Farina 2011), the present study provides direct experimental evidence of the functional coupling between primary motor cortex and antagonist muscles during isometric knee contractions. This result agrees with the “common drive” theory (De Luca and Mambrito 1987; De Luca and Erim 2002), confirming the role of cortical control in antagonist co-activation.

Regulation of muscle activation is function-specific

CMC magnitude in the broad β-band was higher with antagonist than with agonist muscles in both groups of participants. Moreover, CMC magnitude in the low-β-band decreased more in antagonist than in agonist muscles as torque level increased. These results suggest that the regulation of muscle activation is function-specific, with strongest direct functional oscillatory communication between the brain and antagonist muscles. The following potential mechanisms may explain the observed modulation of CMC magnitude with muscle function.

First, CMC may not derive from direct and simple motor cortex-to-muscle descending oscillation propagation. Indeed, intrinsic spinal mechanisms occurring during voluntary contractions (Nielsen 2016) can alter CMC magnitude, as suggested by modeling-based investigation (Williams and Baker 2009; Watanabe and Kohn 2015). Ascending muscle-to-motor cortex drives are also essential in the establishment of corticomuscular coupling. Indeed, CMC is altered after ischemia or arm cooling (Pohja and Salenius 2003; Riddle and Baker 2005; Witham et al. 2011) and peripheral electrical stimulation (Lai et al. 2016), which are known to modulate peripheral nerve conduction time and afferent information, respectively. The contribution of spinal mechanisms and afferent information may, therefore, be specific to muscle function and torque production, causing torque level x muscle function β-band CMC magnitude interaction.

Second, proportions of direct corticospinal projections innervating muscles may have altered broad β-band CMC magnitude in agonist and antagonist muscles. Indeed, distal muscles, which have more direct corticospinal projections than proximal muscles, have higher CMC magnitude (Ushiyama et al. 2010). The latter finding indicates that the proportions of corticomotoneurons directly innervating muscles may be greater when they act as antagonists rather than as agonists.

Finally, the modulation of CMC magnitude may also be interpreted in light of the status quo theory (Engel and Fries 2010). Indeed, high CMC magnitude may reflect the maintenance of a stable state that efficiently processes feedback to recalibrate the sensorimotor system with minimum computational effort (Brown 2000). According to the present CMC results, the muscle function effect may indicate greater computational effort to regulate agonist muscle than antagonist muscle activation.

CMC analysis as a marker of training-induced adaptation

According to the previous analysis of the current database, both groups had comparable force capabilities, but ST participants had lower antagonist co-activation than ET participants in flexion (Dal Maso et al. 2012). In light of numerous investigations that have reported decreased antagonist co-activation in ST athletes (Carolan and Cafarelli 1992; Hakkinen et al. 2000; Griffin and Cafarelli 2005; Amarantini and Bru 2015), this result was interpreted as an ST-induced adaptation mechanism known to occur at cortical, corticospinal, and motor unit levels (Carroll et al. 2002; Griffin and Cafarelli 2005; Folland and Williams 2007; Falvo et al. 2010; Vila-Chã and Falla 2016). The present data indicate increased CMC in the ST group in the lower β-band during flexion and extension exertions and in the upper β-band during extension exertions only. This result agrees with the previous studies showing alteration of CMC with functional abilities of participants (Ushiyama et al. 2010; Larsen et al. 2016). Interestingly, the previous findings highlighted modulation of task-related spectral power of motor cortex oscillations in the lower β-band between ET and ST participants in both directions of contraction (Dal Maso et al. 2012). CMC magnitude, being sensitive to cortico-motoneuronal plasticity occurring after practice (Baker and Baker 2003; Hansen and Nielsen 2004; Perez et al. 2006; Mendez-Balbuena et al. 2012; Larsen et al. 2016), the observed increase of CMC in ST participants may be interpreted as a training adaptation effect. Different mechanisms may, therefore, participate in the control of muscle activation between groups, suggesting that CMC magnitude is a reliable marker of corticomotor adaptations.

Conclusion

The present study revealed a significant CMC between the motor cortex and both agonist and antagonist muscle activities. Its findings provide experimental evidence that the cortex is directly involved in the regulation of both agonist and antagonist muscle co-activation, and suggest that the mechanisms underlying muscle activation are specific to their function. The present observations also revealed that the strength of corticomuscular coupling is sensitive to practice-induced adaptation, emphasizing that CMC may be a relevant measure in investigating the effect of long-term corticomotor adaptation.