Introduction

The principle underpinning the neuromodulatory technique transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to affect brain function is that tDCS modulates the excitability of neurons by changing the resting membrane potential (Bindman et al. 1964; Purpura and McMurtry 1965). tDCS can elicit changes in excitability that outlast the period of stimulation when applied for several minutes during and before a task (Nitsche and Paulus 2000, 2001, 2011). In recent years, it has been suggested that tDCS may be a potential treatment for neurological and psychiatric disorders (Kuo et al. 2014; Liew et al. 2014; Player et al. 2014). Currently, the majority (~ 96%) of tDCS sessions have been limited to intensities of 2 mA or less (Bikson et al. 2016). Thus, this standard of 2 mA has become an unofficial ‘upper limit’ in tDCS research. However, because tDCS technologies and methodologies have improved, exploration of higher intensities is both plausible and recommended (Rothwell 2012). Indeed, some are already exploring tDCS at intensities as high as 4 mA (Chhatbar et al. 2017, 2018; Khadka et al. 2019; Trapp et al. 2019). However, of these higher intensity investigations that included a motor performance, all of them had small sample sizes (≤ 3), and most were primarily concerned with assessing tolerability and not motor performance changes (Chhatbar et al. 2017, 2018; Trapp et al. 2019). Examining a broader range of stimulation parameters is necessary, because recent evidence suggests that a higher current density (which is a function of electrode size and intensity) may not always result in greater increases in excitability (Bastani and Jaberzadeh 2013a; Chew et al. 2015; Kidgell et al. 2013). Furthermore, a recent review also highlighted the variable effects of tDCS on motor performances, especially in association with increasing stimulation intensity (Esmaeilpour et al. 2018).

A few studies have compared the effect of different tDCS intensities on cortical excitability (Ammann et al. 2017; Chew et al. 2015; Ho et al. 2016; Kidgell et al. 2013; Murray et al. 2015), working memory (Papazova et al. 2018; Stephens et al. 2017), and schizophrenia (Andrade 2013; Hoy et al. 2014; Papazova et al. 2018), with mixed results. For example, Ammann et al. (2017) showed that tDCS at 2 mA, but not 1 mA, significantly increased cortical excitability, whereas the findings of Ho et al. (2016) indicated that 2 mA tDCS did not necessarily result in increased excitability than 1 mA tDCS in healthy participants. The effects of different intensities on motor performances are also sparse. Nevertheless, Liu et al. (2018) found a significant improvement of 2 mA over 1 mA tDCS on performance measures of professional rowers, although this was without a comparison to sham. Together, these studies indicate an uncertain effect of increasing intensity to increase the beneficial effects of tDCS. As mentioned above, the uncertain dose–response effect of increasing intensity to enhance neurophysiological and behavioral outcomes was also the conclusion of a recent review (Esmaeilpour et al. 2018). Therefore, there is a need to understand whether increasing current intensity (i.e., > 2 mA) over a given brain area enhances behavioral and neurophysiological outcomes, like fatigability.

Fatigue is a commonly assessed outcome measure in tDCS studies, and is defined as “the decrease in physical and/or mental performance that results from changes in central, psychological, and/or peripheral factors” (Rudroff et al. 2016). These changes depend on the task being performed, the environmental conditions in which it is performed, and the physical and mental capacity of the individual (Rudroff et al. 2016). Specifically, performance fatigability can be defined as the magnitude or rate of change in a performance criterion relative to a reference value over a given time of task performance. On the other hand, perceptions of fatigue can be defined as a subjective sensation of weariness, increasing sense of effort, mismatch between effort expended and actual performance, or exhaustion (Kluger et al. 2013). Improving perceived and performance fatigue in healthy participants and people with neurological disorders, such as multiple sclerosis, has been an important topic for several years (Finsterer and Mahjoub 2014; Lefaucheur et al. 2017). Non-medication treatments (e.g., resistance exercise, yoga, and occupational therapy) and prescription medications (e.g., amantadine, modafinil, and methylphenidate) have yielded small effects, or effects that are insignificant at the individual level (Finsterer and Mahjoub 2014). Thus, alternative treatments for fatigue, like tDCS, have been investigated in healthy participants (see Angius et al. 2018b for a review) and in people with neurological impairments (Cancelli et al. 2018; Ferrucci et al. 2014; Lefaucheur et al. 2017; Tecchio et al. 2014). Furthermore, as both the Angius et al. (2018b) and the Esmaeilpour et al.’s (2018) reviews highlight performance outcomes in tDCS research (e.g., fatigue), especially with different intensities and intensities beyond the 2 mA standard, which can be unpredictable.

The mechanisms underscoring the effects of excitatory tDCS on performance fatigue could be related to an increased cortical excitability within the primary motor cortex (M1), which could lead to hypothetical reductions in supraspinal fatigue and ratings of perceived exertion (Alix-Fages et al. 2019; Angius et al. 2017). Increased corticospinal excitability, together with alterations in motor unit recruitment strategies (Krishnan et al. 2014), could theoretically explain the observed improvements in performance fatigue after or during unilateral or bilateral tDCS (Abdelmoula et al. 2016; Alix-Fages et al. 2019; Angius et al. 2016, 2018a; Cogiamanian et al. 2007; Lattari et al. 2018; Okano et al. 2015; Oki et al. 2016; Radel et al. 2017; Williams et al. 2013). However, there are also reports of no effects of tDCS on fatigue (Flood et al. 2017; Hameau et al. 2018; Kan et al. 2013; Muthalib et al. 2013; Vitor-Costa et al. 2015). Most of these previous studies have investigated fatigue at submaximal performances, often with isometric contractions (Alix-Fages et al. 2019). However, isometric testing has poor applicability to real-world, dynamic performances (Kollock et al. 2015) and isokinetic testing may better reflect functional performance (Sliwowski et al. 2018). Interestingly, only one has reported the effects of tDCS on isokinetic performance fatigability of the knee extensor muscles (Ciccone et al. 2019), and the authors found no improvements in performance fatigability with 2 mA tDCS over the temporal lobe. The results of fatigue investigations may also be ambiguous, because fatigue is often not clearly defined or is used as an outcome without meaningful measurements in clinical and research settings (Rudroff et al. 2016). Furthermore, the lack of standardized tDCS protocols makes it difficult to compare fatigue studies. Nevertheless, most have used (1) isometric contractions, (2) tDCS administered prior to the task, and (3) intensities less than 2 mA. Thus, the effects of tDCS on isokinetic performance fatigue, especially at intensities > 2 mA, are uncertain.

Therefore, the purpose of this study was to determine the effects of 2 mA and 4 mA tDCS on leg muscle fatigability in healthy young adults, using objective performance fatigability data (peak torque and total work; Gleeson and Mercer 1992), obtained from an isokinetic system. It was hypothesized that M1 tDCS would decrease leg muscle fatigability and that the 4 mA intensity would yield greater decreases in fatigability than the 2 mA intensity.

Materials and methods

Participants

Sixteen physically active young adults were recruited (females = 9; mean ± SD, age = 24.5 ± 3.8 years, height = 170 ± 11.7 cm, weight = 71.1 ± 14.4 kg). Inclusion criteria included: (1) young adult (18–30 years); (2) right-side dominant; (3) performing at least 30 min of moderate intensity physical activity on 3 days/week for the previous 3 months; (4) no chronic neurological, psychiatric, or medical conditions; and (5) no psychoactive medications. The exclusion criteria were: (1) pregnancy; (2) holes or fissures in the skull; (3) metal objects or devices implanted in the skull (e.g., metal plate). All participants were naïve to tDCS stimulation. This study was approved by the University of Iowa’s Institutional Review Board and all participants provided written informed consent before beginning participation.

Experimental protocol

This was a double-blind, randomized sham-controlled study design. Participants completed four laboratory visits, with a 5–8 day separation between visits. At the first session, right-side dominance was objectively determined with an isokinetic maximal strength test of the knee extensors and flexors of both legs (see “Isokinetic strength test”) and the participants performed a familiarization trial of the isokinetic fatigue test (FT; see “Isokinetic fatigue test”). Only participants that had right-side dominance were recruited to avoid the potential for functional brain morphology differences between right- and left-dominant people (Jang et al. 2017). After strength testing, the FT was performed on each leg, with a short (< 2 min) interval between the right leg FT (R-FT) and the left leg FT (L-FT). For consistency between participants and subsequent tDCS sessions, the R-FT was always completed first during all four sessions. For the remaining visits (i.e., two, three, and four), one of three tDCS sessions (sham, 2 mA, and 4 mA; see “tDCS sessions”) were randomly administered and R-FT and L-FT were completed in the same order as the first session (i.e., R-FT first).

Isokinetic strength test

All isokinetic tests (i.e., strength and fatigue) were administered with an isokinetic dynamometer (HUMAC NORM, CSMi, Stoughton, MA). The strength test for the right leg commenced with a submaximal warm-up of the knee extensors and flexors (15 concentric/concentric repetitions, 60°/s). After a ≥ 30 s rest interval, the participants performed five sets of one maximal effort knee extension and flexion (concentric/concentric, 60°/s; Montenegro et al. 2015), with a ≥ 30 s rest between sets. After ~ 2 min, the left leg strength test was performed in the same manner as the right. For dominance verification, the highest peak torque from the five maximal efforts was retained. In addition, the test administrators provided verbal encouragement to help the participants achieve maximal efforts.

Isokinetic fatigue test

The FT involved 40 continuous maximal contractions [concentric/concentric 120°/s (Saenz et al. 2010)] of the knee extensors and knee flexors. Similar muscle fatigue protocols have previously been used in healthy and neurological populations (Ciccone et al. 2019; Hameau et al. 2018; Lambert et al. 2001; Mackey et al. 2018; Thorstensson and Karlsson 1976). In sessions during which tDCS was administered (i.e., visits two, three, and four), the participants undertook the same 15 repetition warm-up as described above. After the warm-up, and at the designated time during tDCS (see “tDCS sessions” below), the R-FT followed immediately by the L-FT was performed. Because some have suggested that tDCS might improve the performance of limbs ipsilateral to stimulation (Schambra et al. 2011), the FT was performed with both legs. The total time to complete both FTs was 4.5–5 min, including the transition time between right and left legs. To maximize participant effort, the work achieved in each contraction was visually displayed (i.e., as a series of bars) and verbal encouragement was provided.

tDCS sessions

tDCS was administered with a battery powered 1X1 tDCS Low-Intensity Stimulator (Model 1300A, Soterix Medical Inc., New York, NY). Two carbon electrodes were inserted into 5 × 7 cm EASYpad sponges (35 cm2 surface area; Soterix Medical Inc., New York, NY). Current density was 0.06 mA/cm2 and 0.11 mA/cm2 for the 2 mA and 4 mA intensities, respectively. Each sponge was saturated with 10–15 ml of 0.9% NaCL saline and held in place using an EASYstrap (Soterix Medical Inc., New York, NY). The active electrode was located over C3, using the 10–20 EEG placement convention (Klem et al. 1999), and the return electrode was localized over the supraorbital area on the contralateral side (Fig. 1). This montage, which targeted the dominant M1, was chosen to maximize motor performance (Schambra et al. 2011). It is noted that because size of the electrodes was relatively large (5 cm × 7 cm), the edge of the active electrode either abutted or covered the center of the skull (Cz), similar to previous studies targeting unilateral M1 leg areas (Jayaram and Stinear 2009; Foerster et al. 2018). Thus, the leg areas of the dominant M1, which are located in the longitudinal fissure (Foerster et al. 2018), were also covered by the electrodes. Active tDCS conditions (i.e., 2 mA and 4 mA) began with a 30 s ramp-up, after which the intensity remained at the specified level for 20 min before a 30 s ramp-down to 0 mA. For sham tDCS, the stimulator performed a 30 s ramp-up to 2 mA and a 30 s ramp-down to 0 mA, after which the intensity stayed at 0 mA for 20 min. After the 20 min stimulation time, the stimulator again performed a 30 s ramp-up to 2 mA and a 30 s ramp-down to 0 mA.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Left: location of the active (primary motor cortex) and return (contralateral supraorbital area) electrodes for all tDCS conditions. Right: model of the electrical field for the 2 mA intensity

The tDCS device included a ‘PRE-STIM TICKLE’ function, which activated a 1 mA stimulation for ~ 30 s. This function was performed before each tDCS session to investigate the contact quality between the active and return electrodes, and helped to ensure that the electrodes were adequately soaked and securely placed; adjustments to sponge saturation and scalp contact were made as necessary. During the first tDCS session, the location of the active and return electrodes on the EASYstrap were recorded for each participant and the electrodes were secured in the same place for subsequent sessions. Stimulation was always delivered with the participant seated in the dynamometer chair. At minute 15, the participants performed R-FT and then L-FT. As mentioned above, the total time to complete both FTs was 4.5–5 min. Even though most have administered tDCS before a task (Alix-Fages et al. 2019), this may activate neuronal populations in a non-specific way (Nitsche et al. 2008). However, there is also some evidence, particularly from motor and cognitive learning studies (Ammann et al. 2016; Martin et al. 2014; Stagg et al. 2011, 2013), that performing tDCS during a task, which could theoretically enhance endogenous signals activated during task execution, may be more beneficial. Thus, tDCS stimulation (active or sham) was performed both before and during the FT of the right and left legs.

At the end of each tDCS session, the participants were asked to describe any sensations experienced [e.g., itching, tingling, burning, etc. (Aparicio et al. 2016)] and to rate sensation severity on a 10-point Likert-type scale (1 = lowest, 10 = highest). The participants were also asked to guess which tDCS intensity which they experienced (sham, 2 mA, 4 mA). Feedback about the guesses was not provided until the end of the final session.

Data analysis

To investigate the effects of different intensities of tDCS on leg muscle fatigue, two fatigue indices for the knee extensors and flexors of both legs were calculated: one for torque production (FI-T; percent decline in torque production from the beginning to the end of the FT) and one for work performed (FI-W; percent decline in work performed from the beginning to the end of the FT). The peak torque for each repetition of each FT was used to calculate FI-T as follows: FI-T = ([mean of the first five repetitions − mean of the last five repetitions]/mean of the first five repetitions) × 100 (Ciccone et al. 2019; Hameau et al. 2018; Mackey et al. 2018; Thorstensson and Karlsson 1976). The peak work of each repetition was used to calculate FT-W as follows: (total work performed in the last half/total work performed in the first half) × 100 (Lambert et al. 2001). In addition, the total work (TW) performed during the FT (i.e., sum of the work performed in each repetition) was calculated for fatigue indices that were significantly different between tDCS conditions. However, because several participants were unable to achieve a maximum effort at the beginning of the FT, the first two repetitions were considered adaptation and were not included in either FI calculation or the TW calculation. Sensation reports and blinding accuracy of tDCS were determined by averaging the severity of similar sensation descriptions and calculating the percent of correct tDCS condition guesses, respectively.

Statistical analysis

Because differences between the knee extensors and knee flexors were expected a priori (Gur et al. 1999), and the focus of this study was to determine the effects of different intensities of tDCS on leg muscle fatigability, a stimulation (sham vs. 2 mA vs. 4 mA) repeated-measures ANOVA of the FI-T and FI-W data was performed for each leg muscle group (right extensor, left extensors, right flexors, and left flexors). Additionally, any participant that had an FI-T ≤ 0% (i.e., torque production higher at the end compared to the beginning of the FT) was not included in the statistical analysis. This exclusion helped to avoid bias from less-motivated participants. Pairwise post hoc comparisons (paired t tests) and effect sizes (Cohen’s d) were calculated to clarify any significant tDCS differences. Also, TW was assessed using paired t tests on tDCS conditions that had significant FI-T and FI-W pairs. Because this analysis and study were exploratory in nature, significance was accepted at p < 0.05, uncorrected. In the case of a violation of the sphericity assumption for a repeated-measures ANOVA, Greenhouse–Geisser corrections were used. The assumption of homogeneity of variance was tested using Leven’s test of equal variances and adjustments to the degrees of freedom were made as necessary. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 25 (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, USA).

Results

All participants completed all study visits and none had FI results below the bias correction cutoff (FI-T ≤ 0%). The assumptions for the statistical tests were sufficiently met and no corrections were made. Data are reported as mean ± SD in the text and mean ± SEM in the figures. The results of the repeated-measure ANOVA for FI-T indicated a significant effect of stimulation for the right knee extensors (p < 0.001), but not for the right knee flexors (p = 0.80), the left knee extensors (p = 0.57), or the left knee flexors (p = 0.11). Figure 2 shows the results of the post hoc testing, and revealed that both the 2 mA (54.4 ± 8.2%) and the 4 mA (56.4 ± 7.4%) tDCS conditions had significantly higher FI-T (i.e., more fatigability) compared with sham (50.4 ± 6.7%; p = 0.01, d = 0.73 and p < 0.001, d = 1.61, respectively), but 2 mA was not different from 4 mA (p = 0.143, d = 0.39).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Fatigue index derived from the torque data for the right knee flexors, stratified by stimulation intensity. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. *Significantly larger (i.e., increased fatigability) than sham (p < 0.01)

For FT-W, the repeated-measures ANOVA indicated a significant effect of stimulation, again for the right knee extensors (p < 0.001), with the pairwise testing (Fig. 3), indicating that 2 mA (62.6 ± 5.1%) and 4 mA (59.1 ± 7.4%) conditions had significantly smaller FI-W (i.e., more fatigability) than sham (56.7 ± 5.4%; p = 0.01, d = 0.57 and p < 0.001, d = 1.12, respectively) and that the 4 mA condition was significantly smaller than the 2 mA condition (p = 0.034, d = 0.37). The results of the repeated-measures ANOVA for TW did not reveal any significant differences between any of the tDCS conditions (sham = 3642.6 ± 1331.2 J; 2 mA = 3539.6 ± 1292.5 J; 4 mA = 3622.9 ± 1235.6 J; Fig. 4).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Fatigue index derived from the work data for the right knee flexors, stratified by stimulation intensity. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. *Significantly smaller (i.e., increased fatigability) than sham (p < 0.01) and #Smaller than the 2 mA condition (p = 0.034)

Fig. 4
figure 4

Total work performed by the right knee extensors during the isokinetic fatigue test, stratified by stimulation intensity (n = 38 repetitions). There were no significant differences in total work performed in the three conditions (p > 0.05) despite the increased fatigability of this muscle group

The tolerability and blinding results of the different tDCS conditions are shown in Table 1. Most of the participants reported feeling similar sensations across all three conditions (i.e., tingling, itching, and burning), accompanied by a slight increase in severity. The most intense of these common sensations, burning, occurred in the 4 mA condition, but was still considered moderate. Notably, there were rare (i.e., n = 1–3), but novel sensations reported in the 2 mA and the 4 mA, most of which were mild—moderate. However, there were a few moderate—severe sensations in each condition (sham: poking = 6; 2 mA: prickling = 5.5, spike = 6; 4 mA: pressure = 7, and headache = 8).

Table 1 Blinding and tolerability results for sham, 2 mA, and 4 mA tDCS (n = 16)

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of standard intensity tDCS (2 mA) and higher intensity tDCS (4 mA) on leg muscle fatigability in young, healthy participants. Contrary to our hypothesis, the results of the FI analyses indicated increased fatigability of the right knee extensors in the 2 mA and the 4 mA tDCS conditions compared with sham (Figs. 1, 2) and between the 2 and 4 mA conditions (Fig. 2). Interestingly, the total work performed by this muscle group was the same in all three conditions (Fig. 3). Taken together, these results indicate that, despite a significant increase in the fatigability of the right knee extensors from tDCS stimulation, the participants performed an equal amount of work in each condition. The stimulation tolerability reports also revealed that the severity of the sensations felt during sham, 2 mA, and 4 mA stimulation were very similar for most participants (Table 1). A few participants reported moderate—severe sensations (i.e., > 6), but these were rare and occurred in each tDCS condition, including sham. However, it is noted that all of the participants reported that these sensations were only present during the stimulation period, and even the most severe sensations (pressure = 7 and headache = 8, both reported in the 4 mA condition) resolved within minutes after stimulation ended. Nevertheless, future studies using stimulation intensities > 2 mA warrant considerate application and consistent communication with participants before and during higher intensity tDCS application. For the stimulation blinding, approximately two-third of participants correctly guessed the sham and 2 mA conditions, while slightly less than half correctly guess the 4 mA condition (43.8%). However, an equal number of participants incorrectly guessed 2 mA (43.8%) for the 4 mA condition. Thus, only 12.5% of participants incorrectly guessed sham for the 4 mA condition, which may indicate that participant blinding at higher intensities may not be as feasible as more moderate intensities (i.e., ≤ 2 mA) (Fertonani et al. 2015; Kessler et al. 2012).

Previous studies showed a decrease in fatigability (Angius et al. 2016; Cogiamanian et al. 2007) and increased total work (Sales et al. 2016) from tDCS. However, the fatiguing protocols and measurements of these studies differ from the present study in potentially important ways. For example, Angius et al. (2016) and Cogiamanian et al. (2007) used a submaximal isometric contraction and measured the time to fatigue, and Sales et al. (2016) found increased TW with tDCS, but only used five maximal isokinetic contractions. The present fatigue task included repetitive maximal isokinetic contractions and measured TW over 38 contractions. Thus, the differences in effects of tDCS on muscle fatigue and TW might be task dependent and warrants further investigation.

It is well accepted that excitatory tDCS increases cortical excitability (Dissanayaka et al. 2017; Nitsche and Paulus 2000), and influences a range of physiologic and cognitive outcomes (Berryhill and Martin 2018; Sanchez-Kuhn et al. 2017). Increased cortical excitability could heighten the neural drive to the muscles and increase motor unit (MU) recruitment and/or rate of discharge (Krishnan et al. 2014). Thus, enhanced neural drive could have altered the orderly recruitment of MU [i.e., smallest to largest, (Henneman 1957)] and the larger MUs could have been recruited earlier (Krishnan et al. 2014), resulting in an increased fatigability of the muscle.

It has also been suggested that current flow orientation also affects cortical excitability from tDCS (Rawji et al. 2018) and individual differences in cortical alignment and connectivity (Lee et al. 2019) might alter the purported excitatory and inhibitory effects of the active and return electrodes, respectively. Similarly, because little is known about higher intensity stimulation (> 2 mA), it is also possible that the return electrode is be excitatory at higher intensities, which may affect performance. Furthermore, the size of the stimulating electrodes (which influences current density and the brain areas stimulated) may also play an important role in localizing stimulation effects (Bastani and Jaberzadeh 2013b). The large electrodes in our study (35 cm2) may have stimulated nearby cortical functional areas, which may have inhibitory effects on the motor cortex. Again, as suggested by Bastani and Jaberzadeh (2013b), smaller electrodes may result in more spatially focused stimulation and may increase the efficacy of tDCS for the induction of effective corticospinal excitability.

In addition, if it is assumed that the baseline cortical excitability of young, healthy participants is purposefully homeostatic (Chai et al. 2019), and then, increasing excitability could interfere with the efficient genesis and delivery of desired brain signals. In other words, tDCS may induce hyperexcitability in healthy participants and lead to undesired neuronal activity that may hamper endogenous signals. This idea is also supported by the findings of Bastani and Jaberzadeh (2013a), who found that low-intensity/density tDCS (0.3 mA, current density = 0.013 mA/cm2) was excitatory, while relatively higher intensities/densities (0.7 mA and 1.4 mA; current density = 0.029 mA/cm2 and 0.058 mA/cm2, respectively) were inhibitory in healthy participants. The authors postulated that lower intensity/density stimulation might result in excitatory activation via Ca2+ channels without activating GABA and NMDA receptors, which are voltage-dependent (Mellor and Randall 1998). Similarly, Fujiyama et al. (2017) found significant skill learning improvements from inhibitory ‘priming’ of M1 before excitatory stimulation of M1, compared with M1 excitatory stimulation alone, during a force-learning task in young and older healthy adults. Inhibiting M1 (reducing excitability) may have depressed the baseline cortical activity in these participants and the subsequent excitatory stimulation restored or only slightly increased excitability beyond baseline; thus, the net effect may have been similar to the previously mentioned low-intensity condition of Bastani and Jaberzadeh (2013a). Taken together, these results (including those of the present study) may question the suitability of young, healthy participants for exploring the motor performance effects of higher intensity tDCS (≥ 2 mA); however, more definitive investigation is certainly necessary to justify this suggestion. Nevertheless, exploration of higher intensity stimulation in populations with lower baseline excitability resulting from injury or disease (e.g., multiple sclerosis, Parkinson’s disease, or stroke) is certainly warranted to determine if increasing tDCS intensity further increases benefits (Sanchez-Kuhn et al. 2017).

Similarly, the timing of anodal stimulation (before or during) may differently affect leg muscle fatigability. Administering tDCS before a motor performance might increase cortical excitability in a non-specific way (Nitsche et al. 2008), whereas performing tDCS during the desired task might enhance task-specific cortical signals. However, changing excitability homeostasis, which is purported to alter performance, might be modulated by administering the simulation before task performance (i.e., homeostatic metaplasticity; Ziemann and Siebner 2008). In contrast, non-homeostatic excitability changes, called “gating,” might occur when tDCS is administered during the task (i.e., online; Ziemann and Siebner 2008) and online gating may be better suited for improving motor learning (Ammann et al. 2016). In this study, tDCS was delivered both before and during the fatigue task, but the increased fatigability most likely reflects non-homeostatic (online gating) tDCS effects. Thus, it is possible that accessing the homeostatic metaplastic effects of tDCS, by administering stimulation before the task, might yield different results.

There are a few limitations of note for this study. First, the relatively small number of participants may decrease the generalizability of the results. In addition, cortical excitability was not tested, and thus, the effects of higher intensity tDCS (4 mA) on the excitability of this sample are uncertain. Similarly, as mentioned above, the anode was centered over C3 and the leg areas of M1 are located in the longitudinal fissure (Foerster et al. 2018). Although the size of the electrode was sufficiently large to border or overlap Cz in all participants, indicating that M1 leg areas were covered and potentially stimulated, the different sizes of the participants’ heads might have resulted in inconsistent stimulation of the leg areas between participants. Studies targeting unilateral leg areas might benefit from using smaller electrodes and targeting C1/C2, or purposefully bordering/covering Cz to more reliably target the desired sections of M1. In addition, muscle activity of the knee extensors and flexors was not collected and the physiologic underpinnings of the results are ambiguous. The applicability of single-joint testing to functional activities may also be questionable (Kollock et al. 2015) and suggests caution in interpreting the results to real-world, multi-joint activities. Furthermore, the integrity of participant blinding in repeated-measures tDCS investigations (when participants experience more than one condition/intensity) represents a consistent limitation to studies in this field (O’Connell et al. 2012). This could be especially true for higher intensity (> 2 mA) tDCS. Sensation blinding might also be assessed by determining sensations experienced at different times during the stimulation period or by changing the ‘target’ stimulation in the sham condition. In this study, the ramp-up and ramp-down intensity for the sham condition was 2 mA. However, blinding integrity might have been enhanced by setting the intensity of the sham condition to the highest intensity used in the study (4 mA), or even exceeding the highest intensity (e.g., 5 mA). Such finding might prove beneficial for future repeated-measures tDCS studies and warrants investigation. Finally, this study applied tDCS both before and during the FT, but the optimal time of tDCS application is has not been determined. Some have suggested that tDCS may be best delivered during a task (Nitsche et al. 2008), but other evidence may recommend a time period of several minutes after stimulation (60–90 min) before peak cortical excitability occurs (Batsikadze et al. 2013).

Future work should explore the effects of tDCS at higher intensities (> 2 mA) and more closely determine the intensity dose–response. Understanding this association is especially important, because a recent review concluded that the evidence of increasing tDCS intensity to enhance outcomes was inconclusive (Esmaeilpour et al. 2018). There are also indications that increasing stimulation time and/or intensity may shift the intended tDCS effects from excitatory to inhibitory (Batsikadze et al. 2013; Monte-Silva et al. 2013). Future investigations should also include clinical populations that suffer from reduced cortical activity/excitability (e.g., multiple sclerosis, Parkinson’s disease, and stroke) and might, therefore, experience greater benefits from higher intensity tDCS. In addition, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and electromyography (EMG) should also be included to test changes in cortical excitability and the physiological effects (i.e., muscle activity) of altered brain excitability. Similarly, functional neuroimaging to better elucidate the effects of different intensities of tDCS on brain activity is also needed. Finally, the best timing of high-intensity tDCS stimulation (e.g., during and before) is a key component to improving tDCS applicability and should be efficiently investigated.

Conclusion

The 2 mA and 4 mA tDCS intensities significantly increased the fatigability of the right knee extensors in young, healthy participants, potentially from altered motor unit recruitment/discharge rate or cortical hyperexcitability. However, despite this increase in fatigability, the TW performed in both conditions was not different from sham. The 4 mA tDCS stimulation was generally well tolerated by the participants, and most sensation reports and severities were similar between the tDCS conditions. The few reports of moderate–severe sensations, with the most severe in the 4 mA condition, were transient and quickly resolved after stimulation had ended. Higher intensity stimulation, especially in clinical populations, should be systematically and considerately investigated.