Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a neurodegenerative disease associated with deficits in memory and cognitive functions (Wojsiat et al. 2018). It is the fifth leading cause of death in cases aged 65 years or older (Prasad and Bondy 2014). Aging is the main risk factor for AD, and its incidence has been increasing due to an increase in aging populations worldwide (Winblad et al. 2016). People aged 65 years or older are more vulnerable to developing AD (Prasad and Bondy 2014). In AD, there is a gradual decrease in intellectual function linked to degeneration and death of cerebral cortical neurons, which consequently leads to dementia (Terry and Davies 1980; Prasad and Bondy 2014).

Over the past few decades, different biochemical and genetic events, including elevated oxidative stress (Sultana et al. 2006; Xie et al. 2013), mitochondrial dysfunction (Shoffner et al. 1993; Gibson et al. 2000), chronic inflammation (Yamamoto et al. 2007; Ramirez et al. 2008; Calsolaro and Edison 2016; Marshe et al. 2017), Aß (1–42) peptide production from proteolysis of amyloid precursor protein (APP) (Yankner and Mesulam 1991), inherited mutations in APP, presenilin-1, and presenilin-2 genes (Mohmmad Abdul et al. 2006; Placanica et al. 2009), glia-lymphatic (glymphatic) system impairment (Bosche et al. 2020; Reeves et al. 2020), and hyperphosphorylated tau protein (Prasad and Bondy 2014; Wojsiat et al. 2018; Komaki et al. 2019), have been identified in AD leading to progressive destruction and death of nerve cells. The damaging effects of these factors causing AD become more potent with age. Morphological alterations have been found to be associated with aging in different central nervous system (CNS) areas. Aging plays a crucial role in pathophysiological mechanisms of degenerative disorders, such as AD, Parkinson’s disease (PD), and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (Matteo and Esposito 2003).

The accurate pathophysiology of AD is still unclear; however, neuroinflammation, oxidative stress, and mitochondrial function may be involved (Prasad and Bondy 2014; Wojsiat et al. 2018; Komaki et al. 2019). An increase in oxidative stress may play an important role in aging (Komaki et al. 2019). Oxidative stress due to free radical production and lipid peroxidation is associated with the pathogenesis of cognitive decline (Berr et al. 2000). The deficiency of antioxidant defenses can affect many brain functions, such as memory (Fukui et al. 2001). Neurons in the brain are highly affected by increased reactive oxygen species (ROS) formation and oxidative damage because of the high oxygen usage and energy generation (Mergenthaler et al. 2013). Enhanced oxidative damage due to free radicals can initiate long-term inflammation in AD (Uttara et al. 2009). Also, increased oxidative stress along with pro-inflammatory cytokines possibly promotes neuronal death in AD (Emerit et al. 2004; Prasad and Bondy 2014).

Due to disruption in the oxidant/antioxidant balance in AD, free radicals scavengers and/or increased oxidative stress defense mechanisms are effective in the treatment of AD (Wojsiat et al. 2018). Using antioxidants for the treatment of neurodegenerative disorders has been widely considered (Emerit et al. 2004; Federico et al. 2012). Studies on animal and AD models using single endogenous antioxidants and herbal remedies have indicated the protection of neurons against oxidative stress-related damage (Prasad and Bondy 2014). Thus, restoring mitochondrial function, reducing oxidative stress levels, and decreasing free radicals using antioxidants are the appropriate strategies to treat AD.

Coenzyme Q10 (Q10) is a natural antioxidant, which is part of the electron transport chain. Several studies have assessed the effect of Q10 on animal models of neurodegenerative disorders (Muthukumaran et al. 2014; Sikorska et al. 2014). As an intracellular antioxidant, it protects the phospholipids of the membrane and membrane protein of the mitochondria that scavenge free radicals (Navas et al. 2007; Lee et al. 2013). Mitochondrial dysfunction causes AD. In addition, Q10 is essential to generate ATP by mitochondria. Therefore, Q10 can be added to different micronutrient preparations to treat AD (Prasad and Bondy 2014; Wojsiat et al. 2018). Q10 has neuroprotection against neurodegenerative disorders (Flint 2002). Treatment with Q10 could reduce markers of oxidative stress in animal AD models (Prasad and Bondy 2014; Zhang et al. 2018). Also, Q10 supplementation affects the cholinergic system and protects cholinergic neurons in AD patients (Majumdar et al. 2014; Yang et al. 2016). On the other hand, Q10 (150 mg/day) reduced interleukin (IL-6) as an inflammatory marker (Lee et al. 2012). Q10 levels decline in the brain of aging animals (Matthews et al. 1998; Ebadi et al. 2001).

LTP occurs most prominently in the hippocampus and is regarded as a proper model to study the mechanisms of long-term changes in the CNS synaptic efficiency, resulting in learning and memory formation (Maren and Baudry 1995; Martin et al. 2000). Aβ peptides in the hippocampus can strongly inhibit synaptic plasticity.

We evaluated the Q10 effects on impaired learning and memory and synaptic plasticity in aged rats that received an intracerebroventricular (ICV) injection of Aβ. In summary, we studied whether Q10 treatment can affect the induction of hippocampal LTP in Aβ-injected old rats. Therefore, besides learning and memory behavioral methods, an in vivo field potential recording method was used to assess the neuroprotective impacts of the oral administration of Q10 on impaired hippocampal synaptic plasticity in aged rats receiving Aβ.

Materials and methods

Ethics statement

The experimental procedures were done according to the Veterinary Ethics Committee of the Hamadan University of Medical Sciences (Ethic code: IR.UMSHA.REC.1394.582), Hamadan, Iran, and conducted following the Guide for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals published by the United States National Institutes of Health (NIH Publication No. 85-23, revised 1985).

Animals and experimental design

Adult old aged (24–36 months) male Wistar rats (360–450 g) were prepared from the Hamadan University of Medical Sciences. They were kept in clear cages in a room at 23 ± 1 °C with a relative humidity of 50 ± 5%, under a 12/12 h light/dark cycle. They had free access to food and water. Following 7 days of acclimation, animals were randomly divided into four groups (n = 10 animals per group): group I (control group: receiving normal saline by oral gavage daily), group II (Aβ: receiving a unilateral ICV injection of Aβ (1–42), group III (Q10: receiving 50 mg/kg of Q10 by oral gavage daily), and group IV (Aβ+Q10: Q10 administration by oral gavage daily for 4 weeks before ICV injection of Aβ (Komaki et al. 2019). The experiment timeline is shown in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Experimental timeline. After 4 weeks of treatment of experimental groups with Q10 (50 mg/kg), to induce an AD model, xylazine (10 mg/kg) and ketamine (100 mg/kg) were used to anesthetize rats, and they were placed in the stereotaxic apparatus. An intraventricular injection of Aβ (2 μL) was done (1 μL/2 min). Following recovery, open field and novel object recognition (NOR) tasks were performed. To measure spatial and aversive learning and memory following the training trials, Morris water maze (MWM) and shuttle box tests were performed, respectively. Then, electrophysiological recordings were conducted to determine the excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP) slope and population spike (PS) amplitude in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus. Induction of LTP was done through a high-frequency stimulation of the perforant pathway. After the experimental procedures, the levels of the biomarkers of oxidative stress were calculated by serum assessment

ICV injection of Aβ and neurosurgery

To induce an AD model, Aβ (1–42) (100 μg; Tocris Bioscience, UK) was dissolved in PBS (100 μL; vehicle solution), followed by incubation (37 °C/7 days) before usage. This process formed amyloid fibrils, which are toxic to the nervous system (Lorenzo and Yankner 1994; Asadbegi et al. 2017; Asadbegi et al. 2018). Anesthetization of the rats was done with intraperitoneal (IP) injections of ketamine and xylazine (100 and 10 mg/kg, respectively) (Ahmadi et al. 2021a), before transferring to the stereotaxic device (Stoelting Co., USA). After exposing the skull, we drilled holes in the skull on the ventricular region considering the coordinates as follows: 2 mm lateral to the midline, 1.2 mm posterior to bregma, and 4 mm ventral to the cortex surface (Paxinos and Watson 2005). A 10-μL microsyringe (Hamilton Laboratory Products, USA) was used for injections that lasted 5 min. We kept the syringe in place for 5 min following the injection before its removal. The injection volume (5 μL) was administrated slowly (1 μL/min), and the animals had 5–7 days of recovery (Komaki et al. 2019).

Locomotor activity in the open field

The open field (OF) test was applied to measure locomotor activity. The apparatus was made of white acrylic (50 cm (length) × 50 cm (width) × 38 cm (height)). The field was lit by low ambient room lights (Bisagno et al. 2004). An overhead video camera recorded the time spent by animals in an open field area, and the obtained data were analyzed using video track software. Animals were located in the middle of the OF and could explore for 10 min (Drews et al. 2005). The locomotor activity was considered as the total distance traveled and average velocity (Etaee et al. 2017).

Novel object recognition (NOR) test

NOR was first designed by Ennanceur and Delacour according to the animals’ spontaneous behavior to recognize a new object in a familiar environment (Ennaceur and Delacour 1988). Recognition memory as a kind of declarative memory can measure the animal’s ability to judge or discriminate between objects, considering tactile and visual information (Ennaceur and Delacour 1988; Barker et al. 2007; Antunes and Biala 2012). The NOR test measures cortical and hippocampal functioning. Basically, the task was performed as in our previous studies (Ganji et al. 2017) (Fig. 2). The open box has a black wooden floor (50 × 45 × 35 cm) equipped with video recording software.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Novel object recognition (NOR) test was first designed by Ennanceur and Delacour according to the animals’ spontaneous behavior to recognize a new object in a familiar environment

On the first day, habituation and acquisition phases were performed. Accordingly, two habituation sessions (10 min) in the arena with no object at an interval of 30 min were considered. The training was performed for 30 min following habituation. In this phase, two identical objects were positioned near two adjacent corners of the arena, and the animal was positioned in the middle of the box and had 10 min to find the objects (Cohen and Stackman Jr 2015). The exploration process was regarded as smelling the object. Then, 24 h later, a retention test was done. In this phase, the objects were replaced by a novel (unfamiliar) object, and the animal was placed in the open field for 10 min. The discrimination index (DI) for the new object was measured by dividing the time spent exploring the new object by the total exploration time recorded by video tracking software.

Assessment of spatial memory using Morris water maze (MWM) task

The MWM test as a hippocampal-dependent test assesses spatial learning and memory in rodents (Zarrinkalam et al. 2018; Omidi et al. 2019). As described previously (Karimi et al. 2019; Shekarian et al. 2020; Ahmadi et al. 2021b), the MWM has a black circular pool (diameter: 180 cm; depth: 60 cm) full of water (22 ± 1 °C) to a depth of 35 cm. It is divided into four quadrants at equal distances along the pool rim and has four starting sites. There is a hidden platform (diameter: 10 cm) placed 1 cm below the water surface in the middle of the northern quadrant, which is consistent for all rats throughout the training tasks. In brief, the training session included a block of eight trials daily for 4 consecutive days. The rats were located in the water at one of the four different locations, and the time between the entrance into the water and escape toward the platform (escape latency) was recorded. Each rat was given two blocks of four trials for 60 s at about the same time every day (10:00–12:00) for 4 continuous days. The subjects could stay for 30 s on the platform between the two trials and had 5 min of resting time between the two continuous blocks. When a rat could not find the platform for 60 s, the researcher guided it to the platform and could remain there for 30 s. A video camera (Nikon, Melville, USA) connected to a computer was placed above the pool for recording the parameters, such as the time spent to reach the submerged platform (escape latency), the length of the swimming path (traveled distance), and the time spent in the target quadrant. On day 5, each animal was subjected to one probe trial (60 s) and a visible platform trial. The platform was not available in the probe trials; however, in the visible platform trials, an aluminum foil was used to cover the platform. Escape latency, distance traveled, and the mean swimming speed were recorded in each trial (Zarrinkalam et al. 2018).

Passive avoidance learning (PAL) test

In the PAL task, the subjects learn to avoid an environment where they previously received an adverse stimulus (foot shock). The PAL apparatus, as well as the procedure, were similar to those we previously described (Zarrinkalam et al. 2016; Karimi et al. 2020). In brief, the apparatus is made of transparent plastic and has a rectangular opening guillotine door (6 × 8 cm) to separate the light compartment (20 ×20 ×30 cm) from the dark one (20 × 20 ×30 cm). The dark compartment floor has stainless steel rods (3 mm diameter) placed 1 cm apart. A shock generator is attached to the electrified floor of the dark box. The test was conducted for 2 continuous days. On the first day, after two steps of the habituation trial, training was performed by applying an intermittent electric shock (1.5 s, 0.4 mA intensity) upon the rat’s entrance into the dark chamber through an isolated stimulator. The rat was gently located in the white chamber, and 5 s later, the guillotine door was opened and the rat could enter the dark compartment. Once entering the dark box, the door was closed, followed by delivering an electric shock through the floor grid (acquisition trial). Latency to enter the dark chamber (STLa) and the number of electrical shocks received to the acquisition were noted. Then, animals were transferred to their home cages. On day 2 (memory test), they received no electrical shock. Each animal was again placed in the light compartment (retention trial), and latency to enter the dark chamber (STLr) and the total time spent in the dark compartment (TDC) (as an indicator of inhibitory avoidance behavior) were recorded (Zarrinkalam et al. 2016).

Surgical procedures, electrophysiological recordings, and induction of LTP

The used methods were similar to previous studies (Nazari et al. 2016; Komaki et al. 2017; Omidi et al. 2020a; Omidi et al. 2020b). In brief, after an IP injection of urethane (1.5 g/kg), animals were anesthetized and positioned in the stereotaxic device for surgical procedure and recording. After exposing the skull, we placed a stainless steel concentric bipolar electrode (diameter: 125 μm, insulated with Teflon) in the lateral perforant path (PP) according to the following coordinates: 4.3 mm lateral to the midline, 8.1 mm posterior to bregma, and 3.2 mm ventral below the skull surface (Paxinos and Watson 2005). Also, a bipolar recording electrode (3.8 mm posterior to bregma and 2.3 mm lateral to the midline) was moved to the dentate gyrus (DG), until detecting the maximum field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSP) (commonly, 2.7–3.2 mm ventral).

Electrophysiological recordings

The electrophysiological recordings, as well as LTP induction, were similar to those previously described (Tahmasebi et al. 2015; Nazari et al. 2016; Komaki et al. 2017; Omidi et al. 2020a; Omidi et al. 2020b). After anesthetization of the animals using an IP injection of urethane (ethyl carbamate; Sigma, USA; 1.5 g/kg), they were located in the stereotaxic device. The skin was incised, and the DG location (ML: 2.3 mm from the midline, AP: −3.8 mm from the bregma, and DV: 2.7–3.2 mm from the skull surface) and perforant pathway (PP) (ML: 4.3 mm from the midline, AP: −8.1 mm from the bregma, and DV: 3.2 mm from the skull surface) were defined (Paxinos and Watson 2005), followed by drilling two small burr holes into the DG and PP. Next, bipolar stimulating and recording electrodes were gently lowered along the cortex into the PP and DG regions, respectively. They moved very gently to observe the maximum field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs). After stimulating the PP, the field potential recordings were recorded in the DG.

By changing the single-pulse stimulation intensity and averaging ten responses for each intensity, we generated the input/output (I/O) response curve. The evoked field potential was 50% of the maximum response in the following stimulations (Omidi et al. 2020b). The stimuli (single 0.1 ms biphasic square wave) characteristics were defined according to our package (eTrace, www.sciencebeam.com) followed by sending to a constant current isolator unit (A365, World Precision Instruments, US) at 0.1 Hz.

After making sure of the base response sustainability (about 45 min), high-frequency stimulation (HFS) was delivered for the induction of LTP. HFS protocol was as follows: 400 Hz, 10 bursts of 20 stimuli, 0.2-ms stimulus duration, and the 10-s interburst interval at a stimulus intensity. The fEPSP slope, as well as two variables of maximal response, were noted at 5, 30, and 60 min following HFS for evaluating changes in the DG neuron’s synaptic response.

Biochemical analysis

At the end of the experiment, 5 ml of blood samples were taken from the portal vein by cardiac puncture and transferred into heparinized tubes. The samples were then centrifuged at 3500 rpm for 10 min at 4 °C. Serum samples were frozen at −80 °C and sent for biochemistry measurement. Finally, plasma measurements were performed for malondialdehyde (MDA), total antioxidant capacity (TAC), total thiol group (TTG), and total oxidant status (TOS) (Komaki et al. 2019).

Measurement of TAC

TAC determination in serum was performed by ferric reducing antioxidant power assay (FRAP) (Benzie and Strain 1999). It can reduce 2,4,6-Tris(2-pyridyl)-s-triazine (Fe III-TPTZ) to Fe II-TPTZ (blue) with biological antioxidants. Alterations in absorbance of the sample at 600 nm were compared with FeSO4 7H2O, which was considered a standard (Salehi et al. 2015).

Measurement of MDA

The MDA generation to assess lipid peroxidation (LPO) was distinguished by reaction with thiobarbituric acid (TBA). Briefly, the determination of the LPO products (MDA) was done through the addition of 1.0 mL of 1% TBA reactive substances and 1.0 mL of 20% trichloroacetic acid to the supernatant (100 μL) and then incubating the solution (100 °C/80 min). The solution was cooled on ice, followed by centrifugation (3000 rpm/20 min), and reading the supernatant absorbance at 532 nm (Salehi et al. 2015).

Determination of the TOS

The Earl’s method determined serum TOS (2005). Briefly, 225 μL of reagent 1 (150 μM xylenol orange, 140 mM NaCl, and 1.35 M glycerol in H2SO4 solution (25 mM; pH1.75)) was mixed with the sample (35 μL). The absorbance was read by a spectrophotometer at 560 nm (sample blank), then, 11 μL of reagent 2 (5 μM ferrous ion and 10 mM o-dianisidine in 25 mM H2SO4 solution) was added to the mixture, and then incubation was done for 3 to 4 min. Afterward, its absorption was read at 560 nm. The analytical sensitivity was considered 0.0076 absorbance/μM. Following calibration of the assay with H2O2, the values were reported as mmol H2O2 equivalent/L. Measurement of the method detection limit was done by evaluating the zero calibrators ten times. The detection limit (the mean TOS of the zero calibrators more than the standard deviation) was 1.13 μmol H2O2 equivalent/L (Akalιn et al. 2007; Aslan et al. 2014).

Measurement of TTG

The Ellman’s reagent (DTNB; 5,5’-dithio-bis-(2-nitrobenzoic acid)) was applied to determine TTG following the Hu method (Hu 1994).

Statistical analysis

Values are provided as mean ± SEM and analyzed using GraphPad Prism® 5.0. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to analyze the data obtained from the NOR and PAL tests. Two-way ANOVA was employed to analyze the data obtained from the spatial learning evaluation (training trials) considering days and treatments as repeated measures and between-subjects factors, respectively. One-way and two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni and Tukey’s tests were applied to analyze the data obtained from the probe, working, visibility trials, and biochemical data. Electrophysiology results were analyzed by the two-way ANOVA with repeated measures analysis. Tukey’s multiple comparison test was applied to analyze the significance of the differences between the groups, when appropriate. A p-value of smaller than 0.05 was regarded as significant.

Results

Locomotor activity

Based on the results, no significant differences were found in the distance traveled (Fig. 3A) and mean velocity (Fig. 3B) in OF test between the groups [(p > 0.05); F (3, 34) = 2/992)].

Fig. 3
figure 3

Comparison of distance traveled (A) and mean velocity (cm/s) (B) in the open field arena between experimental groups. Columns indicate mean ± SEM

NOR test

According to the results, the Aβ group had a significantly lower DI (p < 0.01) in comparison with the control, Q10, and Aβ+Q10 groups (Fig. 4). Therefore, DI increased by Q10 in Aβ-injected rats.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Comparison of discrimination indices of the novel object recognition task between the experimental groups. Columns indicate mean ± SEM. ** p < 0.01 versus the control group and # p < 0.05 and ## p < 0.01 versus the Aβ group

MWM task

In the control, Q10, and Aβ+Q10 rats, escape latency showed a significant decrease (control rats: p < 0.001; Q10 rats: p < 0.001; Aβ+Q10 rats: p > 0.05) on day 4 in comparison with the first day (Fig. 5A). In Aβ-injected rats, escape latency failed to change after 4 days of training (p > 0.05). Therefore, Q10 attenuated Aβ deteriorative impact on memory acquisition. All groups except the Aβ group could learn the invisible platform position following 4 days of training.

Fig. 5
figure 5

The average latency to find the invisible platform (A) and swimming distance (B) vs. training days. CoQ10 significantly decreased the latency and distances. Values are provided as mean ± SEM. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, and *** p < 0.001 versus the control group and # p < 0.05, ## p < 0.01, and ### p < 0.001 versus the Aβ group

The experimental groups except Aβ animals had a shorter swimming path during training (p < 0.05) (Fig. 5B). A significant difference was detected between the experimental groups in escape latency (p < 0.001; [F (9, 84) = 2/247]); two-way ANOVA) and swimming distance (p < 0.001, [F (9, 84) = 3/871]). We used swimming speed to measure the rats’ motor activity, which showed no significant changes through training (p > 0.05, data not shown).

Based on the results, Aβ-injected rats receiving Q10 (Aβ+Q10 group) were observed with a significant reduction in escape latency and moved distance than the Aβ-injected rats (Aβ group) (p < 0.05) (Fig. 5A). Therefore, Q10 decreased the Aβ deteriorative effects on memory acquisition and hence improved Aβ-related reference memory impairment.

In addition, a probe trial was performed 24 h following the last training trial on day 5, in which we removed the platform and noted time spent in each quadrant. In Aβ-treated rats, time in the target zone decreased compared to the other three groups. The Aβ+Q10 group spent a longer time in the target zone than the Aβ group (p <0.05) (control rats: 17.91 ± 0.8 s, n = 9; Q10: 18.35 ± 0.86 s, n = 9; Aβ: 10.07 ± 1.73 s, n = 9; Aβ+Q10: 14.67 ± 0.85 s, n = 9, p < 0.001, [F (3, 32) = 11/41]) (Fig. 6). Thus, Q10 could increase time spent in the target area and improve spatial memory impairment in Aβ-treated animals. There were no significant differences between experimental groups in escape latency to discover the visible platform in the visual discrimination task (data not shown, p > 0.05), which indicates no visual deficit in rats.

Fig. 6
figure 6

A probe test was used to measure spatial reference memory. Values are provided as mean ± SEM. *** p < 0.001 versus the control group and # p < 0.05 and ### p < 0.001 versus the Aβ group

PAL test

In the training phase, the results showed that there were no significant differences between the experimental groups in STLa (p > 0.05) (Fig. 7A). Moreover, the experimental groups showed a significant difference in the number of trials to acquisition (p < 0.001). Based on the results, the number of trials to acquisition was significantly higher in the Aβ rats in comparison with the control and Q10 groups (p <0.01) (Fig. 7B). Meanwhile, Q10 administration failed to change the number of trials to acquisition in the Aβ+Q10 group in comparison with the Aβ group (p > 0.05) (Fig. 7B).

Fig. 7
figure 7

Effect of CoQ10 oral administration in the training phase (A, B) and the retrieval phase (C, D) of the step-through passive avoidance task. Entrance latency into the dark chamber (A) and the number of trials to acquisition (B) in the training stage, and latency to enter into the dark chamber (C) and the time spent in the dark chamber (D) in the retrieval phase in the experimental groups are displayed. Columns indicate mean ± SEM. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, and *** p < 0.001 versus the control group and ## p < 0.01 and ### p < 0.001 versus the Aβ group

There was a significant difference between experimental groups in the retrieval phase of the PAL and memory test (p < 0.001). The STLr in Aβ rats was significantly lower compared to the control group (p < 0.001). However, the Aβ and Aβ+Q10 groups showed no remarkable differences (p > 0.05) (Fig. 7C). Also, the experimental groups were found with a significant difference in TDC (p < 0.001). The Aβ group showed a longer TDC compared to the control and Q10 groups (p < 0.01 and p < 0.001, respectively) (Fig. 7D). However, no significant difference was found between the Aβ+Q10 and Aβ groups (p >0.05).

Measurement of evoked potentials

Population spike (PS) and fEPSP are two components of the evoked field potential in the DG. The fEPSP slope was considered as 20–80% of the height amplitude (Asadbegi et al. 2016; Komaki et al. 2017; Komaki et al. 2019). The data analysis tool (eTrace, www.sciencebeam.com) was used to measure the fEPSP slope and PS amplitude.

Through the application of HFS to the PP, we induced LTP in the DG to assess the Q10 impacts on the EPSP slopes as well as PS amplitudes of granular cells of DG in AD animals. Figures 8 and 9 display the effects of Aβ and Q10 on the EPSP slope and PS amplitude, respectively. The significant effect of time (p < 0.001; [F (3, 28) = 17/96]) and treatment (p < 0.001; [F (3, 84) = 21/28]) was found on the EPSP slope of the DG granular cells (Fig. 8).

Fig. 8
figure 8

Time-dependent alterations in hippocampal responses to perforant path stimulation after high-frequency stimulation (HFS). Long-term potentiation (LTP) of the EPSP slope in DG granular cell synapses of the hippocampus showed significant differences between groups. Values are provided as mean ± SEM % of the baseline. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01, and *** p < 0.001 versus the control group and # p < 0.05 versus the Aβ group

Fig. 9
figure 9

Time-dependent alterations in long-term potentiation (LTP) of population spike (PS) amplitudes in the dentate gyrus (DG) granular cell synapses against perforant pathway stimulation after high-frequency stimulation (HFS). Values are provided as means ± SEM % of the baseline. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, and *** p < 0.001 versus the control group and # p < 0.05, ## p < 0.01, and ### p < 0.001 versus the Aβ group

Aβ could cause a significant reduction in the hippocampal EPSP slope than the control and Q10 groups (Aβ: n = 8, 102.8 ± 6.51; control: n = 8, 149.9 ± 8.39 [p < 0.001]; Q10: n = 8, 132.3 ± 3.62; [p < 0.05]). Q10 administration compensated for a reduction in the EPSP slope than the Aβ group (Q10+Aβ, n = 8, 115.8 ± 5.41; Aβ: 102.8 ± 6.51; but it was not significant at p > 0.05).

The obtained results indicated the significant effect of time (p < 0.001; [F (3, 28) = 196/7]) and treatment (p < 0.001; [F (3, 84) = 24/18]) on the PS amplitude of the DG granular cells (Fig. 9). According to the Tukey’s post hoc results, Aβ could cause a significant impairment in the PS amplitude (Aβ: 172.1 ± 13.52; control: 318.8 ± 24.26; Q10: 301.0+19.37; Aβ versus the control [p < 0.001]). The Aβ and Aβ+Q10 groups were found with significant differences in the PS amplitude following Q10 administration (Aβ: 172.1 ± 13.52; Aβ+Q10: 245.1 ± 13.10, Aβ vs. Aβ+Q10 [p < 0.05]). Furthermore, no significant differences were observed in PS amplitude between the Q10 and control groups (Q10: 301.0 + 19.37; control: 318.8 ± 24.26 [p > 0.05]). These findings demonstrated that Aβ injection (ICV) could impair the LTP of the EPSP slope and PS amplitude, whereas treatment with Q10 could increase them.

Effect of Q10 and Aβ on TAC, TOS, TTG, and MDA

Analysis of TAC indicated a significant difference between experimental groups [F (3, 28) = 77/11, p < 0.001]. Moreover, there was a significant difference in the TOS among the experimental groups [F (3, 24) = 31/86, p < 0.001]. In addition, there was a significant difference in the MDA and the TTG levels between the groups [F (3, 28) = 13/71, p < 0.001] [F (3, 24) = 23/69, p < 0.001] (Fig. 10).

Fig. 10
figure 10

The effect of Aβ and Q10 on plasma parameters of total antioxidant capacity (TAC) (A), total oxidant status (TOS) (B), MDA (C), and total thiol group (TTG) (D) levels. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. * p <0.05, ** p <0.01, and *** p < 0.001 versus the control group. # p < 0.05, ## p < 0.01, and ### p < 0.001 versus the Aβ group

Discussion

We investigated whether Q10 treatment can prevent the learning, memory, and synaptic plasticity impairment caused by ICV Aβ injection in aged rats. We used old rats to create a better AD model similar to AD patients.

The main findings of our study were as follows: Q10 could improve the Aβ-related decrease in the DI in the NOR test, spatial learning and memory in the MWM test, passive avoidance learning and memory in the PAL test, and synaptic plasticity impairment in the hippocampal PP-DG pathway in aged rats. In addition, Aβ injection significantly increased serum MDA and TOS levels. Q10, however, significantly reversed these parameters and also increased TAC and TTG levels in the Aβ+Q10 group.

Q10 alone showed no significant effectiveness in the learning and memory performance of aged rats (Q10 group) evidenced by the NOR, PAL, and MWM tests, and also synaptic plasticity (LTP induction). In this regard, in normal conditions, treatment with Q10 (Mcdonald et al. 2005; Sumien et al. 2009) or different antioxidant chemicals was not capable of improving memory performance (Monsef et al. 2019). Therefore, an increase in antioxidant concentration and activity in physiological conditions is not effective in cognition and may even cause a decrease in this process (Bouayed and Bohn 2010).

No significant differences were found in the distance traveled between the experimental groups. Therefore, Aβ or treatment with Q10 could not significantly affect the motor activity of animals, which is consistent with the findings of other studies (Dumont et al. 2011).

According to the data obtained from the training stage of the PAL task, no significant differences were found in STLa between the experimental groups. STLa indicates the rats’ natural preference for dark places. Thus, Q10 or Aβ could not significantly affect the rat behavior before delivering the initial shock and initiation of the acquisition process in the PAL test. In the training stage, the Aβ group was found with an elevation in the number of trials to acquisition in comparison with the control group. This result demonstrates that Aβ can impair the acquisition phase of PAL, which is highly associated with the integrity of some brain areas, such as the hippocampus and the amygdala (Gold 2004). Aβ rats treated with Q10 (Aβ+Q10) failed to change the number of trials to acquisition compared to the Aβ group. This finding can be owing to the research design, experiment’s conditions and duration, and Q10 dosage. In the PAL test, STLr and TDC are regarded as indicators of the retrieval phase of passive avoidance learning and memory processing (Zarrinkalam et al. 2018; Omidi et al. 2019). The Aβ group was found with lower STLr and longer TDC than the control group, which indicates a decrease in the retrieval of PAL memory in the Aβ group than the control group. Q10 failed to change the STLr and TDC in Aβ-treated rats.

According to the data obtained from the memory retrieval of the PAL and NOR tasks, Q10 could improve memory and cognitive deficit in the Aβ+Q10 group in the NOR test, and Q10 could enhance the DI in the NOR test in the Aβ+Q10 group, which indicates the greater effectiveness of Q10 in the brain NOR-processing regions compared to PAL-related areas. Q10 alone caused no significant effect in both PAL or NOR tests in the Q10 group. These findings demonstrate that Q10 was not capable of enhancing learning and memory in the Q10 group.

According to the results of the NOR test, the Aβ group showed less time exploring the new object compared to the three other groups, which can result from neophobia as one of the pitfalls in assessing memory in non-verbal animals (Thorpe et al. 2004). Medial temporal lobe systems, like the hippocampus and amygdala, are associated with emotion-associated memory. The amygdala plays an important role in the processing of emotion, whereas the hippocampus is involved in episodic memory (Yang and Wang 2017). The amygdala modifies both the storage and encoding of hippocampal-dependent memories (Phelps 2004). The hippocampus and basolateral amygdala groups (BLA) can work separately to exhibit their various functions in emotion and memory. Nonetheless, synergism in the amygdala-hippocampus axis is involved in emotion-regulated memories (Yang and Wang 2017). The amygdala is important to learn the valence of new objects from the emotional expressions of others (Blair et al. 2008). Also, amygdala atrophy occurs in early AD (Poulin et al. 2011). Lesions of the amygdala in monkeys could disrupt emotional behavior in some new objects (Zola-Morgan et al. 1991; Sarowar et al. 2017). Thus, amygdala atrophy because of Aβ-induced AD can impair emotional behavior regarding new objects (Zola-Morgan et al. 1991; Sarowar et al. 2017) in AD. Therefore, the shorter exploration time of the Aβ group can be due to memory impairment, not neophobia. The time exploring the novel object was similar in transgenic and wild-type AD mice (Baazaoui and Iqbal 2017). Both groups were similar regarding non-spatial working memory and exploration of a new object. Hence, the neophobia effect on the exploration of new objects in our research might be eliminated.

DI to new objects increased by Q10 in Aβ-treated animals. Thus, Q10 improved non-spatial memory deficit in Aβ-treated rats. Muthukumaran et al. (2018) reported that transgenic AD mice, whether or not they were treated with Q10, acted similarly to non-transgenic mice in the NOR test. Nonetheless, only transgenic mice that received Q10 recognized the familiar object moving to a new location (the NLR test) similar to non-transgenic mice (Muthukumaran et al. 2018)).

The current research on the Q10 neuroprotective impact on cognition in Aβ-injected aged male rats showed that Aβ could significantly diminish spatial memory acquisition and impair spatial memory retention. Q10 could increase the level of memory acquisition and retention, which was found in Aβ rats. Treatment with Q10 attenuated Aβ deteriorative effect on memory and learning in aged male rats. Therefore, supplementation with Q10 improved learning and memory in Aβ-treated rats. Furthermore, the Q10 alone did not affect swimming speed, which suggests that locomotion improvement cannot lead to spatial learning improvement. AD causes a remarkable impairment in memory (Selkoe 2002). Aβ impairs alternation behavior as well as passive avoidance task scores in rats (Rasoolijazi et al. 2013). Also, Aβ intrahippocampal injection induced significant learning impairments in PAL and MWM tests (Asadbegi et al. 2017; Komaki et al. 2019). Our findings are consistent with other studies on Aβ-induced memory impairment (Asadbegi et al. 2017; Shekarian et al. 2020). The following mechanisms are possibly involved in memory and cognitive impairments in AD: (1) changes in synapses and neurons in brain areas associated with learning and memory functions, such as the basal forebrain, the hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, and neocortical association cortices (Mattson et al. 1999) and (2) Aβ neurotoxicity that increases apoptosis, inflammatory responses, the cell wall permeability, and free radical damage (Clippingdale et al. 2001) because an increase in Aβ (Aß 1–42) is involved in the AD pathogenesis (Prasad and Bondy 2014).

In our experiment, Aβ injection significantly increased serum MDA and TOS levels, indicating an increase in serum ROS and oxidant components. Q10 supplementation, however, significantly reversed these parameters and increased TAC and TTG levels, which indicated that Q10 treatment could alter the oxidant/antioxidant balance, in favor of antioxidants. There is a significant reduction in the antioxidant enzyme activities, such as catalase, glutathione peroxidase, and superoxide dismutase in various brain areas in AD (Venkateshappa et al. 2012). Increased oxidant events can increase the generation and accumulation of Aβ. Increased oxidative events accelerate the intracellular buildup of Aβ in neurons (Misonou et al. 2000; Prasad and Bondy 2014). Consequently, Aβ oligomerization, overproduction, and aggregation increase oxidative stress. After the initiation of the positive feedback loop, where ROS generation and Aβ are connected, this loop gently can enhance and accelerate brain pathology (Wojsiat et al. 2018).

Q10 can prevent memory and cognitive impairments in different conditions (Sandhir et al. 2014). Q10 also could improve spatial learning, reduce oxidative injury, and delay early aging (Papucci et al. 2003). Also, Q10 improved cognitive deficit, in experimental models of AD and PD, through an improvement in electron transport within the MC membrane (Sandhir et al. 2014). Q10 possibly can be used for the treatment of AD. Treatment with Q10 in STZ-infused rats could reverse oxidative damage and decline ATP levels in the hippocampus as well as the cerebral cortex of rats injected with STZ (ICV). Q10 modulated cognitive deficit caused by STZ injection (ICV) in rats in the MWM and PAL tests (Ishrat et al. 2006). Cholinergic signaling plays a role in learning and memory formation, and its modification causes cognitive impairment in AD (Cummings 2000). Q10 supplementation improves memory processing through an improvement in hippocampal cholinergic function by an increase in choline acetyltransferase and a decrease in acetylcholinesterase activities (Ishrat et al. 2006), neurogenesis by a reduction in Aβ formation (Muthukumaran et al. 2018) and oxidative stress markers, and an increase in ATP amounts in the hippocampus and cortex (Ishrat et al. 2006). In diabetic and ischemic injury model rats, pre-treatment with Q10 (a strong antioxidant and free radical scavenger in the hippocampus and DG) significantly decreased neuronal loss because of ischemia and decreased activity of caspase-3 associated with apoptotic cell death (Young et al. 2007).

The elevated levels of oxidative stress, mitochondrial impairment, and metabolic aberrancies are the main factors to induce AD (Mattson et al. 1999). In this study, we found that Q10 prevented the memory and cognitive deficit caused by Aβ in aged rats; however, it was weaker compared to adult rats. More ROS are generated in the mitochondria of aged animals, and also, their mitochondria are characterized by higher amounts of lipid peroxides and losses of polyunsaturated fatty acids, suggesting elevated oxidative stress in old age. Therefore, ROS are highly involved in the aging process (Ebadi et al. 2001). There are several factors promoting progressive neurodegeneration, and their deleterious effects become more potent in aging. For example, the glymphatic system causes waste removal and brain fluid clearance during sleep through glia-supported perivascular channels. The impairment of the glymphatic system reduced the clearance of AD-associated proteins. Human studies have reported a diminish in the glymphatic function in AD (Reeves et al. 2020; Silva et al. 2021).

LTP is a persistent elevation in synaptic strength in the hippocampus and has been a famous model to investigate learning and memory formation (Komaki et al. 2019). Aβ caused impairments in synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus in a rat model of AD (Asadbegi et al. 2016). Measurements of hippocampal LTP, as a cellular mechanism of learning and memory, indicate the probable mechanisms of Q10 function in such situations. We found that Aβ attenuated hippocampal LTP induction in aged rats, which is in line with the idea that intrahippocampal injection of Aβ can reduce LTP induction in adult rats (Asadbegi et al. 2016). Aβ injection decreased LTP, whereas treatment with Q10 enhanced LTP in Aβ-injected adult rats (Komaki et al. 2019). The hippocampus is highly associated with learning and memory. AD is initiated with slight changes in synaptic efficacy in the hippocampus leading to noticeable neuronal degeneration (Selkoe 2002).

From another perspective, elevated chronic inflammatory processes are considered an important factor to initiate and promote neurodegeneration in AD and associated neurological diseases regarding inflammatory endothelium activation (Bentz et al. 2010; Bosche et al. 2020; Haupt et al. 2020; Tso et al. 2021). Many investigations using non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs, such as aspirin and ibuprofen, indicated their effectiveness in patients with idiopathic AD (Rich et al. 1995). Therefore, using anti-inflammatory agents combined with antioxidants can be used to reduce the progression of AD (Prasad and Bondy 2014). The Q10 level showed a negative correlation with inflammatory markers, such as TNF-α and IL-6 (Lee et al. 2013). Also, Q10 exerted anti-inflammation activities by a decrease in nuclear factor-κB (NF-κB)-dependent gene expression. NF-κB is activated by the ROS leading to up-regulation of the expression levels of pro-inflammatory cytokines. Nonetheless, this NF-κB-activating cascade is suppressed by antioxidants, such as Q10 (Lee et al. 2013).

Improvement in the CoQ10 bioavailability and consequently its efficiency can be involved in better protection against Aβ-related injury. Although the oil-soluble Q10 exhibits effective neuroprotection in animal models of neurodegenerative disorders, it showed no effect in clinical trials of PD, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, and Huntington’s disease (Muthukumaran et al. 2018).

Such clinical failures were probably due the low bioavailability in such lipid-solubility CoQ10 formulations at comparable doses used on preclinical trials not allowed in FDA-approved human clinical trials. Because oil-soluble CoQ10 at our low dosage could have also been a limiting factor in our research, we tried to improve the Q10 bioavailability with a water-soluble formulation (produced by or obtained from the study of Muthukumaran et al. (2018)). Therefore, clinical trials may have failed because of FDA required much lower doses administrated to participants. Hence, a lower dose of Q10 (50 mg/kg/day) in our research may be allowed for a clinical trial.

Our study may not be a preclinical test on the efficacy of Q10 in preventing AD, but this formulation can be a promising contender for such an effect. MSW or PAL tests are applied in preclinical research and are not appropriate indicants of cognition not caused by AD-induced heightened adverse emotional effects that this treatment might alleviate. Therefore, it can be expected that the clinical trials have provided much less evidence of preserved cognition in AD participants tested in non-threatening contexts. Accordingly, preclinical trials of Q10 can be conducted only on tests, such as NOR/NLR in non-threatening settings. As mentioned, neophobia as a pitfall in the measurement of memory can be eliminated.

Our study was associated with some limitations. Our used model is still not representative of the clinical situation of patients with AD and leads to no direct deductive conclusion. Also, several compounds exhibiting preclinical prevention of AD precursors in the brain of rodents or preventing cognitive-behavioral deficits could not pass clinical trials.

Conclusion

Supplementation with Q10 significantly improved learning and memory deficits and synaptic plasticity impairment induced by Aβ in aged rats. The Q10 concentration is decreased in the brain of aging animals and administration of Q10 minimized Aβ-induced AD severity. Such protective effects can be due to the antioxidant, anti-inflammatory, anti-aging, and antiapoptotic effects of Q10 and also maintenance of mitochondrial integrity and prevention of membrane damage. Accordingly, Q10 is a possible neuroprotective agent against age-related neurodegeneration diseases, such as AD. However, further investigation is necessary to elucidate the possible neuroprotection mechanisms of Q10 on learning and memory and also synaptic plasticity in aged subjects.