Introduction

Pain is a complex phenomenon influenced by a variety of biological, psychological, and social factors [1]. Psychological factors are powerful predictors of the experience of pain [2] and psychological models focus on the characteristic patterns of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors that influence pain perception. Personality is one such characteristic. Together, personality traits and other patterns of thoughts and feelings, such as depression and anxiety, are modeled as either protective or as risk factors for pain. The disease model has focused traditionally on psychological deficits that pose risk factors for pain and related outcomes, such as quality of life or functional impairment [3]. However, more holistic models have been developed to include a focus on protective psychological factors or attributes that promote health, lower pain perception, and increase quality of life [4, 5].

Considering psychological health, it is noted that health is not just the absence of stress or mental illness (i.e., languishing), but also the presence of flourishing (i.e., well-being) [6]. Flourishing entails three factors that reflect psychological health: positive emotions, which indicate emotional well-being, such as positive affect and quality of life; positive psychological functioning, which reflects psychological well-being, such as self-acceptance and personal growth; and positive social functioning, which indicates social well-being, such as social contribution and social integration. The presence of positive personality traits, such as optimism, is indicative of flourishing. This article examines the protective link provided by positive personality traits, including optimism, hope, and self-efficacy, and how these traits may influence pain perception. Further, we will discuss extensive research that has defined the well-established relationship between the potential risk factor, pain catastrophizing, a maladaptive coping mechanism, and pain perception and other related outcomes. Additionally, we will review the inverse relationship between positive traits and pain catastrophizing, and offer a model that explains the inverse link between positive traits and pain perception through lower pain catastrophizing. Finally, we discuss clinical practice recommendations based on the aforementioned relationships.

Methods

We reviewed primary source research articles that were published or in press from January 2000 to January 2013. We retrieved studies between September 2012 and January 2013 from the online databases PubMed, Psych Info, Google Scholar and Academic Premier using the following keywords alone and in combination: pain catastrophizing, pain perception, pain, positive traits, optimism, hope, self-efficacy, and depression.

Positive Traits and Pain Perception

Numerous studies have demonstrated a protective link between positive personality traits and pain perception [7]. A majority of studies have focused on optimism in clinical populations and shown how optimism may predict or indicate the strength of the relationship between pain and many important life outcomes. Optimism is a generalized expectancy for positive outcomes [8] and is often measured with the Life Orientation Test-Revised (see Table 1). Clinical populations studied include patients with cancer [9, 10], sickle cell disease [11], osteoarthritis [12], and face pain [13]. Among 218 late-stage cancer patients undergoing chemotherapy, optimism played a small protective role with severity of pain [9]. Among 334 lung cancer patients, optimism partially mediated the link between pain and quality of life [10]. A similar finding emerged with 72 older adults with osteoarthritis in that optimism partially explained the relationship between pain and life satisfaction [12]. In a study of 27 adolescents with sickle cell disease, optimism moderated the relationship between use of pain medication and pain severity [11]. Specifically, an adaptive pattern of opioid use was present among those with medium and high, but not low, optimism.

Table 1 Commonly used measures for positive psychological traits and pain catastrophizing

Additionally, optimism has been linked with pain response among patients with temporomandibular disorder (TMD) [14]. In a case control study of 20 patients (TMD) and 28 pain-free controls, TMD patients with lower optimism had lower pain tolerance times and higher pain unpleasantness in an ischemic pain task following a stressor compared with controls and TMD patients with higher optimism. Furthermore, TMD patients with lower optimism had higher biomarkers of stress response [norepinephrine and interleukin-6 (IL-6)] during experimental stress compared with TMD patients with higher optimism. Interestingly, lower optimism was associated with higher IL-6 at baseline, indicating a possible susceptibility towards greater inflammatory stress response. A large (n = 5,696) cross-sectional study of TMD patients also found an inverse relationship between optimism and facial pain [13]. Importantly, the association was only present among patients without depression. In addition, studies have demonstrated that optimism is protective against pain when recovering from surgery, including inguinal hernia repair [15], arthroscopic knee surgery [16], and coronary artery bypass graft surgery [17].

Research with non-clinical samples has helped elucidate mechanisms by which optimism may protect against pain. Those higher in optimism were more responsive to a placebo expectation for analgesia, experiencing less pain in response to a cold pressor task than those in a no-expectation condition [18, 19]. It is speculated that those higher in optimism are more inclined to respond to positive placebo expectations [18]. Further, the placebo response characteristic of optimists is likely aided by lower state anxiety at subsequent painful events [19]. The consistent negative association between optimism and pain in many studies has led to concern that optimistic individuals may disengage from confronting health issues [20]. However, it has been demonstrated the healthy participants with higher optimism exhibit the expected pain response (less pain and cardiovascular reactivity) under typical circumstances, but that this pattern is eliminated when primed to think about health and wellness. The results of this study provide evidence that optimists do not “blindly” accept pain that warrants action, but that they shift into an approach-oriented coping mode. Finally, a healthy sample of 149 diverse participants demonstrated that the protective link between optimism and pain perception holds across ethnic groups [21].

Although the link between optimism and pain has received a majority of attention in the literature, some research exists on the link between hope and pain. Whereas optimism is a generalized expectancy for positive outcomes [8], hope taps goal-directed thinking and consists of pathways (perceived routes toward goals) and agency (motivation to pursue routes toward goals), as conceptualized by Snyder [22]. Among healthy participants, higher dispositional hope has been linked with higher pain threshold, longer pain tolerance, and lower pain perception in a cold pressor task [23]. Furthermore, healthy participants receiving an intervention designed to increase hope demonstrated longer pain tolerance than controls [24].

Hope has been most often studied among cancer patients. However, the relationship between hope and pain is not always as clear as with optimism and pain, which may stem from a greater variety of measures employing different operational definitions to measure hope (see Table 1). In some studies hope was comparable between those with and without pain [25, 26], but in others those with higher hope had fewer pain symptoms and fatigue [27], lower pain interference [25, 28] and higher meaning ascribed to pain [26]. Of note, in one study, those with cancer pain had lower hope than those without pain [28]. Pain intensity among cancer patients has been associated with hope in some (e.g., Hsu [28]) studies and not others (e.g., Lin [25]). Furthermore, hope was not associated with pain among patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain and it was speculated that this surprising results may have been owing to a restricted range of pain scores [29].

Additional research has examined the link between self-efficacy, pain, and related outcomes, such as functional impairment and quality of life [3033]. Self-efficacy is the belief that one has the ability to achieve a particular goal [34]. Self-efficacy beliefs for managing pain have been linked to pain and related outcomes among arthritis patients in numerous studies [3537]. Self-efficacy may explain gender differences often observed in clinical and experimental pain responding [38]. Jackson et al. [39] found that physical and task specific self-efficacy fully mediated the link between gender and pain perception in a cold pressor task. In addition, self-efficacy accounted for a substantial amount of variance in experimental pain tolerance between marathon runners and matched controls [40]. In another study, individuals with high self-efficacy who were provided a choice of coping strategies displayed increased tolerance of acute pain and lower pain reports [41]. Overall, the current research into the relationship between positive psychological traits and pain perception provides a comprehensive picture that optimism, hope, and self-efficacy positively influence well-being and health, and that flourishing may be protective against negative health outcomes.

Pain Catastrophizing and Pain Perception

In contrast to the way that positive psychological traits often buffer pain experience, certain cognitive factors (the way one thinks about painful experiences) can heighten pain perception. Pain catastrophizing is one such cognitive factor that is a negative amplification of pain-related thoughts through rumination (repetitive thoughts about pain), magnification (exaggerated concern about negative consequences of pain), and helplessness (believing nothing will change the pain) [42]. Pain catastrophizing has been linked with pain in hundreds of studies in varied patient populations [42, 43]. Indeed, the results are maintained after controlling for depression [44] and anxiety [45]. Likewise, pain catastrophizing has been the strongest predictor of pain among other related constructs such as fear and body vigilance [46]. Pain catastrophizing has explained observed ethnic [47] and gender [48] differences in pain perception. Moreover, age differences in pain catastrophizing appear to be based on type of pain, as well as whether pain characteristics are sensory or affective [49].

Important research established the direction of the relationship between catastrophizing and pain among healthy participants, such that catastrophizing precedes increased pain response [50]. Similarly, prospective studies have demonstrated that reducing pain catastrophizing brings about lower pain and disability [42, 51]. Critically, reductions in pain catastrophizing have been achieved through cognitive behavioral interventions [5254]. Experimental work has demonstrated that catastrophizing can be manipulated and that catastrophic thinking is linked with lower pain endurance compared with those employing positive coping self-statements [55].

Some work suggests a differential relationship between trait and state catastrophizing. Trait or general catastrophizing is assessed by asking how participants typically respond to pain, whereas state- or situation-specific catastrophizing is assessed by asking about pain response in a particular situation, such as during or following a specific experiment. In some cases, clinical and non-clinical participants differ in trait and state pain catastrophizing. Fibromyalgia (FMS) patients had greater trait catastrophizing than controls, but similar state in response to thumbnail pressure pain. Further, only in FMS patients was there a correlation between activation of the left posterior parietal cortex and state catastrophizing. This brain region is an integration center for somatosensory information [56]. Similarly, within some studies trait catastrophizing was stable across ethnicity in healthy participants, whereas some variation in situational catastrophizing exists [57]. Variation in research results across studies may stem from differences in the catastrophizing construct under examination (trait vs state). It is important that investigators be clear about what type of catastrophizing is being assessed to improve methodological consistency across the literature.

Some research has examined the possible mechanisms that explain the link between pain catastrophizing and pain perception. Higher catastrophizing during experimental pain is associated with lower activation of descending pain-inhibitory controls (DNIC), especially among women [58]. Interestingly, women also showed a lower DNIC response than men, which could help explain greater pain perception and more negative pain outcomes among women. It would be advantageous for future research to determine whether modifying pain catastrophizing affects DNIC processes [58].

A growing body of work has begun to assess the neural correlates of pain catastrophizing during the administration of noxious stimuli. Catastrophizing is linked with increased brain activity in regions associated with anticipation of pain [medial frontal cortex (MFC), cerebellum), attention to pain [dorsal anterior cingulate gyrus, rostral anterior cingulate cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPF)], emotional aspects of pain (claustrum, closely connected to amygdala) and motor activity [44, 59]. Furthermore, pain catastrophizing is associated with different patterns of cortical response depending on the intensity of the pain [59]. Specifically, during moderate as opposed to mild pain, pain catastrophizing is linked with lower activity in the DLPF and MFC—regions of the brain responsible for top-down pain suppression [59].

Among individuals with major depressive disorder and not in healthy controls the helplessness subscale of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale (see Table 1) was related to activation of the right amygdala during the anticipation of pain [60]. This brain region has been found to be associated with passive coping styles. Other recent research demonstrated that pain catastrophizing is related to phantom limb pain in upper limb amputees. Further, using electroencephalography, associations between pain catastrophizing and N/P135 dipole located in the area around the secondary somatosensory cortex were found. This area is predominately related to discriminative and affective-motivational aspects of human pain processing [61•]. In addition, temporal summation, a marker of central pain facilitation, has also been linked with pain catastrophizing [58]. This validates psychological approaches that seek to modify attentional focus, interpretation, and emotional processes surrounding pain as are often found in cognitive behavioral therapy [62].

Some research has demonstrated that positive psychological traits, such as self-efficacy, can also act as mediators between catastrophizing and pain and catastrophizing and pain-related outcomes [63, 64]. The belief in one’s ability to control pain fully mediated the link between pain catastrophizing and pain among osteoarthritis patients, while perceptions of ability for physical functions fully explained the relationship between pain catastrophizing and physical disability [64]. Indeed, the belief in one’s ability for dealing with emotional symptoms of arthritis partially explained the link between pain catastrophizing and psychological disability. In a similar study, beliefs about the ability to cope with arthritis symptoms partially explained the relationship between pain catastrophizing and physical functioning among osteoarthritis patients [63]. All of these studies highlight the importance of understanding pain catastrophizing, as it appears critical in determining pain experience. This promising body of research indicates that understanding whom, why, and when individual’s catastrophize, and that recognizing possible neural mechanisms involved will give a better insight into pain perception.

Positive Traits and Pain Catastrophizing

Ample research has linked pain catastrophizing with negative psychological experiences, such as depression, anxiety, and fear [44, 45, 6569]. Pain catastrophizing fully mediated the relationship between pain and emotional distress among 46 back pain outpatients [70]. Furthermore, pain catastrophizing has explained the link between pre-surgical anxiety and post-surgical pain [71]. Given the positive relationship between pain catastrophizing and negative psychological experiences it stands to reason that pain catastrophizing would be linked inversely with positive psychological qualities. Those with higher levels of positive traits such as optimism [72•, 73•], hope [73•, 74], and self-efficacy [63, 64, 75] are, in fact, less likely to engage in pain catastrophizing. Furthermore, positive emotions and resiliency are associated with lower pain catastrophizing [76].

The inverse relationship between positive traits and pain catastrophizing might be understood in the context of the link between positive traits and mental health. Positive traits such as hope [77] and self-efficacy [75] are associated inversely with negative psychological sequelae, such as depression. Individuals high in hope have been found to cope better with daily stress and negative emotions, whereas those low in hope have shown stronger stress reactions and poorer emotional recovery [78]. In predicting future outcomes, it has also been demonstrated that hope can act as a resiliency factor. Those higher in hope have lower future levels of depression and anxiety than those with low in hope [79]. Further, among patients with depression, those with higher levels of optimism experience better coronary artery bypass surgery treatment outcomes [80].

Individuals who possess higher levels of positive traits, such as hope or optimism, are more likely to experience higher levels of positive emotions. Hope and optimism are thinking processes about the pursuit of a goal, and higher levels can create “a sense of affective zest” [22]. Correspondingly, those who experience greater levels of positive emotions possess higher resilience, which equips them to confront difficult experiences, which could include pain [81, 82]. Experimental studies have demonstrated that positive emotions counteract negative emotions [82]. Participants underwent a negative emotion induction by preparing for a time-pressured speech. Then they were assigned randomly to films, which induced either positive, negative, or neutral emotions. Those who experienced positive emotions following the stressful task experienced faster cardiovascular recovery, suggesting the health benefits of positive emotions, which should, theoretically, extend to pain.

Link Between Positive Traits, Pain Catastrophizing, and Pain Perception

Although there is accumulating evidence about positive traits, pain catastrophizing, and pain perception, it was only recently that research established that the link between positive traits and pain perception operates through pain catastrophizing (see Fig. 1). The first study to demonstrate this link sampled a healthy community sample of 114 men and women [73•]. Both trait hope and optimism were associated inversely with pain response in a cold pressor task. All three dimensions of pain catastrophizing (rumination, magnification, and helplessness) partially mediated the link between hope and optimism with pain perception in independent models. Since then, this study has been replicated with a sample of 140 osteoarthritis patients and expanded upon by using a different pain stimulus (heat), a different measure of pain catastrophizing coping strategies questionnaire (CSQ), and temporal summation as an outcome measure, which reflects central pain facilitation [72•]. Similar to previous research, those with higher levels of optimism displayed lower temporal summation, which indicates less pain facilitation. In addition, pain catastrophizing was a significant mediator of the link between optimism and temporal summation.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Conceptual model of relationship between positive traits, pain catastrophizing, and pain perception

The consistent results in correlational analyses have led to an investigation into whether positive traits could be related causally to experimental pain. Hanssen et al. [83••] addressed this question by manipulating optimism experimentally. They demonstrated a causal link between optimism and pain perception. Healthy participants were assigned randomly to visualization and writing about a future best possible self to induce optimism, or visualization and writing about a typical day (control). Participants then completed the cold pressor task with a visual analog scale measure of pain perception taken at intervals throughout the task, as well as a post-measure. Those in the optimism condition consistently reported lower pain throughout the cold pressor task. Consistent with prior research, situational pain catastrophizing mediated the relationship between optimism and pain [83••]. This study demonstrates that optimism can not only be modified, but that doing so diminishes the experience of pain.

Practice Implications

It is recommended that psychological approaches to pain reduction include a focus on reducing pain catastrophizing and increasing thoughts, feelings, and behaviors associated with positive traits, such as hope, optimism, and self-efficacy. Cognitive behavioral interventions are the most widely used approaches for modifying pain catastrophizing and have proven effective in multiple studies [5255]. Psychological treatment often involves multiple sessions over several months to achieve sustained treatment outcomes [52]. Modifying catastrophizing may include activities such as examining automatic thoughts, restructuring unhelpful thoughts, planning, and positive-self-talk [53].

Briefer cognitive behavioral interventions have also proven effective for modifying pain catastrophizing and reducing experimental pain [54]. A manualized protocol included instruction in three strategies for reducing catastrophic thinking: distraction, mindfulness and acceptance, and cognitive restructuring. Researchers provided examples of distraction and mindfulness/acceptance. Cognitive restructuring was taught by an interactive discussion lasting approximately 5 mins of how to examine a thought and reframe it in a more helpful or realistic way. Then, participants practiced restructuring thoughts with assistance from a trained research assistant as needed. The entire intervention lasted approximately 10 mins and was effective in reducing pain catastrophizing, increasing pain tolerance, and reducing subjective pain report [54]. Given evidence that brief cognitive interventions may be associated with changes in future coping behaviors [84], it is necessary for prospective studies to try to determine the long-term effects of brief interventions.

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) [85] is another successful approach for the treatment of pain [86]. In contrast to traditional cognitive behavioral approaches, the focus of ACT is on committing to pursuing valued activities without trying to avoid or control pain. A goal is to improve psychological flexibility and reduce the effect of pain on functioning [87]. There is evidence that pain acceptance is linked with better adjustment among chronic pain patients [88] and that acceptance-based coping is a stronger predictor of adjustment than control-based coping [89]. Furthermore, the benefits of pain acceptance coping on activity have been demonstrated prospectively [90]. The link between pain intensity and fear about pain among patients with chronic pain was less strong among those with higher acceptance [91], suggesting a protective role of acceptance in the negative emotional sequelae of pain. Furthermore, patients who increase acceptance of pain show decreased pain anxiety, which is linked with better treatment outcomes [92].

Acceptance has been increased successfully through brief coping instructions in laboratory settings involving shifting attention and focus [93]. Furthermore, the teaching of acceptance and commitment through experiential activities and metaphors is associated with a variety of adaptive treatment outcomes [93]. Acceptance of pain and commitment to pursuing valued activities may be viewed a marker of resilience [94•]. Similarly, dispositional optimism is often viewed as a source of an individual’s resilience [94•], and the experience of positive emotions plays a significant role in promoting mental activities and fostering experiences that build resilience [82]. Given that both acceptance and positive emotions are conceptualized as forms of resilience, and that they both play a prominent role in pain perception and pain catastrophizing, it would be advantageous if future research and therapeutic interventions assessed these resilience mechanisms together.

Much research has shown that mood can be manipulated and that interventions targeted to increase positive traits translate to improved experimental pain outcomes. Positive mood inductions include activities such as showing clips from humorous films or brief stories that invoke positive feelings, such as joy, are associated with better pain tolerance and reduced pain perception [95, 96]. Advising patients to engage with media containing humorous or other positive emotional content could yield benefits in a variety of contexts. In an acute situation, such as prior to undergoing a medical procedure, a humorous video clip could be shown or an inspirational story could be read. In chronic situations, such as a prolonged illness or injury, a regimen containing “infusions” of positive emotions throughout the day in either regular or variable intervals could be implemented. Non-media-based approaches, such as journaling for increasing positive emotions, such as gratitude, have also proven effective [97, 98], and may be useful in situations where using technology is not feasible or appropriate.

Brief interventions targeting positive traits have also been effective in improving experimental pain outcomes among healthy participants. An intervention to increase hope consisted of a structured 16-min session. The session included: (i) guided imagery—instruction to think of a desired goal and build motivation and strategies to accomplish the goal, and considered how the experience might help in achieving future goals and dialogue; and (ii) a discussion of why the identified goal is important and verbalization of the material visualized in the previous step. Next, there was a strategies instruction, which provided information on how to increase goal-directed thinking, pathways thinking, and agency, with tips translating this general information for use on the cold pressor task. Finally, they completed a worksheet with instructions to write about another experience in pursuing goals, listing positive self-talk statements and strategies for the cold pressor task, and providing an estimate of expected pain tolerance time. This intervention was successful and individuals in the hope treatment condition experienced significantly longer tolerance in the cold pressor task than those in the control group [24]. Future work to test this protocol with pain patients is recommended.

A brief intervention to increase optimism using a best possible self-activity that included writing and visualization has also proven successful [83••]. Participants were instructed to think about their best possible self for 1 min, then to write about this topic for 15 mins, and, finally, to imagine the story they recorded vividly for 5 mins. This approach has been used successfully to increase optimism in previous studies [99, 100]. Those in the best possible self-condition experienced a change in expectations for future outcomes and reported correspondingly lower pain intensity during a cold pressor task compared with those in the control group [83••]. Extending this approach to clinical pain patients and observing long-term outcomes would be valuable.

Approaches to increase self-efficacy often provide education or coping skills [101]. It has been suggested that approaches for improving self-efficacy should increase their use of technology [101]. This is especially important for patients who work full-time or have other time restrictions, which may pose barriers to regular clinic visits over a sustained period of time [101]. Online dissemination of positive psychology exercises has proven feasible and effective [102].

Conclusion

The benefit of high levels of the positive personality traits optimism, hope, and self-efficacy has been demonstrated in clinical and healthy populations exposed to pain. Further, these traits provide a protective influence for pain perception. Conversely, certain cognitive factors (the way one thinks about painful experiences) can be maladaptive and negatively influence pain perception. Pain catastrophizing is a well-established risk factor for increased pain perception and there are numerous neurological studies that have revealed activity in brain regions associated with pain control and integration, which provides support for this relationship. Ample research has linked pain catastrophizing with negative psychological experiences, such as depression and anxiety. Foremost for this review, a corresponding, but smaller, body of research demonstrates the inverse relationship between pain catastrophizing and the positive traits of optimism, hope, and self-efficacy. Recent research has provided an integrated psychological model examining positive traits and pain catastrophizing together to understand pain perception. Specifically, lower levels of pain catastrophizing explain the inverse link between positive traits and pain perception. These basic science approaches answer fundamental questions that can be translated into evidence-based treatments. Practice implications for reducing pain perception include a focus on cognitive behavioral strategies for improving levels of hope, optimism, and self-efficacy, and reducing pain catastrophizing. It will be important for future research to determine whether there is a causal link between positive traits and pain catastrophizing. Understanding whether increasing positive traits reduces catastrophizing or whether reducing catastrophizing increases positive traits will provide direction in crafting psychological interventions to reduce the experience of pain.