Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a complex and common neurodevelopmental disorder. Recent estimates suggest that 1 in every 54 children is diagnosed with ASD by the age of 8 years old, increasing public awareness and concern to how society may best support these individuals and their families (Maenner et al., 2020). ASD is characterized by deficits in two primary domains, including impaired social communication and interaction, as well as restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests (APA, 2013). For individuals with ASD, difficulties in social communication often manifest as decreased social reciprocity and less engagement with peers (Humphrey & Symes, 2011). The complexities of the social world require continuous interpretation and response to situations appropriate to context, which may be especially difficult and stressful for older children and adolescents with ASD because of increased social demands and onset of puberty (Picci & Scherf, 2015). By examining physiological stress responses to social stressors, we may be able to elucidate how youth with ASD interact with their social world, as well as the extent to which this changes during adolescence.

The Autonomic Nervous System (ANS) plays an important role in physiological regulation and response to stress. The parasympathetic (PNS) and sympathetic (SNS) branches of the ANS function primarily in opposition—the PNS acts to conserve energy (‘rest and digest’), the SNS is responsible for threat mobilization (‘fight or flight’). The ANS has been proposed as a behavioral regulator, with the balance and pattern of responses between the PNS and the SNS critical to shaping response to changing environmental conditions (Berntson et al., 2008). The dynamic ability of the autonomic system to flexibly adapt to changing demands may be important for emotional processing and responding, while an inflexible, invariable system is often characteristic of pathological states, such as anxiety disorders (Friedman, 2007; Friedman & Thayer, 1998; Thayer et al., 1996, 2000).

The PNS and SNS both project to the sinoatrial (SA) node of the heart, influencing beat-to-beat variability in heart rhythm (heart rate variability; HRV). Parasympathetic regulation is frequently indexed using respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), which represents the high frequency heartbeat intervals that fluctuate with respiration (Berntson et al., 1993). RSA increases with greater output of the parasympathetically-mediated vagal nerve, and is sensitive to cholinergic but not adrenergic blockade, supporting its use as a sensitive marker of PNS functioning (Berntson et al., 1993; Cacioppo et al., 1994).

According to the Polyvagal Theory (Porges, 1995, 2001, 2003a, 2007), the default physiological state is one in which the parasympathetically-mediated vagus nerve maintains a ‘brake’ on the heart, slowing heart rate. In the presence of a stressor, however, vagal influence on the heart is said to withdraw, reducing cholinergic input to the SA node (Benarroch, 2012) and allowing for an adaptive increase in cardiac output without engaging the more metabolically demanding SNS (Porges, 2001, 2007; Wolff et al., 2012). For example, in a meta-analysis of 44 studies in children examining response to a stressor, greater parasympathetic withdrawal was associated with fewer externalizing and internalizing symptoms; however, clinical/at-risk groups (e.g., attention deficit/hyperactivity, social phobia, suicidal, etc.) displayed less decline in parasympathetic regulation in response to a stressor (Graziano & Derefinko, 2013). Further, young children who demonstrate increased vagal flexibility and changes in response to stressors also show diminished sympathetic activation to a number of stressful conditions when provided with social support (Wolff et al., 2012). The regulatory capacity of the vagal ‘brake’ may therefore have substantial long-term impacts on physical and psychological health, as chronic SNS hyperactivity is associated with a number of illnesses and psychopathologies, such as inflammation (e.g. Marvar & Harrison, 2012), depression (e.g. Gold, 2015; Thayer et al., 1998), and anxiety (e.g. Brosschot et al., 2006; Friedman, 2007; Thayer et al., 1996).

This vagal flexibility (Berntson et al., 2008) is also said to have an important role in regulating social behavior. The parasympathetically-mediated ‘social engagement system’ (Porges, 2001, 2003b) includes interconnected brainstem nuclei that regulate the myelinated vagus as well as cranial nerves directly involved in controlling muscles of the face and head. Calm, restful visceral states are said to promote these pro-social behaviors such as eye contact and language production; however, dysregulation of the vagal system and/or mobilization of the fight or flight system will block this social engagement system and prevent activation of the facial muscles (Porges, 2003a, b, 2007). Empirical support for the social engagement system includes evidence that individuals who demonstrate more social cooperation and engagement tend to have higher PNS regulation (Beffara et al., 2016; Kogan et al., 2014; Lischke et al., 2018). Additionally, young adults with higher vagal tone are found to be more socially engaged than their peers with lower PNS regulation (Geisler et al., 2013). Dysfunction within the vagal and social engagement system would therefore presumably lead to deficits in a number of socially adaptive behaviors, which may have significant implications for a number of clinical populations, including ASD.

Increasing evidence supports atypical autonomic regulation and arousal in individuals with ASD. A number of studies cite lower baseline parasympathetic regulation in ASD relative to individuals with typical development (TD) (e.g., Bal et al., 2010; Edmiston et al., 2016; Guy et al., 2014; Ming et al., 2005; Neuhaus et al., 2016; Vaughan Van Hecke et al., 2009). Young, preschool-aged children with ASD and higher resting PNS arousal have been reported to gesture more and engage in more sharing behavior (Patriquin et al., 2013). In contrast, in youth with ASD, those with lower baseline parasympathetic values demonstrate increasingly severe social symptoms (Edmiston et al., 2016; Vaughan Van Hecke et al., 2009). There is further evidence for differences in PNS stress reactivity or responsivity to various tasks. During a sensory challenge protocol, young children with ASD demonstrated significantly less change in RSA from rest to individual sensory stressors, providing support for reduced vagal flexibility in the ASD group (Schaaf et al., 2015). In response to an attention-demanding processing task, there further appears an atypical lack of vagal withdrawal in children ASD, who instead showed an increase in RSA to the task (Porges et al., 2013). In social contexts, school-aged youth with ASD have evidenced blunted RSA relative to TD peers during social interactions (Neuhaus et al., 2016; Vaughan Van Hecke et al., 2009), when higher RSA would be expected to promote the parasympathetically-mediated social engagement system (Porges, 2003b).

Despite the findings above, evidence for atypical autonomic functioning in ASD remains inconsistent and likely dependent on numerous influential factors, such as context, developmental influences, symptom severity, and more. Some studies have not found differences in parasympathetic (e.g., Kushki et al., 2014; Levine et al., 2012; Muscatello et al., 2020; Watson et al., 2012) or sympathetic (Edmiston et al., 2017b; Neuhaus et al., 2016; Schaaf et al., 2015) functioning at rest in youth with ASD. While some studies have examined parasympathetic regulation during various stressors and reported lower RSA across contexts (e.g. Edmiston et al., 2016; Guy et al., 2014), they have not reported a significant difference in parasympathetic reactivity to social evaluative stress between youth with and without ASD (Corbett et al., 2019a; Edmiston et al., 2016; Hollocks et al., 2014; Kushki et al., 2014; Sheinkopf et al., 2013). This suggests an important distinction between overall parasympathetic tone versus acute vagal flexibility and highlights the need to not only distinguish between the two, but also to further understand the potential implications of maladaptive states of each, especially for youth with ASD.

These inconsistencies within the ASD literature emphasize the need to examine associated factors which may influence physiological states and responses to stress. It is plausible that the type of stressor may contribute to the reported inconsistencies. Previous research examining another stress system, the hypothalamic pituitary axis (HPA) indexed by increases in salivary cortisol, has shown that situations that rely on social communication and interaction with peers often result in enhanced stress response in children with ASD (Corbett et al., 2010, 2014; Lopata et al., 2008; Schupp et al., 2013). Similarly, within the sympathetic branch of the ANS, there is evidence for increased sympathetic influence during interactions with a familiar partner in boys with ASD compared to TD (Neuhaus et al., 2016). However, other contexts such as social evaluation that trigger an adaptive stress response in TD youth, do not consistently promote a cortisol or sympathetic response in youth with ASD (e.g., Corbett et al., 2019a, 2021; Edmiston et al., 2017b; Lanni et al., 2012; Levine et al., 2012). These findings suggest that not all social contexts are equally salient for youth with ASD. Moreover, developmental factors, such as age and puberty, have contributed to differences between youth with ASD and with TD in the HPA axis (Corbett et al., 2010, 2021; Muscatello & Corbett, 2018; Schupp et al., 2013). Similar developmental effects might be present for the autonomic system but have not been explored.

It is also necessary to acknowledge the autonomic system may be influenced by physical health variables, such as body weight, height, and body mass index (BMI) (Masi et al., 2007; Molfino et al., 2009; Shibao, 2012; Thayer & Sternberg, 2006). Specifically, BMI, which is an index of the amount of body fat, can alter cardiac functioning and therefore impact ANS output. Youth with ASD have been reported to show elevated BMI relative to youth with TD (e.g., Corbett et al., 2020; Healy et al., 2019; Hill et al., 2015; McCoy & Morgan, 2020; Must et al., 2017).

Despite the potential influence of these recognized developmental (age, puberty) and physical (BMI) factors, research of autonomic functioning in school-age and adolescent youth with ASD has been inconsistent in controlling for many of these factors, which may be influencing the significant variability in findings. Previous research in TD populations have noted steadily increasing HRV throughout development (e.g., Eyre et al., 2014) but decreased PNS activity in obese children and adolescents (e.g., Eyre et al., 2014; Rabbia et al., 2003). Despite such findings, few studies in ASD have examined age (Harder et al., 2016; Kushki et al., 2014; Ming et al., 2005; Muscatello et al., 2021) or pubertal effects (Edmiston et al., 2016). To our knowledge, no studies have directly examined the effects of physical health variables such as BMI on ANS functioning in autism, and only a limited number of studies explicitly controlled for or matched diagnostic groups on BMI (Bricout et al., 2018; Harder et al., 2016; Muscatello et al., 2020).

The primary goal of the study was to examine parasympathetic regulation and response to a social evaluative threat paradigm in a large, well-characterized sample of youth, ages 10–13 years, with and without ASD. We sought to identify expected differences between groups in response to the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST; Kirschbaum et al., 1993). Further, in an effort to address inconsistencies in previous research, we evaluated factors expected to contribute to ANS response. Based upon the extant literature (e.g. Neuhaus et al., 2016; Thayer & Sternberg, 2006; Vaughan Van Hecke et al., 2009), it was hypothesized that: (1) youth with TD would demonstrate more parasympathetic regulation and vagal flexibility, with higher RSA on average as well as a larger decrease in RSA in response to the social evaluation, compared to youth with ASD who would show a blunted parasympathetic profile, (2) higher pubertal status and older age would predict higher resting RSA and greater change in RSA from baseline, and (3) higher BMI would predict lower RSA and a blunted slope in response to social evaluation in ASD and TD youth.

Methods

Participants

Data were collected as part of a longitudinal study on pubertal development and stress (Corbett, 2017). The current study includes data from Year 1 enrollment when the children were between 10-years-0-months and 13-years-11-months of age. The total enrolled sample included 241 youth with ASD (n = 138) or TD (n = 103). The ASD group comprised 102 males and 36 females, while the TD group included 57 males and 46 females. Demographic information for the full sample is presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics

All participants were required to have an intelligence quotient (IQ) score ≥ 70 as estimated by the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, Second Edition (WASI-II, (Wechsler, 2011)). Participants in the ASD group had a diagnosis of ASD based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual-5 (APA, 2013) and confirmed by: (1) a previous diagnosis by a psychologist, psychiatrist, or behavioral pediatrician with autism expertise; (2) current clinical judgment, and (3) corroborated by the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-2; Lord et al., 2012), a semi-structured interview-based instrument administered by research-reliable personnel. Additionally, TD participants were determined to have no other developmental delay or neurodevelopmental diagnosis and were required to score < 10 on the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ; Rutter et al., 2003), which is a parent-report questionnaire used to screen for ASD symptoms.

As the larger longitudinal study included examination of hormonal responses, exclusion criteria for both groups included current use of medications known to alter the Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal (HPA) axis [e.g., corticosteroids; see (Granger et al., 2009)] or HPG axis (e.g., growth hormone), or medical conditions known to impact pubertal development (e.g., Cushing’s Disease). Also, participants taking oral contraceptives, growth hormones, or nicotine all known to influence the HPA axis, were excluded (Foley & Kirschbaum, 2010; Kirschbaum et al., 1995). An estimated 42% of children with ASD have been reported to take at least one psychotropic medication (Mire et al., 2014), thus the current study did not include a completely medication-naïve sample to be more representative of the overall ASD population. In the ASD group, 65.2% of youth were taking at least one medication, while 17.5% of TD participants reported taking a daily medication.

The Vanderbilt Institutional Review Board (IRB) approved the study, which was carried out in accordance with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki). All parents/guardians provided informed written consent and youth participants gave verbal assent prior to inclusion in the study.

Procedures

Participation included two research visits to the University. On Visit 1, the diagnostic (ADOS-2) and cognitive measures (WASI-II) were administered. Parents completed the Social Responsiveness Scale, 2nd Edition (Constantino & Gruber, 2012), which covers several areas of social functioning symptoms, including social motivation, communication, awareness, and cognition, as well as repetitive behaviors and interests. Additionally, participants completed a brief physical examination, as described below. On Visit 2, participants were exposed to the stress paradigm, the TSST-Child Version (Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 1997; Kirschbaum et al., 1993).

Physical Examination (PE)

Participants completed a physical examination, conducted by trained, licensed, study physicians to assess pubertal development, height, and weight. Recent research with the sample comparing physical exam to parent- and self-report demonstrated that physical exam is the optimal approach for more precise pubertal measurement (Corbett et al., 2019b). Therefore, physical exam scores were used in the current study.

The PE was completed to reliably identify pubertal development and assign Tanner stage (Marshall & Tanner, 1969, 1970). The exam ascertained two measures with five stages for Genitals (G1-G5 for boys) or Breasts (B1-B5 for girls; GB stage) and Pubic hair (P1-P5 for both genders; PH stage). The exam, as described previously by Corbett et al., 2019b, consisted of visual inspection and categorization of pubertal and genital maturation. To be consistent with the original Tanner staging and to maximize participation, palpation of breasts or measurement of testes was not conducted.

Height and weight were measured once using a calibrated stand-on Health-o-meter TM Professional 499KL Waist High Digital Scale with Height Rod (Hogentogler & Co., MD, USA). Height was measured to the nearest inch and weight was measured to the nearest 0.1 lb. Children were weighed and measured in light clothing. BMI was calculated using the standard formula (lb/in2) × 703 for use with the Center for Disease Control (CDC) growth charts for children and adolescents (2 through 19 years; https://www.cdc.gov/healthyweight/bmi/calculator.html) in order to convert to percentiles for statistical analyses.

Social Stress Paradigm

The TSST-Child Version (Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 1997; Kirschbaum et al., 1993) is a well-validated, experimentally induced psychosocial stressor known to reliably activate a physiological stress response in TD populations (Kudielka et al., 2004) including children and adolescents (Seddon et al., 2020). The TSST is a 20-min task divided into four subcomponents: (1) Intro/Preparation, (2) Present Speech, (3) Serial Subtraction, and (4) Debriefing/Recovery. The protocol involves a scenario in which the participant must deliver the ending to a short story in front of a panel of judges (unresponsive judges showing neutral facial expressions) who will, purportedly, be judging the child’s performance against that of other children. For the protocol, mixed-age (adult and peer), as well as mixed-sex judges were used since this contributes to a stronger effect than using female judges only (Seddon et al., 2020). The 5-min speech task is followed by a 5-min serial subtraction task. Of the 241 enrolled participants, 212 completed the full TSST protocol, with 29 participants failing to return for the second visit. There was no difference in number of youth with ASD (n = 17) or TD (n = 12) who did not return for Visit 2 [χ2(1) = 0.02, p = 0.87].

Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia (RSA)

RSA was collected using MindWare Mobile Impedance Cardiograph units (MindWare Technologies LTD, Gahanna, OH) for synchronized electrocardiography (ECG) and respiration data collection using a seven-electrode configuration. Participants were told they would be wearing ‘stickers’ throughout the protocol. Following an explanation of the electrode locations on the torso, participants were given the opportunity to place an electrode on their hand prior to placement. After placement, there was a five-minute acclimation period before initiating the baseline collection protocol (explained below). Of the 212 participants that completed the stress paradigm, four participants with ASD did not have HRV data due to an inability to tolerate the sensory aspects of the protocol. An additional three participants were missing RSA data due to use of anticholinergic medication (1 ASD), atypically low RSA values (1 ASD), or atypical heart rhythms (1 TD).

Baseline RSA was acquired using a 5-min resting collection period in which participants were instructed to sit quietly without engaging in other tasks. Following the baseline period, participants were taken to a separate room for the TSST protocol, at which point they received instructions for the task from the two judges. The participants were escorted back to the first assessment room, where they were given five minutes to prepare their speech. RSA collection started immediately prior to the preparation period (Prep) and was collected continuously throughout the TSST paradigm, calculated on a minute-by-minute basis and averaged into five-minute epochs for each major period of the paradigm- Baseline, Prep, Speech, Math, and Recovery (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Timeline of TSST procedure

RSA was derived in accordance with guidelines set forth by the Society for Psychophysiological Research committee on heart rate variability (Electrophysiology, 1996). ECG signal was sampled at 500 Hz and analyzed using the Heart Rate Variability Software Suite provided by MindWare Technologies (MindWare Technologies LTD, Gahanna, OH). RSA was quantified as the integral power within the respiratory frequency band (0.12–0.40 Hz), and respiration was monitored by impedance cardiography. The respiration signal was displayed to ensure that values were within the designated frequency band. Respiratory frequency was confirmed to lie within the high frequency/RSA band (0.12–0.40 Hz) for all participants. Of the total processed data, 3.5% were excluded due to excessive motion artifact, equipment error, or cardiac arrhythmias. RSA was measured in ms2.

Statistical Analysis

All hypotheses were tested using linear mixed effects models with a random intercept for subject to account for correlation within subjects. To test the hypothesis that youth with TD would demonstrate higher RSA levels on average, we fit a baseline model to predict RSA levels adjusted for sex, diagnosis and a covariate indicating timepoint within the TSST protocol. To test whether there was a larger decrease in RSA in response to the social evaluation in TD youth, an interaction term between diagnosis and timepoint was included. We tested the second and third hypotheses by adding age, PH stage, BMI, and their interactions with TSST time point to the model. To test the second hypothesis that higher pubertal status and older age would be associated with higher resting RSA, age, PH stage and their interactions with time were tested separately. Finally, to test the third hypothesis that higher BMI would predict lower RSA, we tested BMI and the interaction between BMI and time. The model was repeated using GB stage as well. For all models, we performed Wald F-tests using Type 2 sum of squares in order to test the main effects prior to including the interaction effects. All analyses were performed using R version 4.0.3. R packages used are listed in Supplementary Materials.

Results

Table 1 presents demographic and clinical characteristics of participants including age, sex, race, medication use, IQ, ADOS, BMI, Pubic Hair (PH) stage, Genital/Breast (GB) stage, and RSA across the TSST. The median age was similar for both groups—11.67 for TD youth and 11.25 for ASD youth. Median BMI percentile, which accounts for age and sex, was much lower in TD youth (52.5) compared to ASD youth (71). A majority of participants were in Stage 1 or 2 of PH and GB stages. The distribution of the sample included 7.9% African American, 83.0% Caucasian, 0.4% Asian, and 8.7% Mixed. Results of RSA levels at each stage are presented in Table 1.

Results from Linear Effects Modeling

Baseline Model

In order to test the first hypothesis, we fit a model including sex, diagnosis, timepoint, and the interaction between diagnosis and timepoint to investigate the effect of diagnosis on RSA response to the TSST (Table 2). In this model, RSA varies significantly across time (p < 0.0001) and is different between groups. Specifically, lower RSA levels, consistent with decreased PNS regulation, are associated with ASD diagnosis (p = 0.022) (Fig. 2). However, there was no evidence that the pattern of change in RSA levels across time (PNS responsivity) are different between TD and ASD groups (diagnosis by timepoint interaction: p = 0.32). Both groups display similar patterns of increased RSA during the Prep and Recovery stages and decreased RSA in the Speech and Math stages. See Supplementary Table S1 for parameter estimates.

Table 2 Type 2 sum of squares ANOVA table to test main effects and interaction for diagnosis and time (TSST)
Fig. 2
figure 2

Estimated RSA during TSST by diagnosis averaged across age and sex. Youth with ASD demonstrated significantly lower RSA on average, while the pattern of RSA response over time appeared similar

Models with Age, Puberty, and BMI

To test the second hypothesis that there are associations between development and RSA, we added age and PH stage and their interaction terms with time. Age and PH stage did not contribute substantially over the effects of the other covariates and the effect of diagnosis was reduced (p = 0.11). There were no interaction effects between age and time (p = 0.073) and PH stage and time (p = 0.52). Therefore, we did not find sufficient evidence for an association between age and PH stage with RSA values (Table 3).

Table 3 Type 2 sum of squares ANOVA table to test main effects and interactions of Age, BMI, and PH stage with TSST timepoint

To test the third hypothesis that higher BMI would be associated with lower RSA overall and would result in less change in RSA relative to baseline, we included an interaction between BMI and time in the model (Table 3; Supplementary Table S2 for parameter estimates). In contrast to the lack of associations for age and PH stage, results showed that higher BMI was strongly associated with lower RSA levels (p = 0.040), but there was no interaction effect between BMI and time (p = 0.68). Figure 3 shows the plot of RSA levels across stages for various BMI, demonstrating youth with elevated BMI tended to show lower overall RSA values.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Estimated RSA during TSST across BMI Percentile. Plot depicts model results of BMI effect on RSA, averaged across diagnosis, sex, age, and pubertal stage. Elevated Percentile BMI is associated with lower average RSA at every time point of the TSST

To investigate whether results differed based on type of Tanner stage, we fit this same model with GB stage and found similar results (see Supplementary Tables S3 and S4). There were no interaction effects between age and time, GB stage and time, and BMI and time, though BMI was predictive of RSA levels (p = 0.029). Similar to the model fitting PH stage, diagnosis was not predictive of RSA.

Preliminary Post-Hoc Analysis of Social Communication Symptoms

The lack of diagnostic group differences suggests a need to consider individual traits and behaviors and their relation to autonomic functioning. Exploratory analyses were conducted to investigate the extent to which social communication symptoms, as measured by the SRS-2 (Constantino & Gruber, 2012), were associated with differences in RSA, regardless of diagnosis. A linear model including the main effects of PH stage, BMI, time, and SRS Total T score tested the association of individual physical and behavioral characteristics with RSA response to the TSST (Table 4). Significant effects were observed with SRS scores (p = 0.025) and with BMI (p = 0.049), where RSA levels decreased with increasing BMI and with more social symptoms (elevated SRS T score) (Fig. 4). Similar findings were observed in a model with GB Stage (Supplemental Table S5).

Table 4 Type 2 sum of squares ANOVA table to test main effects of BMI, PH stage, and SRS total scores
Fig. 4
figure 4

Estimated RSA during TSST across SRS Total T Scores. Plot depicts model results averaged across sex, PH stage, age, and BMI percentile. SRS scores have been chosen as the following: 10th percentile (43), 25th percentile (47), 50th percentile (64), 75th percentile (78) and 90th percentile (86). More severe social symptoms (high SRS score) are associated with lower RSA at every timepoint of the TSST

Discussion

The current study sought to examine parasympathetic response to the TSST in youth with and without ASD, predicting differences between youth with ASD and with TD at rest and in response to the TSST. Specifically, we hypothesized that youth with ASD would demonstrate an atypical parasympathetic response, illustrated by lower resting baseline RSA and blunted reactivity to social evaluation. Factors that may influence ANS response were considered, including age, puberty, and BMI, as we expected them to be associated with parasympathetic regulation and response.

The first model showed lower RSA in the ASD group compared to the TD group, supporting the hypothesis that youth with ASD would demonstrate reduced parasympathetic regulation. However, there was no diagnosis by timepoint interaction, suggesting groups did not differ in RSA reactivity and therefore, the first hypothesis was only partially supported. Both groups demonstrated significant changes in RSA in response to the TSST, including an initial increase in PNS regulation during the prep period, followed by the expected decrease in RSA in response to the TSST speech and math tasks. Importantly, both groups did not differ in the change in slopes over time, suggesting the groups display similar patterns of change and vagal flexibility during a social evaluative stress paradigm, though the youth with ASD experience slightly decreased parasympathetic regulation on average.

The reduced parasympathetic regulation in ASD youth suggests they may be in a state of heightened arousal with less influence from the PNS, which promotes calmer, relaxed states. The total extent to which elevated arousal may affect social or psychological functioning in ASD is unknown. Low vagal tone has been associated with increased incidence of anxiety (e.g., Beauchaine, 2015; Friedman & Thayer, 1998; Kushki et al., 2013; Thayer et al., 1996) as well as decreased social skills (e.g., Patriquin et al., 2019; Porges, 2007). In the current study, social symptoms often characteristic of ASD were correlated with lower RSA regulation, regardless of diagnosis. While we did not evaluate potential comorbid symptoms in this sample of youth, identifying physiological differences in a sample as young as 10–13 years of age suggest an important need to examine the relationship between autonomic and socioemotional functioning in ASD, especially as these youth transition through the adolescent period.

Interestingly, despite apparent differences in the overall parasympathetic regulation within this sample, no differences in reactivity were observed. This distinction may be an important one, whereas baseline vagal activity is suggested to be linked to temperamental states, but RSA reactivity may be linked to attention and engagement with the environment, as well as emotional regulation or mood stability (Beauchaine, 2001). As reviewed by Beauchaine (2001), depression and anxiety, among other emotional traits, are more commonly related to baseline parasympathetic tone, while emotional states, such as panic or anger, are related to atypical vagal flexibility or withdrawal. Therefore, in the context of our findings, youth with ASD may be more susceptible to symptoms of anxiety and depression even though attention and emotional processes involved in the TSST response do not significantly differ relative to youth with TD.

It is important to note that the current study stands in contrast to previous findings in other stress systems, such as the HPA axis and sympathetic nervous system. It has previously been shown that youth with ASD actually demonstrate less cortisol release or sympathetic activation (i.e., lower stress response) to the TSST relative to peers with TD, suggesting they do not perceive the paradigm to be particularly stressful (Corbett et al., 2019a, 2021; Lanni et al., 2012). Indeed, recent research has shown that children with ASD who demonstrate blunted cortisol response to the TSST also tend to more frequently mis-identify neutral faces (Corbett et al., 2019a). Further, more severe social symptoms characteristic of ASD are related to a blunted sympathetic response (Edmiston et al., 2017b). Impaired ability to recognize facial affect and other social cues may limit a child’s ability to identify the situation as threatening and evaluative. Given this, it was hypothesized that a similar pattern of responses would be observed in the current sample for parasympathetic regulation, with a blunted RSA withdrawal response to the stressor.

The lack of similar findings within the PNS could be due to several factors, most notable of which is the distinct roles these two systems play in regulating the body’s physiological stress response. The parasympathetic system and HPA axis have largely opposing functions within the body, where the HPA axis is tightly coupled to the sympathoexcitatory circuits of the ANS. The vagus nerve may also be involved in HPA axis regulation, exerting inhibitory control over cortisol release (Thayer & Sternberg, 2006; Thayer et al., 2006). Similarly, the sympathetic system, much like the HPA axis, tends to promote more metabolically-demanding stress responses and is inhibited by the vagal system (e.g., Thayer & Brosschot, 2005; Thayer & Lane, 2009; Ulrich-Lai & Herman, 2009). The sympathetic branch of the ANS can be described as mediating ‘fight or flight’ physiological responses, and thus may be more susceptible to real or perceived threats. Indeed, there is some evidence for atypical, possibly maladaptive sympathetic responses to stress in ASD (Edmiston et al., 2017b; Neuhaus et al., 2016; Schaaf et al., 2015). There exists a tight coupling between the branches of the ANS, though a simple, reciprocal relationship cannot be assumed (Berntson et al., 2008). Therefore, future research should investigate stress response across the PNS, SNS, and HPA axis to elucidate a more thorough and complete picture of the physiological profile in youth with ASD (Muscatello et al., 2020, 2021) and the extent to which responses of these systems relate to observed behaviors and emotional states.

It is further acknowledged that the chosen stress condition may not have been optimal to measure RSA reactivity. Differences in social context have revealed dissociations in stress response in youth with ASD between social evaluation (TSST) and social interaction (Corbett et al., 2012; Edmiston et al., 2017a; Lanni et al., 2012), such that social evaluative threat did not mount a significant stress response, whereas a friendly, social interaction resulted in elevated stress for youth with ASD. Similarly, the extent to which the TSST is an appropriate paradigm to look for possible underlying parasympathetic differences must be considered. In other words, the TSST may not activate the parasympathetically-mediated ‘social engagement system’ (Porges, 2001, 2003b) and therefore group differences would not be present. There is limited reciprocal communication in the TSST, as the raters are instructed not to provide feedback. Therefore, the task is more a ‘performance’ than a ‘reciprocal conversation.’ It may be the case that a task that promotes social cooperation and engagement would be more likely to lead to differences in PNS regulation and reactivity (Beffara et al., 2016; Kogan et al., 2014; Lischke et al., 2018; Muscatello et al., 2021). Additionally, findings of an association between SRS scores and RSA further suggest social functioning, communication, and engagement may play a critical role in regulating parasympathetic responses. The role of context may underlie the lack of difference in typical or atypical response depending upon the stressor, and future research should examine the extent to which different social scenarios activate the parasympathetic response.

Extending beyond diagnostic differences in parasympathetic response, we examined the hypotheses that age, puberty, and BMI would be related to RSA regulation and response. Concerning the hypothesized developmental effects, neither age nor pubertal stage (either GB or PH stage) were significant predictors of RSA. While previous evidence supports developmental effects on the HPA axis stress response in youth with ASD (Corbett et al., 2021; Muscatello & Corbett, 2018; Schupp et al., 2013), the extant research is more limited for autonomic response and development in ASD. Nevertheless, evidence within typically developing populations suggests age, and to a more limited extent, puberty, may influence parasympathetic response. For example, in a study of children ages 8–15 years, younger children demonstrated significantly larger decreases in RSA relative to baseline compared to older children during a cognitive tracing task (El-Sheikh, 2005). Notably, no group differences were observed during a social stressor task involving listening to an argument, which may be more closely aligned to the social components of the TSST paradigm. Trends in previous literature suggest resting parasympathetic regulation tends to increase through childhood (e.g. Gentzler et al., 2012; Hinnant et al., 2011; Michels et al., 2013), though there may be substantial variability in these trends by late childhood and early adolescence (Gentzler et al., 2012). Similarly, some limited relationships between pubertal status and RSA response have been observed, where higher stages of biological maturity are associated with lower levels of RSA suppression during a cognitive stressor (El-Sheikh, 2005). Though empirical evidence of pubertal effects on the parasympathetic system is minimal, biological maturity remains an important variable to consider in studies of RSA regulation and reactivity (Allen & Matthews, 1997; Hinnant et al., 2018). Notably, the majority of the sample was in Tanner stages 1 and 2 (73% TD and 72% ASD), and therefore it may be the case that as the youth mature over the course of the 4-year longitudinal study, pubertal and age effects may begin to emerge within the increased variability.

In contrast, the hypothesis that BMI would predict parasympathetic regulation was supported, as youth with elevated BMI also demonstrated significantly lower RSA on average. This finding is consistent with previous research showing a relationship between BMI and autonomic functioning (e.g., Eyre et al., 2014; Landsberg, 1986; Rabbia et al., 2003), including observations that these relationships are present not only in high-risk, obese patients, but also in non-obese, healthy individuals (Molfino et al., 2009), suggesting a close relationship between body mass and physiology. Further emphasizing this point, adult patients that lost at least 10% of their body weight demonstrated significant gains in parasympathetic control, along with decreases in the sympathetic system (Arone et al., 1995). Therefore, high parasympathetic regulation is generally considered to be a positive marker of health status and reduced risk for a variety of health conditions (Masi et al., 2007; Thayer & Sternberg, 2006). Similarly, decreasing BMI through weight loss reduces risk for a variety of health conditions such as type 2 diabetes or high blood pressure (Vidal, 2002).

Childhood obesity remains a significant public health concern in the United States, and rates of overweight and obesity are particularly high in ASD, with estimates ranging from 18.0 to 42.0% overweight and 10.0–30.4% obese (Corbett et al., 2020; Criado et al., 2018; Curtin et al., 2010, 2014; Whiteley et al., 2004; Zuckerman et al., 2014). Prevalence of elevated BMI ostensibly rise in youth with ASD as they enter the adolescent period, where recent studies have noted increases in BMI as youth with ASD advance through puberty (Corbett et al., 2020). There may be a variety of factors which contribute to this prevalence of overweight and obesity in ASD, with medication side effects (e.g., Curtin et al., 2014) or reduced physical activity (such as from peer isolation) (e.g., Must et al., 2014) being just a few contributing factors. No matter the reason, the physical and psychological consequences cannot be overstated, with an increased risk for several health conditions (e.g., type 2 diabetes, hypertension) (e.g., Bray, 2004), as well as elevated incidence of depression, low self-esteem, and peer victimization (see Rankin et al., 2016 for review) in those with overweight/obesity.

Finally, it should be noted that in the larger model including BMI, as well as puberty and age, there was no longer a significant main effect for diagnosis. In a recent report with the current sample, significant differences in BMI were observed such that adolescents with ASD showed significantly greater BMI on average compared to their peers with typical development (Corbett et al., 2020). It is likely these BMI differences also contributed to RSA regulation and response to the TSST, as demonstrated in Fig. 3, where youth with higher BMI had lower RSA regardless of diagnosis.

The use of physiological markers like RSA has become increasingly common, with studies investigating the feasibility of using RSA and heart rate variability to index emotional stress (Beauchaine, 2015), anxiety (Thayer et al., 1996), and depression (Rottenberg, 2007; Rottenberg et al., 2007). However, this technique is not without its limitations. There are a number of variables which may impact the quantification and interpretation of HRV, including, but not limited to, respiratory rates, physical activity or movement, and posture (see Berntson et al., 1997; Grossman & D’Augelli, 2007 for review). For example, ECG abnormalities and abnormal beats can alter the validity of RSA (Berntson et al., 1997), and obesity and extremes in BMI have been associated with these ECG abnormalities (Fraley et al., 2005). The findings from the current study reveal that BMI is a significant driver of RSA response and regulation. Within a sample that significantly differs in overweight/obesity status (Corbett et al., 2020), the contributions of BMI may be driving many of the observed diagnostic effects reported in initial models and therefore highlight the need to control for such covariates in future analyses given the possible effect on findings. Moreover, additional analyses considering social functioning, but not diagnosis, showed that those with more severe social symptoms, which are frequently associated with ASD, demonstrated significantly lower RSA on average. Given the notable heterogeneity of the autism diagnosis, future research should focus more on unique traits and characteristics to determine whether individuals with ASD and certain physical and behavioral profiles are more likely to experience atypical autonomic functioning.

Clinical implications

The nature of parasympathetic dysfunction and its relation to stress responses and social functioning in youth with ASD remains difficult to clearly elucidate. Although several of the hypotheses in the current study were not confirmed, an important finding is that BMI was a key predictor in the current results and potentially previous findings. While scientifically interesting, it is clinically concerning that BMI plays such a prominent role in physiological regulation and RSA. Regarding physical health, autonomic imbalance and reduced parasympathetic regulation increase risk for a number of cardiovascular diseases (see Thayer et al., 2010 for review). On the flip side, it has been shown that when individuals lose weight, there is a normalization of RSA response as well as many other encouraging health outcomes, providing support for the use of RSA as an index of poor health status. There is an emerging and alarming literature showing higher rates of overweight and obesity in youth with ASD (e.g., Corbett et al., 2020; Criado et al., 2018), yet there is little-to-no research investigating the extent to which BMI differences may be contributing to observed autonomic dysfunction in some with ASD. Due to the link between BMI and physiological regulation, it is plausible that normalization of body weight may also normalize RSA response, and possibly, further lead to improvements in other physical and mental health symptoms associated with parasympathetic dysregulation in ASD. Considerably more research is required in order to examine these hypotheses and to further understand the link between BMI, physiological regulation, and other diagnostic and health status markers in individuals with ASD.

Limitations and Future Directions

The study was strengthened by the relatively large, well-characterized sample of youth with and without ASD. Additionally, the use of physical exam by a medically-licensed study team member, as opposed to self- or parent-report, ensured accurate reporting of Tanner stage and body mass index. Nevertheless, there are study limitations that should be acknowledged. First, the sample included a relatively narrow age range of 10–13-year-olds, with most youth still in the earlier stages of puberty (Tanner stages 1 and 2). Thus, there may not have been enough variability in the sample to detect development effects. The sample is part of a four-year longitudinal study and follow-up analyses in future years may reveal the influence of age and puberty on physiological response. Second, the study did not enroll youth with ASD with accompanying intellectual impairment, and participants were predominantly White; thereby, results are not fully representative and generalizable to the larger population. Future research including a more racially and ethnically diverse sample of youth with and without intellectual impairment will be important in order to better understand the role of RSA in the broad spectrum of ASD. Lack of diagnostic group-based differences suggest a need to examine individual characteristics and differences in behaviors. While preliminary findings suggest social functioning is related to lower parasympathetic functioning, regardless of diagnosis, further research is needed to identify traits and behaviors which may leave one susceptible to atypical autonomic response. Finally, results of the current study emphasize the need to consider potential covariates when elucidating autonomic regulation and response. Consideration of other confounds such as posture, activity, and respiration, may more clearly elucidate the utility of RSA as a biomarker of stress or psychiatric comorbidities in youth with ASD while controlling for extraneous variables.

Conclusion

The current study demonstrated that when accounting for BMI differences among youth with and without ASD, there is no evidence of atypical parasympathetic reactivity to social evaluation in ASD youth. Physical health, such as BMI, influences resting autonomic regulation, while pubertal effects may become more apparent in later adolescence with more advanced physical maturation. Due to the impact of numerous covariates, diagnostic differences in autonomic functioning should be considered under multiple contexts (e.g., social evaluation vs. social interaction) while accounting for other contributing variables. Considering the potential implications with social functioning, cognition, or psychiatric comorbidities, a consistent and rigorous methodological approach is necessary to identifying the utility of RSA as a biomarker of these symptoms in ASD.