Introduction

Many species of amphibians worldwide, more than other taxa, are experiencing population declines, local extinctions, and even global extinctions (Alford & Richards, 1999). One taxonomic group that may benefit from these amphibian population declines and extinctions are mosquitoes. Surprisingly few studies have considered the roles of amphibians in affecting mosquitoes. Yet, these limited studies demonstrate that anuran larvae may be strong competitors (Blaustein & Margalit, 1994, 1996; Mokany & Shine, 2003; Stav et al., 2010) and sometimes predators (Blaustein & Margalit, 1996; Blum et al., 1997) of mosquito larvae, while urodele larvae, known to drastically structure communities via predation (Morin, 1980; Blaustein et al., 1996; Benoy, 2008), may be important predators of mosquito larvae as well (Brodman & Dorton, 2006; DuRant & Hopkins, 2008; Rubbo et al., 2011; Reinhardt et al., 2013).

Amphibians may also influence oviposition habitat selection (OHS) by mosquitoes. Natural selection should favor the ability of gravid females to assess habitat quality for their progeny. In temporary pools, habitat quality can be defined in significant part by predation pressure (e.g., Morin et al., 1983; Wellborn et al., 1996). Future risk of predation to progeny in temporary pool habitats, if detectable, can be fairly predictable to a gravid female searching for an oviposition site because most predatory stages of aquatic predators are confined to the pool into which they were deposited by their mother (Blaustein, 1999). Indeed, there is a growing body of literature that is demonstrating that many pond organisms with complex life cycles including mosquitoes (reviewed in Vonesh & Blaustein, 2010), other aquatic insects (e.g., Resetarits, 2001), and amphibians (e.g., Binckley & Resetarits, 2008) are able to assess water bodies for risk of predation to their progeny, and choose oviposition sites accordingly. OHS by mosquitoes in response to risk of predation by urodeles has very rarely been assessed (Rubbo et al., 2011).

Experiments that demonstrate that there are fewer prey eggs observed in pools containing predators have not necessarily demonstrated that the mechanism for this observed distribution is OHS in response to predation risk; when the predator is not caged, a plausible explanation for such a pattern is that the prey females are ovipositing randomly with regards to among-pool predator distributions, but that the predators are preying upon the eggs, leaving fewer eggs for the researcher to observe and count. Few studies have considered this possibility, yet egg predation may be prevalent across prey species and predator species in temporary pools (Vonesh, 2005). Ecologists may try to rule out this possibility by including a caged-predator treatment, where presumably, the predator risk cues, in particular, predator-released kairomones, are readily detectable by prey females outside the cage in the pool but the predators cannot eat the eggs (e.g., Blaustein et al., 2004). Alternatively, experimental designs may employ predator-conditioned water but without the predator itself (Munga et al., 2006; Silberbush et al., 2010). However, an experimental design demonstrating no reduction in a caged-predator or predator-conditioned water treatment but a reduction in a free-roaming predator treatment does not necessarily indicate by default, egg predation: prey females may still detect cues of predation risk in free-roaming predator treatments and not in caged-predator or predator-conditioned water treatments by visual or tactile cues (Devereaux & Mokany, 2006). Mosquito egg raft predation or egg raft disruption has rarely been tested, yet it has been demonstrated for backswimmers (Chesson, 1984) and odonates (Stav et al., 1999).

Of interest in this paper are the potential effects of the larval fire salamander, Salamandra infraimmaculata Martens, on egg raft distributions of the mosquito, Culiseta longiareolata Macquart. This ubiquitous and abundant mosquito is likely found along the entire range of S. infraimmaculata and over much of the range of other Salamandra species (Margalit et al., 1988; Roiz et al., 2007; Becker & Hoffmann, 2011). S. infraimmaculata is considered locally endangered in Israel (Dolev & Perevolotsky, 2004). Their larvae are keystone, size-selective predators in temporary and permanent pools (Blaustein et al., 1996; Eitam et al., 2005), drastically reducing amphibian and large macroinvertebrate densities, including those of C. longiareolata (Segev & Blaustein, 2007; Blaustein, unpublished data). C. longiareolata oviposit egg rafts on the water surface between dusk and dawn. C. longiareolata larvae, relative to other mosquito species, are highly vulnerable to a wide variety of predators (Blaustein, 1998; Roberts, 2012). The females of this species have been shown to be able to chemically detect and avoid various backswimmer (Notonectidae) species (Eitam et al., 2002; Silberbush et al., 2010) when ovipositing, and to avoid Anax imperator (Aeshnidae: Anisoptera) larvae when ovipositing, apparently by means other than chemical cues (Stav et al., 1999, 2000). C. longiareolata egg rafts have also been shown to be vulnerable to predation by A. imperator (Stav et al., 1999).

The risk of predation by amphibians may depend on the developmental stage of the amphibian present in the pool. Many, but not all, amphibian species cease to feed when metamorphosing (Wells, 2007). This has never been explicitly tested for larval Salamandra. If Salamandra larvae, which are voracious predators for 2 months or more prior to metamorphosis (Degani, 1996; Eitam et al., 2005), cease to predate at some point in the metamorphic process, they would still be present in the water but nonlethal to prey for between several days and several weeks while completing metamorphosis. For some aquatic prey species, detecting predation risk comes in the form of detecting cues from consumed or injured prey (e.g., Laurila et al., 1997; Mogali et al., 2011). If C. longiareolata are capable of detecting risk of predation from chemical cues resulting from ingested prey, then these cues may not exist for metamorphosing larvae. Thus, if C. longiareolata avoids ovipositing in response to cues left from consumed larvae, they will not avoid the non-feeding metamorphosing larvae when ovipositing.

Here, we first conducted experiments to compare predation rates on C. longiareolata larvae and egg rafts by metamorphosing S. infraimmaculata larvae and similarly sized pre-metamorphosing larvae. We then conducted experiments to determine whether among-pool egg raft distributions were affected by either caged or free-roaming Salamandra larvae and if so, whether this distribution was affected by different developmental states—i.e., pre-metamorphosing versus metamorphosing larvae. These experiments, all combined, allowed us to assess whether any among-treatment differences in distribution in egg rafts could be attributed to egg raft predation and/or to OHS.

Methods

Study animals

Salamandra infraimmaculata larvae used in these experiments were collected from breeding sites in the Galilee Mountains, Israel. Both pre-metamorphosing and metamorphosing Salamandra used were the same size: 3.4–3.6 cm snout-vent length. While anurans have very specific, described developmental stages from egg to complete metamorphosis (Wells, 2007), no such general description exists for Salamandra. Pre-metamorphosing Salamandra larvae are characterized by full tail fins and long gills compared to shrunken, narrow tail fins and shrunken, short gills of metamorphosing larvae (e.g., Degani, 1996). For metamorphosing individuals, we chose individuals that had roughly one-half the width in tail fin and one-half the length of gills found in pre-metamorphosing larvae. Larvae of some, but not all, urodele species cease to feed during the process of metamorphosis (Wells, 2007), though it was not documented, prior to this study if metamorphosing Salamandra cease to feed.

The experiments were conducted with permission of the Israel Nature and Parks Authority permit 2009/36605 and the Haifa University Animal Experimentation Ethics Committee permit 190/10.

Experiment 1. Predation by metamorphosing and pre-metamorphosing Salamandra infraimmaculata larvae on Culiseta longiareolata larvae

To compare predation rates by metamorphosing and pre-metamorphosing salamander larvae on C. longiareolata larvae, we conducted laboratory predation trials between March and April 2010. Rectangular laboratory tubs (length × width × height: 29 × 18 × 13 cm) with 3 l of aged tap water served as arenas to measure predation. Treatments consisted of (1) control (no Salamandra larva); (2) one pre-metamorphosing Salamandra larva; (3) one metamorphosing Salamandra larva. Salamandra larvae were not fed for 24 h prior to a predation trial. Twenty-third-instar C. longiareolata larvae, collected from outdoor, water-filled plastic tubs, were placed into each tub. The number surviving was counted after 1 h. Temperatures ranged from 22 to 24° C. Each treatment was replicated eight times.

Experiment 2. Egg raft predation

To determine whether metamorphosing and pre-metamorphosing Salamandra larvae prey upon C. longiareolata egg rafts, we conducted an outdoor experiment in plastic containers (54 × 41 × 20 cm) filled to 20 l of aged tap water. Treatments consisted of (1) control (no Salamandra larvae); (2) three pre-metamorphosing Salamandra larvae; (3) three metamorphosing Salamandra larvae. This density is well within the range of natural densities (Blaustein, unpublished data). The salamander larvae were allowed to acclimate to the tubs for 48 h prior to adding egg rafts. Trials, seven per treatment, were conducted using Culiseta egg rafts between mid-April and mid-May 2010. Just prior to sunset, to each tub, we added three C. longiareolata egg rafts that had been oviposited in other tubs the previous night. We covered the top with mosquito window screening to prevent additional mosquito oviposition and influences by most other animals, but not necessarily small insects. Fire ants have been shown to prey on single mosquito eggs on moist soil above the water surface (Lee et al., 1994). The next morning, we counted remaining egg rafts.

In addition, to further determine if missing egg rafts in the outdoor mesocosm experiment were due to Salamandra egg raft predation, we also conducted observations in the laboratory. Into rectangular plastic tubs (length × width × height: 29 × 18 × 13 cm) containing 2 l of water, we added a single C. longiareolata egg raft to 7 tubs without a Salamandra larvae and to 14 tubs containing a single Salamandra larvae of similar size to other experiments and starved for 48 h. In none of the replicates were the Salamandra initially attracted to any movement caused by the placement of the egg raft into the water. We observed whether the egg raft was still present after 2 h. We additionally videoed several tubs with a Salamandra larva and egg raft (Nikon D3100 camera with 105 macro lens) to capture potential egg raft predation.

Experiment 3. Effects of pre-metamorphosing Salamandra on among-pool egg raft distribution

We conducted an outdoor, artificial pool experiment to determine if pre-metamorphosing Salamandra larvae (caged or free) could influence the observed among-pool distribution of C. longiareolata egg rafts. The experiment was conducted in the rural village of Koranit, Lower Galilee, Israel (460 m asl) in a yard partially shaded by Punica granatum trees. To serve as artificial pools, we used plastic tubs identical to those used in the egg raft predation experiment (length × width × height: 54 × 41 × 20 cm) with ~20 l of a mixture of rainwater and aged tap water. We added 1 g of dry cat food (Friskies Indoor Delight) to serve as a nutrient base. We randomly assigned artificial pools to one of three treatments: (1) control (no Salamandra larvae); (2) caged pre-metamorphosing Salamandra larvae (three Salamandra were caged and thus were unable to prey on deposited egg rafts or interfere with alighting mosquito females); (3) free pre-metamorphosing Salamandra (three Salamandra outside the cage and thus could potentially prey upon egg rafts and attack or interfere with alighting mosquito females).

On 5 March, we added three pre-metamorphosing Salamandra larvae to all free and caged Salamandra tubs. Individuals that initiated metamorphosis were removed and replaced. Each treatment was replicated with six tubs (total = 18 tubs). Inter-tub distances were ~0.5–0.7 m. Previous studies showed C. longiareolata adults to be able to discriminate between non-predator and predator (backswimmer) tubs at these inter-pool distances (e.g., Eitam et al., 2002; Blaustein et al., 2004). To all 18 tubs, regardless of treatment, we added a cage [2 l plastic bottle with a grid of holes (3 mm diameter every 2 cm)]. The holes allowed chemicals released by a caged Salamandra to diffuse from the cage into the tub. We added cages to all pools (not just the caged Salamandra pools) so that any potential effects of Salamandra on mosquito behavior observed in the caged treatment would not be confounded with effects of the cage itself. To further ensure that the tub water itself outside the cage was conditioned with Salamandra-released chemicals, each morning, we lifted the cage above the tub momentarily to allow the entire water content inside the cage to drain into the tub, and then immediately returned the cage to the tub. Because caged Salamandra likely fed less than free-roaming Salamandra, and because we were limited by our permit in the number of larvae that we could use, every 2 days, we removed all Salamandra larvae, and those inside cages were transferred to a randomly chosen free-Salamandra tub and those that were outside the cages (free treatment) were transferred into cages in randomly chosen caged treatment tubs. C. longiareolata egg rafts were counted and collected from 7–15 March 2010 (=9 nights).

Experiment 4. Effects of metamorphosing Salamandra larvae on among-pool egg raft distributions

This experiment was identical to the previous one, except that we used metamorphosing larvae instead of pre-metamorphosing ones, nearly completely metamorphosed individuals were replaced with partially metamorphosed as described earlier, and egg rafts were counted and collected from 29 March to 7 April 2010 (=9 nights).

Statistical analysis

For response variables C. longiareolata larval survival (experiment 1), egg raft survival (experiment 2), and total number of egg rafts per pool (experiments 3 and 4), we first subjected the data to a Levene’s test for homogeneity of variance. In each case, we failed to detect a deviation in homogeneity of variance and thus used the non-transformed numbers in conducting analyses of variance, choosing an alpha level of 0.05 (two tailed). In the cases of experiments 3 and 4, there were no indications of time × treatment interactions, and particularly because of the low number of egg rafts on any given day, we used the sum across all days for each pool as the response variable. If the ANOVA revealed a statistically significant difference, we then tested for treatment differences using a Tukey’s HSD test.

Results

Experiment 1. Predation by pre-metamorphosing and metamorphosing Salamandra infraimmaculata larvae on Culiseta longiareolata larvae

In the laboratory experiment, Culiseta larvae were vulnerable to predation by pre-metamorphosing salamander larvae only. There was virtually no larval Culiseta mortality in the control replicates (98.1% survival) or in the metamorphosing Salamandra treatment (93.2% survival) but mortality was strongly reduced in the presence of pre-metamorphosing salamander larvae (43.1% survival), a statistically significant reduction (F 2,23 = 67.84; P < 0.0001; Fig. 1a).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Larval survival (a) and egg raft survival (b) of C. longiareolata in the absence of S. infraimmaculata, and in the presence of metamorphosing or pre-metamorphosing Salamandra larvae. Error bars are one standard error. Different letters above error bars indicate statistically significant differences based on Tukey’s HSD test. The larval survival experiment was conducted in the laboratory and the egg raft survival experiment was conducted in outdoor-screened mesocosms

Experiment 2. Predation by pre-metamorphosing and metamorphosing Salamandra infraimmaculata larvae on Culiseta longiareolata egg rafts

Consistent with the results of Experiment 1, in the outdoor tub egg raft predation experiment, Culiseta egg rafts were vulnerable only to pre-metamorphosing salamander larvae. Egg raft survival was high in both the control and metamorphosing Salamandra tubs, but was reduced by 50% in the pre-metamorphosing salamander larvae tubs (F 2,18 = 7.00, P = 0.0056; Fig. 1b). In the laboratory observations, we found that 7 of 7 egg rafts were still present on the water surface after 48 h in the absence of a Salamandra larva while only 5 of 14 were still present in the presence of a pre-metamorphosing Salamandra larva. An online video demonstrates the ‘capture’ and swallowing of egg rafts (Online Resource 1).

Experiment 3. Effects of pre-metamorphosing Salamandra on among-pool egg raft distribution

Egg rafts were most abundant in control pools. There were 24% fewer in the caged Salamandra treatment though this difference was not statistically significant. Compared to the control tubs, there was a statistically significant 68% reduction of egg rafts in the free-roaming Salamandra tubs (F 2,15 = 4.01; P = 0.040; Fig. 2a).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Culiseta longiareolata egg raft distribution among outdoor mesocosms with no, caged, or free-roaming S. infraimmaculata. Graph a represents the experiment done with pre-metamorphosing Salamandra larvae. Graph b represents the experiment done with metamorphosing Salamandra larvae. Error bars are one standard error. Different letters above error bars indicate statistically significant differences based on Tukey’s HSD test

Experiment 4. Effects of metamorphosing Salamandra on among-pool egg raft distribution

Metamorphosing Salamandra, caged or free-roaming, did not cause any statistically significant reduction in egg rafts (F 2,15 = 0.12, P = 0.889; Fig. 2b).

Discussion

Many urodele species worldwide are of conservation concern, and their population declines and extinctions may be beneficial for mosquito populations. Our study adds to a surprisingly small number of studies that tests and demonstrates the strong predatory effects that larval urodeles can have on mosquito larvae (Brodman & Dorton, 2006; DuRant & Hopkins, 2008; Rubbo et al., 2011). Our study also demonstrates for the first time that a urodele preys upon mosquito egg rafts.

We found that C. longiareolata larvae are highly vulnerable to predation by pre-metamorphosing S. infraimmaculata larvae but not to metamorphosing ones. Vulnerability of this larval mosquito to pre-metamorphosing salamander larvae is not surprising. S. infraimmaculata is a generalist predator (e.g., Blaustein et al., 1996; Eitam et al., 2005) and C. longiareolata larvae, even in comparison with larvae of other mosquito species, are highly vulnerable to a wide range of predators (Blaustein & Whitman, 2009; Roberts, 2012).

We found that pre-metamorphosing, but not metamorphosing S. infraimmaculata prey on egg rafts. Very few studies have examined and shown that predators of any taxonomic group (backswimmers: Chesson, 1984; odonate larvae: Stav et al., 1999) prey upon, or disrupt and break up, mosquito egg rafts. Fire ants can prey upon single mosquito eggs laid on moist soil (Lee et al., 1994). Some terrestrial arthropods may prey upon egg rafts on the water surface but to our knowledge, this has not been studied. We know of no previous studies demonstrating that urodele larvae are predators of mosquito eggs. Although Salamandra larvae generally are strongly attracted to prey movement, they also may use olfactory cues to detect prey. For example, larvae this species have been fed in the laboratory with minced beef liver (Cohen et al., 2006). They apparently use chemical cues to determine genetic similarity of conspecific larvae and modulate their aggression or cannibalism accordingly (Markman et al., 2009). Egg rafts can also become a moving prey item and thus provide a visual-movement cue; they are commonly wind-blown across the water surface or may be moved by water turbulence when a Salamandra larva or other organisms swim near the egg raft.

That metamorphosing S. infraimmaculata did not prey on C. longiareolata is not a trivial finding as many urodele species continue to be predaceous while they metamorphose, though becoming less efficient as they transition from an aquatic suction feeder to a terrestrial predator (Lauder & Shaffer, 1986). Consistent with a reduction to cessation of feeding as they metamorphose, S. infraimmaculata loses considerable weight while metamorphosing (Blaustein, unpublished data). Thus, if prey can accurately track risk of predation, in the case of S. infraimmaculata, it should be able to distinguish between pre-metamorphosing and metamorphosing larvae. One way this could be tracked is if the cue comes from excretions of consumed prey (Laurila et al., 1997).

Traditionally, experiments in real aquatic habitats or outdoor mesocosms have measured the putative effect of mosquito antagonists (predators or competitors) by comparing mosquito larval densities or mosquito emergence in control versus antagonist plots. To assess this, experimenters were implicitly assuming random oviposition with respect to the antagonist. We now know that in many cases, this assumption is not true—that many mosquitoes detect many predators of their offspring and avoid ovipositing in pools of high antagonist risk (Vonesh & Blaustein, 2010). Thus, assuming random oviposition likely overestimates the effect of the antagonists on mosquito populations (Spencer et al., 2002; Blaustein et al., 2010), it is important to understand OHS of mosquitoes and also to properly interpret a reduction of eggs in predator pools. Although we failed to find a reduction of egg rafts in the caged pre-metamorphosing Salamandra treatment (which would indicate OHS), we cannot necessarily conclude that the reduction of egg rafts in the free-roaming pre-metamorphosing Salamandra treatment can be attributed completely to egg raft predation and that OHS does not contribute at all to this reduction. Future studies can determine whether the small but statistically insignificant reduction of egg rafts that we found in the caged treatment is real or additionally, whether the cage itself masks other predator risk cues such as visual or tactile cues that the ovipositing mosquito would normally use to detect and avoid Salamandra including whether mosquito females that have already alighted onto the water surface are frightened off by attacking Salamandra. C. longiareolata has shown strong oviposition avoidance of other insect predators (e.g., Stav et al., 1999; Blaustein et al., 2004). Additionally, crustacean eggs have shown a hatching inhibition in the presence of S. infraimmaculata larvae, presumably an evolutionary response to risk of predation (Blaustein, 1997; Spencer & Blaustein, 2001).

Culiseta longiareolata may not have evolved oviposition avoidance to this particular predator due to low temporal and spatial overlap despite a substantial predatory impact of young salamander larvae on its eggs and larval stages. Salamandra breed largely during the winter and metamorphose by end of April in temporary pools in this region while C. longiareolata breed largely between late March and early June. In addition, our work was conducted near the southern-most limit of S. infraimmaculata (Warburg, 1994; Degani, 1996). C. longiareolata is found both South of Salamandra’s southern border and also at lower elevations within the same region (Margalit et al., 1988; Blank & Blaustein, 2012).

In summary, we have demonstrated that S. infraimmaculata larvae, prior to initiating metamorphosis, prey heavily on the mosquito C. longiareolata—both on its larvae and its eggs. We were unable to demonstrate that this mosquito can also avoid pools containing Salamandra larvae. Understanding the mechanisms explaining fewer egg rafts in the presence of pre-metamorphosing Salamandra larvae is very important when it comes to understanding its effects on prey populations. When a prey mosquito species can avoid predator pools, the effect of the predator on the adult population is reduced (Spencer et al., 2002; Kershenbaum et al., 2012). Thus, predators that do not elicit an oviposition response or a weak oviposition response in mosquitoes may be more important natural or biocontrol agents than predators that do elicit oviposition avoidance.