Introduction

Abnormal cortical excitability may play an important role in predisposition to the spontaneous, cortical spreading depression that has been suggested to represent the pathophysiology of migraine aura [1, 2]. To date, a lack of habituation in migraineurs has been the principal and reproducible interictal abnormality observed in studies of evoked potentials, such as visual-evoked potentials (VEPs) [310]. Previous VEP studies revealed heterogeneous results; a lack of VEP habituation may correlate with metabolic abnormalities and decreased thalamo-cortical activity [11, 12]. The increased VEP amplitude that is revealed in migraine with aura (MA), but not in migraine without aura (MO), suggests abnormal excitability in the visual cortex [1315].

There have been a number of reports on steady-state VEPs induced by flash stimuli [1627], including an habituation study [28]. These studies suggest a general modification in the pattern of visual reactivity. However, no study of steady-state VEPs (SSVEPs) has yet evaluated pattern-reversal stimuli with respect to habituation behavior or spatial frequency. The present study hypothesized that various spatial frequencies would differentially affect SSVEPs in MO and MA. The aim of the present study was to clarify habituation behavior in MO and MA at four different spatial frequencies to determine the spatial and temporal characteristics of VEP by means of SSVEP.

Methods

Subjects

Thirty-six subjects participated in the study: 12 patients with MO, 12 patients with MA, and 12 healthy controls (HC). Migraine diagnosis was based on criteria of ICHD-II [29]. Demographic and clinical characteristics of patients and HC are shown in Table 1. At the time of investigation, all patients were in an interictal state, with at least 72 h from a migraine attack. The interval before the next headache was also monitored to avoid including patients who experienced a migraine in the 72 h following the experimental task. The patients and HCs exhibited normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity and were drug-free. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects.

Table 1 Demographic data and clinical characteristics

Visual stimulation

Black and white checkerboard patterns were generated on a 17-inch monitor, which subtended 21° × 17° at a viewing distance of 90 cm. SSVEPs were performed using Neuropak Λ (Nihon Kohden, Tokyo, Japan). Four spatial frequencies of 0.5, 1.0, 2.0, and 4.0 cycle per degree (cpd) were presented. The contrast in each spatial frequency was set to 90%, with a mean luminance of 14 cd/m2. The stimulus pattern was alternated at 7.5 Hz (15 reversal/s). Four different spatial frequencies of SSVEPs were randomly presented. The subjects were comfortably seated and asked to maintain eyes on a fixed central point.

Recording

Silver–silver chloride electrodes were attached to the scalp with collodion. Electrode impedance was maintained below 5000 Ω. Binocular SSVEPs were recorded from a mid-occipital electrode (2.5 cm above the inion) with a mid-frontal reference (12 cm above the nasion). Analog data were filtered between 0.1 and 70 Hz, and were digitized at a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz. Inputs from scalp electrodes were fed into preamplifiers adjusted to a bandwidth of 1–250 Hz. Artifact rejection was automatically controlled to avoid contamination of the signal by eye movement. For each spatial frequency, a total 80 consecutive VEP responses were collected and averaged off-line 4 blocks of 20 (30 s duration per block). The blocks were acquired sequentially. Under each spatial frequency condition, presentation of visual stimuli was separated by at least 2 min.

Data analysis

The amplified electroencephalogram, which was synchronized to temporal modulation of the stimulus, was digitized and stored on a computer for further analysis. Fast Fourier transforms (FFTs) were used to calculate amplitude (μV) for double the checkerboard reversal frequency of 7.5 Hz, i.e., the second harmonic response (2F = 15 Hz). According to our previous study on SSVEPs [30], because there were no significant phase differences between MO, MA, and HC, only the 2F amplitude was analyzed.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using the SPSS 11.0 statistical software package for Windows. The results were compared using repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). The within-subject factor was spatial frequency, and between-subject factors served as the diagnosis and the four blocks. Mauchly’s sphericity test was performed prior to ANOVA. When appropriate, the number of degrees of freedom was adjusted according to Greenhouse and Geisser. The post hoc test analysis was performed using Tukey’s HSD test. Habituation, i.e., sequential amplitude changes, was calculated using a relative value, and amplitudes of blocks 2–4 were divided by the amplitude of block 1. The level of significance was set at P < 0.05.

Results

Figure 1 shows representative VEP waveforms from a normal subject and FFTs in four sequential blocks at 2.0 cpd. VEPs were characterized by two dominant waveform frequencies that corresponded to a reversal frequency. In the FFTs, the second harmonic response (2F, 15 Hz) was predominant.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Representative VEP waveforms and fast Fourier transformations (FFTs) in a healthy control during four sequential blocks at 2.0 cpd. Steady-state responses are shown in the upper column. The FFTs demonstrate that 2F is clearly distinguishable in the amplitude spectrum (arrow head, lower column). The 2F amplitude was evaluated at each block as 1.4, 1.6, 1.6, and 1.3 μ, respectively

Figure 2 shows sequential mean amplitudes at each spatial frequency in the migraineurs and HC. In the first block, the mean amplitude in MO at 0.5 cpd was less than in the other blocks. However, amplitudes were greater in migraineurs at all spatial frequencies. A significant interaction between spatial frequency and diagnosis was observed (Table 2), showing that spatial frequency affected HC and migraineurs differently. In the MA patients, a post hoc test revealed that amplitude differences reached statistical significance at 2.0 cpd [F(8, 258) = 5.0, P < 0.001 for MO, P < 0.05 for HC]. There was no interaction between block and diagnosis or spatial frequency. HC and migraine patients did not show clear habituation or a lack of habituation. Figure 3 shows sequential amplitude changes at each spatial frequency in migraineurs and HC. A significant interaction between diagnosis and spatial frequency was observed (Table 3). In the MA patients, when a lower spatial frequency (0.5 cpd) was presented, the amplitude tended to increase over the blocks. In a post hoc test, the sequential amplitude changes in MA were significantly different from HC ([F(12, 262) = 2.4, P < 0.05]). These results indicated that MA showed increased sequential amplitude changes only at lower spatial frequency.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean VEP amplitudes in four blocks in migraine without aura (MO), migraine with aura (MA), and healthy controls (HC) at 0.5, 1.0, 2.0, and 4.0 cpd. Error bars represent SEM

Table 2 Effect of diagnosis and spatial frequency on steady-state VEP amplitude1 and amplitude habituation (block factor interaction)2 and overall habituation effect3
Fig. 3
figure 3

Sequential amplitude changes at 0.5, 1.0, 2.0, and 4.0 cpd in MO, MA, and HC. The error bars represent SEM

Table 3 Effect of diagnosis and spatial frequency on steady-state VEP amplitude1 and amplitude habituation2 and the overall habituation effect3

Discussion

Interictal SSVEPs at 7.5 Hz exhibited no habituation in HCs, and migraine patients did not show a clear lack of habituation. The mean amplitude in each block, as well as sequential amplitude changes and average amplitude, were influenced by spatial frequency. At 2.0 cpd, MA patients exhibited greater VEP amplitude in all four blocks, and 0.5 cpd resulted in increased sequential amplitude changes.

Results from the present study were not consistent with previous reports [35, 7], which demonstrated deficient habituation in migraineurs recorded by check size 8′ and 3.1 Hz, i.e., higher spatial frequency and non-flickering higher temporal frequency of transient-type pattern-reversal VEP (TVEP). The TVEP alteration in migraine-dependent pattern spatial frequency was reported to be 2.0 Hz or less, which was a slower temporal frequency [14, 15, 3133]. These studies failed to detect habituation abnormalities. To date, habituation phenomena in normal subjects have not been evaluated in a SSVEP study. The present results are, however, consistent with steady-state VEPs using flash stimuli in a low frequency range of 3, 6, and 9 Hz [28]. These VEP studies are supported by habituation characteristics that show a relationship among stimulus repetition rates, stimulus characteristics, and degree of habituation [34]. Accordingly, the faster and smaller check-size stimulation by TVEP may play a crucial role in habituation behavior [14].

The present SSVEP study is in accordance with previous studies [30, 35] that demonstrated greater migraine amplitude between attacks. Furthermore, the amplitude tended to increase depending on spatial frequency. Generally, the response to higher spatial frequency preferentially stimulates the macular region, which has a much larger cortical representation, as opposed to lower spatial frequency that preferentially stimulates the periphery of the retina [36, 37]. The increased response to higher spatial frequency could be explained by altered excitability of the visual cortex in migraine patients [30]. A number of studies have not observed consistent MA versus MO, or migraine versus control, amplitude differences [3843]. However, the present data suggested that increased amplitude was a general phenomenon, in particular in MA patients [1315, 30]. Although the lack of habituation could be a result of a pre-cortical site [11, 12], high VEP amplitude could be explained in the present study by the concept of cortical hyper-responsiveness in the visual cortex [1, 44, 45]. The discrepancy between these studies might be explained by different recording conditions, such as the use of spatial and temporal frequency.

The visual system is subdivided into two parallel channels of visual processing—the magnocellular (M) and parvocellular (P) pathways, which specialize in different visual functions. The M pathway, which receives input primarily from the retinal periphery, is more sensitive to high-temporal and low-spatial frequency stimuli, while the P pathway receives input primarily from the foveal and perifoveal areas and is preferentially activated by low-temporal and high-spatial frequency stimulation [14, 46, 47]. Spatial and temporal frequency has been suggested to play an important role in visual processing of migraine [30, 37, 4853]. Psychophysical tests identified a pre-cortical site of action in the pathophysiology of migraine [49, 50, 52]. Previous reports have suggested that migraine affects the M [50, 54], or both the M and P [53, 55], pathways, while other studies did not confirm M pathway deficits [56]. Using TVEP, Oelkers et al. [14] hypothesized that migraine patients have a relatively impaired P pathway and a predominant M pathway. Different involvement of the P and M pathways may play a role in habituation behavior.

In this study, the HC did not show habituation, but there were increased sequential amplitude changes in MA patients at lower spatial frequency of 0.5 cpd. This suggests the excitability change cannot be interpreted as “lack of habituation”. In accordance with our study, Chen et al. [33] reported that migraineurs showed a larger amplitude increment than controls in sequential blocks only at lower spatial frequency. The findings may suggest “augmented potentiation” or “relative habituation deficit”. A recent study by Omland et al. [57] also revealed that habituation may not be detectable in healthy subjects using large 65′ checks, i.e., lower spatial frequency and reversal rate had less influence than lower spatial frequency on habituation. The difference in excitability of the visual cortex in migraine may preferentially involve the M pathway [33].

A previous study [28] reported that steady-state VEP, induced by flash stimuli at low frequency range, altered amplitude in a complex way, which could be explained by a variability phenomenon. Increased variability with response to low-temporal frequency in migraine could be interpreted as an overactive regulation mechanism, and this effect may result from activation of the M extrastriate pathways. Unstructured flash stimuli are likely to be processed primarily in the M pathway [36]. Therefore, increased sequential amplitude changes in MA might be due to stimulation conditions at a low-spatial frequency and flickering, which are similar to M pathway conditions. The current findings might be linked to predominance of the M system in MA and suggest that P–M imbalance plays a role in migraine pathophysiology. This is consistent with previous reports of VEPs [30] and psychophysical findings [45, 58]. Although the question of predominance of P or M neuronal dysfunction remains, it is likely that migraine patients have pre-cortical dysfunctions, in addition to altered excitability of the visual cortex [28].

Our previous TVEP study [37] demonstrated high-spatial frequency (4.0 cpd) with a high-contrast (98%) pattern, which was preferable to stimulate the P pathway. These conditions were determined to be effective for revealing abnormal reactivity in MO and MA patients [58, 59]. However, using these conditions, a low-contrast (29%) pattern also resulted in increased amplitude. Similar to previous results [60], hyperreactivity of the P pathway is not likely to result in migraine.

Our previous SSVEP study at 5 and 10 Hz [30] demonstrated that MA patients exhibited high-amplitude VEPs, but no significant amplitude differences were detected at 2.0 cpd. The discrepancy between this and the current study may be due to the different temporal frequencies utilized. A checkerboard VEP pattern is very complex in terms of FFTs, and a 0.5 cpd pattern includes other components of cpd [61]. Furthermore, a temporal frequency of 7.5 Hz does not selectively stimulate the M pathway in high-contrast (90%) vs. low-contrast (20%) conditions [62, 63]. VEPs induced by square-wave gratings are required to determine amplitude changes at each frequency, as well as to determine P pathway functions [59].

A limitation of this study is the stimulus time of VEPs. Previous VEP habituation studies used longer recording sessions of 3–15 min [3, 4, 14, 15, 33, 57], compared with 2 min in our study. The criteria of habituation proposed that a high repetition rate speeds up the habituation, and a low repetition rate slows it down [34]. A future study with a longer SSVEP stimulus time is required to clarify the habituation behavior in migraine.

In conclusion, interictal migraine patients exhibited high-amplitude SSVEPs, which were dependent on spatial frequency, and may be related to altered excitability in pre-cortical and cortical visual processing.