Introduction

Migraine is one of the most common and burdensome primary headache disorder [1]. Headache is accompanied by nausea, vomiting, and light/sound sensitivity, and increased by routine physical activity [2]. Research on investigating the pathophysiological mechanisms of migraine is still one of the hottest topics. During an attack, photophobia is reported in 82.5% of the migraineurs [3]. Anatomy and physiology of photophobia have been reviewed in detail [4]. Basically, “visual system pathway dysfunction” has been postulated in migraineurs [5, 6]. Compared to the healthy control, the brains of migraineurs are reported to be hyperresponsive, especially in the occipital cortex, demonstrated by functional MRI studies [7]. Besides, light or glare is known to trigger migraine attacks. Similarly, among the aura symptoms, visual aura is reported to be the most common type of aura compared to the speech/language disturbances, sensory, motor, brainstem, or monocular visual loss [8]. On electrophysiological studies, pattern-reversal repetitive visual evoked potential (rVEP) dominates in migraineurs, supported by the evidence of lack of habituation to the repeated stimuli, interictally [9]. In migraineurs, increased responsiveness in the visual cortex is reported [10].

Some migraineurs report transient cognitive dysfunctions such as concentration and comprehension difficulties during or shortly after an attack [11]. Among the migraineurs with frequent attacks and long history of migraine, disturbances of memory, attention, and low speed of visuomotor processing are reported [12]. However, due to methodological differences, there are some controversial reports about cognitive functioning in migraineurs, either poor or normal cognitive performance [13]. The migraine severity and duration may also be confounding factors for this discrepancy. Children with migraine are reported to have visual attention deficits [14]. Visual attention may be assessed by Cancellation Test [15]. It measures the capacity of sustained attention, the sensory component related with perceptual representations, and motor component related with visual search/scan [15]. The structure of the test includes factors that are labeled as visual search, scan, impulsivity, and response speed.

In this study, we aimed to investigate if any difference exists, interictally, in visual attention and visual habituation of frequently attacked migraineurs compared to the healthy control group. The effects of migraine preventive drugs on these parameters were also assessed.

Methods

Participants

Female and males aged between 18 and 60 years, who had an adequate level of understanding the questionnaires with written consent, were enrolled prospectively between July, 2009 and July, 2011. The Local Ethical Committee approved the study. The migraine group composed of headache patients admitted to Neurology Department who were diagnosed with migraine based on ICHD-II criteria [16]. The inclusion criteria were as follows: to have >4 migraine attacks/months for more than 3 months, each attack severity of VAS ≥ 3/10 (in a scale of 0–10, in which “0” is no pain and “10” worst pain imaginable), eligible to take prophylactic treatment either with propranolol, flunarizine, or topiramate. The control group composed of age- and sex-matched healthy volunteers. Exclusion criteria for the participants were as follows: Migraine patients currently taking or have received any medications ≤3 months ago for migraine prophylaxis; patients who have any contraindication to take the study drugs; patients having any type of headaches other than migraine; patients with any type of painful conditions, the presence of any systemic, metabolic, endocrinologic, neurologic, or psychiatric disease; patients taking any type of drugs which may affect cognitive and electrophysiological studies, history of optic neuritis, or any pathology of the visual system, pregnancy, history of substance/drug abuse, history of neurosurgery. Migraine history, neurological examination, laboratory evaluations, and when needed neuroradiological examinations were performed. All participants had a baseline VEP and Cancellation test interictally (headache-free interval, at least 3 days before or after a migraine attack). Migraine patients were randomized into 3 subgroups; propranolol 40 mg tid, flunarizine 5 mg bid, and topiramate 50 mg bid. Baseline and 3 months after prophylactic treatment, pattern-reversal repetitive VEP recordings, and Cancellation test were performed.

The repetitive pattern-reversal VEP (rVEP) Recording Method

The subjects were asked to focus on the red spot in the center of the checkerboard, with one eye closed. Medelec Synergy EMG/EP unit (MEDELEC Synergy, USA) was used for VEP recordings. The sweep speed was 250 ms, filters 1–100 Hz, sensitivity 5 μV, and contrast 100%. In a standard VEP recording protocol, a minimum of 2 VEP waves are averaged by 200 stimuli recorded from Oz–Cz electrodes. For the repetitive pattern-reversal VEP technique, the responses to uninterrupted visual stimuli at 3.1 Hz were recorded during 10 sequential blocks. Each block consisted of 100 averaged VEP responses. The peak-to-peak amplitudes of N1-P1 and P1-N2 peaks were calculated [10]. The amplitude changes of 5th and 10th blocks were further compared with that of block 1 to assess the response of habituation (i.e., decrease) or potentiation (i.e., increase) as described before [17]. All data were analyzed offline, blinded to the study groups.

The Cancellation test

The Cancellation test (CT) involves searching and scanning for target letter or symbols against a background of distractor [15]. The test has four sheets: organized letters, organized symbols, random letters, and random symbols (Fig. 1). In organized sets, the distribution of stimuli arranged in rows and columns follows a sequence. In the random sets, stimuli are distributed randomly on sheets without any logical sequence. Each set contains 60 target stimuli (15 targets located in each quadrant) embedded in 300 items. Subjects are required to circle the target stimuli. After every 10 correct responses, the colored pencil is changed. The test was performed at hospital by one the authors (FI). The total scores for correct target responses (CT1), number of omission errors (CT2), number of incorrect responses (CT3), total number of errors (CT4), and time to complete/duration (CT5) were calculated. The test–retest reliability coefficients of CT scores in healthy adults changed between 0.32 and 0.81 [18]. The data were analyzed by an experienced clinical psychologist (EEB) who was blinded to the groups.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The cancellation test sheets

Statistical analysis

The data were analyzed with the PASW Statistics version 18 software package. The normal distribution of variables was verified by the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test and Shapiro–Wilk test. Comparisons of the dependent groups were made either with Student’s t test (parametric) or Wilcoxon signed-rank test (non-parametric). Comparisons of independent groups were made either with Student’s t test (parametric) or Mann–Whitney U test (non-parametric). Chi-square (X 2) test was used to investigate whether distributions of categorical variables differ within groups. Data are shown as mean ± SD for continuous variables and absolute numbers for dichotomous variables. p value less than 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

In this study, 87 subjects were eligible. Age (35.6 ± 9.1 versus 34.2 ± 9.6 years) and sex (83 versus 77% female) did not differ between the migraineurs (n = 52) and healthy control group (n = 35) (p > 0.05). Four migraineurs were diagnosed with migraine with aura. The migraineurs had 7.6 ± 4.3 attacks/month and disease duration was 6.9 ± 5.8 years. For prophylaxis, the migraineurs were randomized to flunarizine (n = 16), propranolol (n = 19), and topiramate (n = 17) treatment groups. The migraine attack frequency and disease duration did not differ between the treatment groups (all p > 0.05).

In migraineurs, sustained visual attention parameters, assessed by the Cancellation test, were statistically distorted compared to the healthy control group (Table 1a). Compared to the healthy control group, before preventive treatment, the number of correct targets was less (59.33 ± 0.63 versus 58.53 ± 1.28 p < 0.05), omission errors were more (0.67 ± 0.63 versus 1.46 ± 1.28, p < 0.05), total number of errors were more (0.69 ± 0.69 versus 1.62 ± 1.38 p < 0.05), and time to complete/duration of the tests were longer (80.36 ± 17.16 versus 105.3 ± 44.06 p < 0.05). These results suggest attention deficit and prolonged reaction time in frequently attacked migraineurs compared to the age- and sex-matched healthy control group.

Table 1 (a) Comparison of Cancellation test scores in healthy control and migraineurs before treatment. (b) Comparison of Cancellation test scores, baseline, and 3 months after treatment in migraineurs

Three months after preventive treatment, compared to their baseline values, visual attention parameters in migraineurs improved. The number of migraineurs completed the study was less than recruited due to loss of follow-up. However, the comparisons were made according to the pre- and post-treatment data of the same group of patients who completed the study. Basically, in migraineurs, compared to their baseline values, 3 months after migraine preventive treatment, the number of correct targets was more (58.53 ± 1.24 versus 59.20 ± 0.94, p < 0.05), omission errors were less (1.47 ± 1.24 versus 0.80 ± 0.94, p < 0.05), total number of errors were less (1.55 ± 1.20 versus 0.85 ± 0.98, p < 0.05), and time to complete/duration of the tests were shorter (103.61 ± 44.73 versus 96.15 ± 39.29, p < 0.05) (Table 1b).

To assess habituation or potentiation response in migraineurs and healthy control group, sub-averaged peak-to peak amplitudes at blocks 5 and 10 were compared with that of block 1. In healthy control, the amplitudes of the 1st and the 10th blocks were 8.91 ± 2.73 and 7.72 ± 2.71 µV on the right side and 8.39 ± 2.29, 8.02 ± 3.72 µV on left side, respectively. There was a decrease in amplitude and habituation ability was statistically significant in the healthy control (all p < 0.05) (Table 2). However in migraineurs before treatment, the amplitudes of the 1st and the 10th blocks were 8.94 ± 3.63 and 8.78 ± 3.55 µV on the right side and 8.67 ± 3.62, 8.55 ± 3.62 µV on left side, respectively. Habituation ability was not detected in migraineurs (all p > 0.05) (Table 2). Three months after migraine preventive treatment, loss of habituation ability was restored (Table 3). The amplitudes of the 1st and the 10th blocks were 8.93 ± 2.99, 7.96 ± 2.54 µV on the right and 9.23 ± 3.33, 8.30 ± 2.76 µV on the left side, respectively (all p < 0.05).

Table 2 Comparisons of p100 amplitudes between the 1st–5th–10th blocks in healthy control and migraineurs
Table 3 Comparisons of p100 Amplitudes between the 1st–5th–10th blocks, 3 months after treatment in migraineurs, and in propranolol, flunarizine, and topiramate subgroups

When each study drugs were analyzed separately, all drugs were effective in restoring the habituation ability in migraineurs (Table 3). The percentage changes (% mean ± SD) are also included in Table 3. For propranolol, the amplitudes of the 1st and the 10th blocks were 9.32 ± 2.67, 7.82 ± 2.57 µV on the right and 9.37 ± 2.88, 8.03 ± 2.56 µV on the left side, respectively. For flunarizine, the amplitudes of the 1st and the 10th blocks were 8.63 ± 3.86, 7.83 ± 3.34 µV on the right and 9.78 ± 4.28, 8.82 ± 3.48 µV on the left side, respectively. For topiramate, the amplitudes of the 1st and the 10th blocks were 8.82 ± 2.53, 8.29 ± 1.56 µV on the right and 8.48 ± 2.81, 8.08 ± 2.30 µV on the left side, respectively (Table 3).

Discussion

In this study, we aimed to identify if any difference exists in visual attention and visual evoked habituation of frequently attacked migraineurs compared to age- and sex-matched healthy control group. The effects of migraine preventive treatment on these parameters were also assessed.

Migraine is a common primary headache with a prevalence of 15–17% in females and 6% in males [19]. In our study, the female migraineurs were also 4.8 times more than the male migraineurs. Age and sex did not differ between the groups. The pathophysiology of migraine is complex [20]. The migrainous brain is structurally, anatomically, and functionally altered, and very sensitive to fluctuations in homeostasis [20]. It is both hypersensitive to external stimuli and hyperresponsive to repeated stimuli [21]. In a repetitive pattern-reversal VEP study, loss of habituation was proposed to be related with a functional disconnection of the thalamus leading to decreased intracortical lateral inhibition in migraineurs [22]. Altered processing of sensory stimuli and altered synchronicity in migraine patients were also proposed to be related with thalamocortical dysrhythmia [23]. In a recent human imaging study, the presence of abnormal low-frequency oscillations in thalamocortical networks in the interictal phase of migraineurs was demonstrated [24].

Although migraine is known to be relatively a benign and episodic neurological disease, some neuroimaging findings [25] and neuropsychological impairments are reported in frequently attacked migraineurs and with long disease duration [13]. Frequently attacked migraineurs are reported to have compromised physical, mental, and social functioning [26]. In a healthy brain, the normal selective attention function requires attenuation of sensory responses in the cortex [27]. It is suggested that loss of habituation might affect daily cognition in migraineurs such as difficulties in attention [28]. In migraineurs, interictally, neurocognitive processing of visual stimuli is reported to be dysfunctional, called as “lack of cognitive-level visual habituation” [28]. In a neurophysiological study, 232 unfamiliar images were viewed and event-related potentials were recorded in the healthy control group and migraineurs [28]. The amplitude of late positive potentials was found to be increased across trial blocks in migraineurs [28]. In the literature, compared to the healthy control group, slower reaction times are reported in migraineurs assessed by the Stroop test [29]. Cancellation Test (CT) measures the capacity of selective sustained attention, the sensory component related with perceptual representations, motor component related with visual search and scan, and motivational component related with affect. In our study, we found distorted visual attention performance in migraineurs assessed by CT. Compared to the healthy control group, the number of correct targets was less, omission errors were more, total number of errors were more, and time to complete/duration of the tests were longer in migraineurs. These results suggest attention deficits and prolonged reaction time in migraineurs compared to the age- and sex-matched healthy control group. In a previous study, children with migraine were also shown to have selective and alternate attention difficulties compared to the healthy control group [14]. Abnormal neuronal oscillations, mainly thalamocortical dysrhythmia and misguided striatal oscillations, were also proposed in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder [30].

In our study, the migraineurs were randomized to flunarizine (n = 16), propranolol (n = 19), and topiramate (n = 17) treatment groups. The migraine attack frequency and disease duration did not differ between the treatment groups (all p > 0.05). Three months after migraine preventive treatment, compared to their baseline values, visual attention parameters improved. The number of correct targets was more, omission errors were less, total number of errors was less, and time to complete/duration of the tests were shorter. Similar to our findings, children with migraine reported to have visual attention deficits and migraine preventive treatment (sodium valproate, amitriptyline, flunarizine) reversed their performance assessed by CT [31]. We do not think this is a sole training effect, because CT was performed 3 months apart and it does not test the long-term memory, but only attention. Although propranolol, flunarizine, and topiramate may produce memory disturbances and behavioral side effects (e.g., depression) [32, 33], our results suggest that migraine itself might have deleterious effects on attention. Migraine preventive treatment may restore visual attention.

Repetitive stimulation is associated with selective activation of anaerobic glycolysis. The amplitude of evoked cortical response to repetitive stimuli normally decreases in healthy brain, which is known as “habituation” [34]. This habituation phenomenon is known to protect brain against over-excitation and lactate accumulation due to repetitive stimulation [35]. However, in migraineurs, this protective phenomenon is shown to be dysfunctional and loss of habituation and even potentiation have been reported [36]. In our study, we evaluated interictal habituation response by repetitive visual stimulation as described before [17]. In the healthy control group, the habituation ability was preserved; however, it was not detected in migraineurs. There are some conflicting reports against the loss of habituation in migraineurs [37]. We may assume methodological differences such as repetitive VEP technique, migraine severity, and disease duration may affect these negative reports. As reported before, we suggest comparing the N1-P1 amplitudes of the 1st block to the 10th block of 100 VEPs each, in frequently attacked migraineurs [17]. In a recent test–retest reliability study of visual habituation, loss of habituation was confirmed in migraineurs [38]. Similarly, both in blinded and non-blinded analyses, VEP habituation was reported to be deficient in migraineurs interictally [39].

Three months after migraine preventive treatment, loss of habituation ability was restored in our migraineurs. When each study drugs were analyzed separately, all drugs were effective in restoring the habituation ability. In a transcranial magnetic stimulation study of 29 migraineurs, cortical excitability of the visual cortex was also shown to be modulated by propranolol preventive treatment [40]. The action mechanism of flunarizine in migraine prophylaxis is not completely understood. In a whole-cell patch clamp recordings of cultured rat cortical neurons, flunarizine was shown to block sodium and calcium currents, a potential mechanism of cortical hyperexcitability [41]. In our clinical study, we were able to show that flunarizine restored loss of habituation in migraineurs, probably through the same mechanism. Topiramate was also able to restore habituation in our migraineurs. Similar to our result, in a study of migraine patients without aura, 2-month treatment with topiramate was shown to reverse loss of habituation assessed by acoustic stimulation [42]. In another study conducted by magnetoencephalography, visual cortex excitability was shown to be associated with remission in chronic migraineurs after topiramate preventive treatment [43]. One of the limitations of our study might be the limited number of migraine patients in each group to demonstrate a statistically significant difference among three different migraine preventive drugs. The number of subjects who were retested after 3 months of treatment was lower than the recruited. However, the comparisons and analyses were made according to the pre- and post-treatment data of the same patients who completed the study. The other limitation of our study was that we were not able to identify a possible correlation between the VEP habituation parameters, Cancellation Test items, and the clinical response to the prophylactic treatments because of insufficient clinical data. However, our study was designed only to assess the effects of 3 months of migraine prophylactic treatment on visual attention and electrophysiological (visual habituation) parameters in migraineurs. Propranolol, flunarizine, and topiramate are drugs that are approved to be clinically effective on migraine prophylaxis. In our study, we did not design to collect data in detail of the clinical outcome measures such as more than 30 or 50% of reduction in migraine attack frequency or migraine severity.

In this study, we demonstrated that 3 different preventive drugs (propranolol, flunarizine, and topiramate) restored visual attention and evoked cortical hyperresponsiveness in migraineurs. We may assume that visual attention deficit in migraineurs may be due to the same pathophysiological mechanisms underlying VEP habituation deficit such as “thalamocortical dysrhythmia”. This proposal may require further detailed investigations.

Conclusions

In summary, this is a prospective, randomized, electrophysiological, and neurophysiological study to assess the effects of preventive treatment in frequently attacked migraineurs compared to the healthy control group. It demonstrates an objective data on how frequent migraine attacks affect brain responsivity and attention. It also provides how migraine preventive drugs restore these brain functions. The loss of habituation ability and low visual attention performance in migraine patients may be restored by migraine preventive treatments. We hope this electrophysiological study accompanied by neuropsychological tests may provide an objective and quantitative assessment tool for our understanding of migraine pathophysiology and response to different treatment modalities.