Introduction

For several decades, 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetmine (MDMA) has garnered interest as an adjunct to psychotherapy due to its profound acute mood-elevating and prosocial effects (Greer and Tolbert 1986). Recently, just two sessions of MDMA-assisted psychotherapy have been found to promote long-term reductions in symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Mithoefer et al. 2011). Although the pharmacological and psychological mechanisms of this effect are not well understood, pre-clinical research suggests that MDMA enhances the extinction of fear memory (Young et al. 2015). Further mechanistic studies of this phenomenon are required to anticipate important considerations for the use of MDMA in a clinical setting.

Many PTSD patients continue to experience intensely fearful responses to traumatic memories long after the fear response serves a protective function, which may be due to impairments in the extinction of fear memory (Rothbaum and Davis 2003). The extinction of fear memory is a learning process in which the fear response to an aversive memory diminishes as it repeatedly fails to predict an expected threat. Fear extinction processes can be engaged by exposure-based therapies for PTSD through the repeated re-exposure to trauma-related stimuli in the absence of harmful consequences (Rothbaum and Schwartz 2002). Pharmacological adjuncts that enhance extinction learning have been hypothesized to improve outcomes in exposure-based therapy (de Kleine et al. 2013). In fear-conditioned mice, administering MDMA systemically or directly to the amygdala 30 min prior to fear extinction training results in lasting improvements in fear memory extinction (Young et al. 2015). This effect depends on signaling through brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) initiated when MDMA and extinction training are combined. However, translating the extinction-enhancing properties of MDMA in rodents to exposure-based therapies in human populations requires a significantly greater understanding of the mechanisms by which MDMA enhances the extinction of fear memory.

In the brain, MDMA primarily targets monoaminergic transporters and receptors (Battaglia et al. 1988), several of which have been demonstrated to play a role in the extinction of fear memory. Like other phenethylamine derivatives, MDMA potently releases dopamine (DA), norepinephrine (NE), and serotonin (5-HT) through their respective transporters (Green et al. 2003). Treatments that increase 5-HT or NE neurotransmission—selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), exogenous NE, and direct serotonergic/adrenergic agonists—all have been observed to enhance fear memory extinction in certain cases (Burghardt and Bauer 2013; Mueller and Cahill 2010; Zhang et al. 2013). MDMA’s effects on transporter-mediated release can be obstructed by pre-treatment with selective reuptake inhibitors (Fitzgerald and Reid 1990; Hekmatpanah and Peroutka 1990; Hysek et al. 2012). Specifically, SSRIs and norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors (NRIs) also inhibit some of MDMA’s behavioral/subjective effects (Hysek et al. 2011; Liechti and Vollenweider 2000b), demonstrating that MDMA’s unique behavioral profile depends on access to transporters of 5-HT and NE.

The primary purpose of the current study was to identify monoaminergic mechanisms through which MDMA enhances the extinction of fear memory. A second aim of this study was to validate the effect of MDMA on fear memory extinction using a measure of fear behavior more translatable to humans. Previous studies with MDMA have used conditioned freezing, which is a stereotypic fear behavior in rodents. Conversely, fear-potentiated startle (FPS) is a highly conserved fear behavior across species, wherein the presence of a fear-eliciting stimulus increases the startle reflex to a burst of noise (Davis 2006). FPS has been repeatedly used as an index of conditioned fear expression and extinction across animal and human investigations (Briscione et al. 2014; Norrholm et al. 2006). Though their expression relies on different neurological systems, extinction of both conditioned freezing and FPS depends on the amygdala, where MDMA has increased BDNF signaling important for learning (Myers and Davis 2007; Young et al. 2015). While similar patterns of freezing and FPS expression are observed when simultaneously measuring them in response to a conditioned aversive stimulus, FPS appears to extinguish more slowly than conditioned freezing (Daldrup et al. 2015).

Using auditory Pavlovian fear conditioning to instate a fear memory, we demonstrate a leftward shift in the dose-response relationship between MDMA and the extinction of FPS in comparison to observations in previous studies of the extinction of conditioned freezing. Using selective pharmacological inhibitors of DA, NE, or 5-HT transporters, we reveal a crucial role for the 5-HT transporter (5-HTT) in MDMA’s enhancing effect on fear memory extinction. This effect was also blocked by pharmacological treatments that inhibit 5-HT2A receptors (5-HT2AR). The behavioral and pharmacological data here provide further evidence of MDMA’s enhancing effect on fear memory extinction and also underscore the importance of 5-HT neurotransmission in this effect.

Methods and materials

Animals

Male C57BL/6 mice were from the Jackson Laboratory (Bar Harbor, ME, USA) and bred at the Yerkes National Primate Research Center at Emory University. Mice were group-housed in ventilated cages and maintained on ad libitum food and water. Lights in the vivarium turned on at 7:00 a.m. and turned off at 9:00 p.m. All experiments were performed on male mice between postnatal days 49 and 112. Studies were performed during the lights-on phase, with experiments taking place between 9:00 a.m. and 5:00 p.m. Mice were trained and tested during the lights-on phase, when rates of fear extinction are comparatively slower (Chaudhury and Colwell 2002). Studies were in accordance with National Institutes of Health guidelines, and all procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Emory University.

Drugs

S,R(±)-MDMA (MDMA), M100907 (M100), and RTI-336 were supplied by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (Research Technology Branch, Research Triangle Park, NC, USA). Citalopram was obtained from Eli Lilly and Co. (Indianapolis, IN, USA). Reboxetine was obtained from Tocris Bioscience (Avonmouth, Bristol, UK). (−)-1-(2,5-Dimethoxy-4-iodophenyl)-2-aminopropane hydrochloride (DOI) was obtained from Sigma-Aldrich (St. Louis, MO, USA). MDMA, citalopram, reboxetine, and DOI were dissolved in 0.9% saline immediately before experimentation. RTI-336 was initially dissolved in 100% ethanol and diluted to less than a 2% concentration with saline. M100 was dissolved in saline and 0.1 N HCl. Drug vehicle served as control for each respective experiment with a given drug. All solutions were injected intraperitoneally (i.p.) at a volume of 10 μL/g body weight. MDMA was administered 30 min prior to the behavioral procedure based on a previous study (Young et al. 2015), and transporter inhibitors were administered 30 min before MDMA because all three drugs reach peak brain concentrations within 30 min of administration and remain elevated for at least 90 min (Kimmel et al. 2008; Kreilgaard et al. 2008; Strolin Benedetti et al. 1995).

Conditioning and extinction of freezing behavior

The procedure for acquisition and sub-optimal extinction of conditioned freezing are described in detail previously (Young et al. 2015). Briefly, mice were exposed to cued fear conditioning on day 1, fear extinction training on day 3, and extinction testing on day 4. Cued fear conditioning consisted of four pairings of a CS tone (75–80 dB, 6.0 kHz, 30 s) and a US footshock (0.6 mA, 1 s). Extinction training was carried out 48 h after fear conditioning (“Day 3”) in a different context from conditioning, where mice were exposed to 16 CS tones separated by 45 s each. Saline or MDMA was administered 30 min prior to extinction training, and mice were individually housed during the period between drug administration and the behavioral procedure. Freezing was scored by video recording and FreezeView software (Coulbourn Instruments, San Diego, CA, USA). For experiments exploring the effects of chronically administered citalopram, mice were treated daily with 10 mg/kg (i.p.) of citalopram for 22 days between fear conditioning and extinction training. Mice were habituated to handling and i.p. injection for 2 days prior to experimentation.

Fear-potentiated startle

Studies exploring FPS were carried out using a startle response system from San Diego Instruments (San Diego, CA, USA). Two separate FPS experiments were performed, and the protocols were based on previous studies of FPS in mice (Smith et al. 2011). The first experiment tested the effects of a range of MDMA doses on the within- and between-session extinction of FPS. On days 1–3 (“Acclimation”) of the first experiment, mice were placed in the startle chamber, given a 5-min period during which no auditory stimuli were presented, and then presented with 50 semi-random 20-ms white noise startle stimuli (10 each of 70, 80, 85, 90, and 100 dB) with a 30-s inter-trial interval (ITI). On day 4 (“PreTest”), study mice were given a 5-min acclimation period followed by presentation of 10 startle stimuli (85 dB; 1 min ITI), and then 20 “CS + startle” trials consisting of a 30-s, 12-kHz, 70-dB tone that terminated with a white-noise startle stimulus. The CS tone used in the PreTest is identical to the one used as the CS on the following fear conditioning day. On day 5, animals were fear conditioned to 10 CS-US pairings (0.25 s, 0.4-mA) in the same fear conditioning chambers used in previously described experiments. On day 6 (“Memory Test”), FPS was assessed by presenting four interspersed “startle stimulus alone” and CS + startle stimuli. FPS during the Memory Test was averaged for all mice during this trial, and treatment groups were prospectively counterbalanced for equal FPS. On day 7, the effect of MDMA on the within-session extinction of FPS was tested by administering vehicle or MDMA 20 min prior to placing mice in the startle chamber. After drug administration, mice were individually housed in familiar small covered translucent plastic buckets filled with bedding. Sub-threshold extinction of FPS (Smith et al. 2011) consisted of the same protocol used during the PreTest on day 4. Between-session extinction learning (or extinction retention) was tested on day 8 using the same protocol.

The second FPS experiment tested the effects of chronic citalopram treatment on the effects of MDMA. This approach was identical in most ways to the previous experiment, except that fear conditioning occurred on day 1, followed by 22 days of treatment. Memory testing, within-session extinction training, and between-extinction (retention) testing occurred on days 24, 25 and 26, respectively. Startle chambers were cleaned with distilled water in between individual animal test sessions and then cleaned again with Quattricide at the end of each daily session. Separate chambers used for fear conditioning were cleaned with 70% EtOH. Startle was assessed in 100-ms blocks during the presentation of the startle stimulus. During acclimation periods, startle was assessed every 30 s. FPS was calculated by the following formula using average startle measurements across a session:

$$ \%FPS=\left({}^{``}CS + {\mathrm{startle}}^{"}-{}^{``}\mathrm{Startle}\ \mathrm{stimulus}\ {\mathrm{alone}}^{"}\right)/{}^{``}\mathrm{Startle}\ \mathrm{stimulus}\ {\mathrm{alone}}^{"\ast }\ 100 $$

Statistical analysis

Behavioral data were analyzed with SPSS 22.0 (IBM; Armonk, NY) and Prism 5.0 (GraphPad) using a two-tailed t test, one- or two-way ANOVA, or a repeated-measures ANOVA with α = 0.05. Post hoc ANOVA comparisons were made using Dunnett’s test for dose-response data and Bonferroni’s test for all other data. Data in figures are presented as mean ± SE. The data variance was similar between the groups compared in each statistical analysis using Bartlett’s test for equal variances.

Results

Pre-treatment with inhibitor of 5-HTT, but not with other transporter inhibitors, blocks MDMA’s enhancing effect on the extinction of conditioned freezing

Administration of a range of doses of citalopram (SSRI), reboxetine (NRI), or RTI-336 (DAT inhibitor) 60 min prior to fear extinction training revealed that only 20 mg/kg of citalopram reduced conditioned freezing during extinction training (Fig. 1a–c) [citalopram: F (3,28) = 11.22, p < 0.001; n = 8/group]. However, no effects of citalopram, reboxetine, or RTI-336 treatment were observed the following day when conditioned freezing was tested (Fig. 1a–c) [citalopram: F (3,28) = 0.18, p = 0.098; n = 8/group]. Ten milligrams per kilogram of each drug was used in all subsequent experiments because it was the highest dose administered that had no significant effect on conditioned freezing or fear extinction when administered alone 60 min prior to extinction training.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Effect of transporter inhibitors on fear extinction and MDMA-induced enhancement of fear extinction. Conditioned freezing during extinction training 60 min after receiving a dose of citalopram, reboxetine, or RTI-336 (ac; left). Conditioned freezing the following day (n = 8/group) (ac; right). d Total average conditioned freezing during extinction training in vehicle- or MDMA-treated mice that received vehicle, citalopram, reboxetine, or RTI-336 60 min prior to extinction training (n = 15 in vehicle pre-treatment groups; n = 9 in drug pre-treatment groups). e Total average conditioned freezing the day after treatment. *p < 0.05; ^p < 0.01, #p < 0.001

When all pre-treatment groups were analyzed together [n vehicle = 15; n treatment = 9], a main effect of MDMA treatment was observed [F (1,76) = 25.19, p < 0.0001]. Individual two-way ANOVAs were performed for each transporter inhibitor-treated group in comparison to the group that received drug vehicle prior to MDMA. Only RTI-336 treatment had a main effect on conditioned freezing [F (1,44) = 32.64, p < 0.001], but it did not prevent MDMA from reducing conditioned freezing (Fig. 1d) [RTI-336 × MDMA: F (1,44) = 0.039, p = 0.843]. Although both citalopram and reboxetine treatment prevented the MDMA-induced reductions in conditioned freezing observed in the vehicle-treated group, neither citalopram [F (1,44) = 1.47, p = 0.232] nor reboxetine [F (1,44) = 0.087, p = 0.769] treatment interacted with MDMA’s effect (Fig. 1d).

The following day, MDMA maintained a significant main effect on conditioned freezing [F (1,76) = 17.61, p < 0.001]. Post hoc comparisons within each pre-treatment group revealed that MDMA-induced reductions in freezing observed in animals treated with vehicle prior to MDMA [p = 0.001] were prevented when mice were pre-treated with either citalopram [p > 0.99] or reboxetine [p = 0.08], but not with RTI-336 [p = 0.03]. However, only mice treated with citalopram prior to MDMA exhibited significantly greater conditioned freezing than mice that received vehicle prior to MDMA [p < 0.001].

MDMA’s effects on fear extinction are blocked by chronic daily injections of citalopram

Daily citalopram administration for 22 days significantly altered the effect of MDMA on fear extinction. Extinction training was carried out 2 days after the final chronic treatment to avoid acute effects of treatment (Fig. 2a). During extinction training, mice treated with MDMA exhibited significantly less freezing during the first two sets of four CSs if they had not been chronically treated with citalopram. Chronic treatment with citalopram abolished this effect across trials [one-way ANOVA for CS1: F (3,36) = 31.46, p < 0.001; one-way ANOVA for CS2: F (3,36) = 5.59, p = 0.003; n = 10/group] (Fig. 2b). Chronic citalopram treatment also abolished the effect of MDMA on average conditioned freezing for the whole extinction training session [chronic treatment × pre-treatment: F (1,36) = 4.51, p = 0.041; n = 10/group] (Fig. 2c). Similar effects were observed the following day during extinction testing, where MDMA-treated mice that received chronic vehicle exhibited significantly less conditioned freezing during the first four CSs than groups treated chronically with citalopram or without MDMA [one-way ANOVA for CS1: F (3,36) = 3.95; p = 0.016; n = 10/group] (Fig. 2b). On average, across the whole of extinction testing, chronic treatment with citalopram blocked MDMA-induced reductions in conditioned freezing (Fig. 2d) [chronic treatment × pre-treatment: F (1,36) = 7.74, p = 0.009; n = 10/group]. Here, chronic treatment with citalopram alone did not affect conditioned freezing [F (1,36) = 3.49, p = 0.07; n = 10/group].

Fig. 2
figure 2

Chronically administered citalopram inhibits MDMA’s effect on fear extinction 48 h later. a Experimental model. b Conditioned freezing across groups of four CS re-exposures during extinction training (left) and testing (right) (n = 10/group). c Average total conditioned freezing to CS during extinction training. Chronically administered citalopram blocked MDMA-induced reductions in conditioned freezing. d Average total conditioned freezing to CS during extinction testing. Chronically administered citalopram significantly obstructed MDMA-induced reductions in conditioned freezing. e Experimental model. f Average total conditioned freezing during extinction testing. Acute citalopram treatment 24 h prior to extinction training did not alter MDMA’s lasting effect on conditioned freezing during extinction testing (n = 8/group). *p < 0.05; # p < 0.001

To rule out the possibility that residual citalopram remaining from the last day of chronic treatment was affecting fear extinction, a separate group of fear-conditioned mice was treated with vehicle or citalopram 24 h prior to extinction training and MDMA treatment (Fig. 2e). Treatment with citalopram 24 h before extinction training did not affect the persistent reductions in conditioned freezing observed in MDMA-treated mice during extinction testing (Fig. 2f) [F (1,28) = 17.56, p = 0.0003, for main effect of MDMA treatment; F (1,28) = 0.001, p = 0.970, for main effect of citalopram treatment; F (1,28) = 0.16, p = 0.696, for citalopram × MDMA treatments].

Transporter-mediated 5-HT release is insufficient to enhance extinction: a role for the 5-HT2A receptor

Administration of a range of doses of fenfluramine—a compound that selectively releases 5-HT through the 5-HTT—30 min prior to extinction training dose-dependently increased conditioned freezing during extinction training [F (3,28) = 3.46, p = 0.029; n = 8/group]. These effects were not maintained during extinction training the following day when mice were re-exposed to 14 CS tones, as animals previously treated with vehicle exhibited similar levels of conditioned freezing in comparison to all fenfluramine treatment groups (Fig. 3a) [F (3,28) = 0.48, p = 0.696; n = 8/group].

Fig. 3
figure 3

Effect of fenfluramine and the 5-HT2A receptor on fear memory extinction. a Total conditioned freezing to CS re-exposure during extinction training (left) and testing (right) when fenfluramine was administered 30 min prior to extinction training (n = 8/group). b Total number of head-twitches induced by the 5-HT2AR agonist DOI (1 mg/kg) 48 h after 22 days of daily vehicle or citalopram administration (n = 5/group). c Total conditioned freezing to CS re-exposure during extinction training when the 5-HT2AR antagonist M100 was administered 30 min prior to MDMA treatment (n = 8/group). d Total conditioned freezing to CS re-exposure during extinction testing the day after extinction training (n = 8/group). *p < 0.05; ^p < 0.01; #p < 0.001

Next, we explored the effect of chronic citalopram treatment on a behavior dependent on the 5-HT2A receptor (5-HT2AR). Mice were treated with the 5-HT2AR agonist DOI (1 mg/kg, i.p.) 48 h after the last dose of chronic daily treatment with either vehicle or citalopram. Mice treated chronically with citalopram exhibited significantly less DOI-induced head-twitch behavior across a 10-min observation period (Fig. 3b) [t(8) = 2.41, p = 0.042; n = 5/group]. To explore the role of the 5-HT2AR in MDMA’s effect on extinction, the selective 5-HT2AR antagonist M100 was administered 30 min prior to MDMA treatment. M100 (0.6 mg/kg) obstructed MDMA’s effect during extinction training (Fig. 3c) [F (1,28) = 7.69, p = 0.01, for main effect of M100; F (1,28) = 9.17, p = 0.005, for main effect of MDMA; F (1,28) = 1.72, p = 0.200, for interaction effect; n = 8/group) and also significantly attenuated MDMA’s effect on conditioned freezing 24 h later (Fig. 3d) [F (1,28) = 0.038, p = 0.846, for main effect of M100; F (1,28) = 4.39, p = 0.044, for main effect of MDMA; F (1,28) = 6.81, p = 0.014, for M100 × MDMA treatments; p = 8/group].

Chronically administered citalopram blocks MDMA’s enhancement of the extinction of fear-potentiated startle

All three doses of MDMA administered prior to extinction training FPS during the training session. One-way ANOVA analysis of the average FPS exhibited during each group of four CS presentations during extinction training revealed significant effects of MDMA treatment across the extinction training session (Fig. 4b) [CS1 F (3,60) = 13.95, p < 0.001; CS2 F (3,60) = 8.00, p < 0.001; CS5 F (3,60) = 6.05, p = 0.001, for main effects of treatment; n = 16/group]. Total average FPS during extinction training was also reduced by all doses of MDMA (Fig. 4c) [F (3,60) = 8.91, p < 0.0001; n = 16/group]. However, when retention of extinction was tested the following day, only animals treated with 7.8 mg/kg of MDMA exhibited significantly less FPS (Fig. 4d) [F (3,60) = 3.45, p = 0.022; n = 16/group]. Analysis of each individual group of four CS presentations during extinction testing revealed a significant effect of treatment only in the final four CS presentations (Fig. 4b) [CS10 F (3,60) = 0.0024, p = 0.002; n = 16/group].

Fig. 4
figure 4

MDMA’s effects on extinction of fear-potentiated startle (FPS). a Experimental model. MDMA was administered 20 min prior to extinction training. b Average FPS across sets of four CS re-exposures during memory testing (Pre), extinction training (day 7), and extinction testing (day 8). A 7.8-mg/kg treatment resulted in significantly less FPS during CS trials 1, 2, 5, and 10 compared to vehicle treatment (n = 16/group). c Total FPS in response to CS re-exposure during extinction training when MDMA was administered 20 min prior to extinction training. All treatment groups had significantly less FPS than the control vehicle-treated group (n = 16/group). d Total FPS in response to CS re-exposure during extinction testing. Only the 7.8-mg/kg treatment resulted in significantly less FPS than the vehicle-treated group (n = 16/group). *p < 0.05; ^p < 0.01; #p < 0.001

Chronic daily injections of citalopram (10 mg/kg; i.p.) for 22 days did not alter FPS induced by four CS presentations during a brief memory test on day 24 compared to vehicle (Fig. 5b). The following day, MDMA treatment (7.8 mg/kg) prior to extinction training significantly reduced FPS regardless of whether mice had been chronically treated with either citalopram or vehicle (Fig. 5c) [F (1,44 = 23.08, p < 0.001, for main effect of acute treatment; F (1,44) = 0.179, p = 0.674, for main effect of chronic treatment; F (1,40) = 0.403, p = 0.529, for acute × chronic treatment, n = 12/group]. When retention of fear extinction was tested in the mice the following day, reductions in FPS facilitated by MDMA were abolished in mice that had been chronically treated with citalopram (Fig. 5d) [chronic treatment × acute treatment: F (1,44) = 5.28, p = 0.026; n = 12/group].

Fig. 5
figure 5

Chronically administered citalopram inhibits MDMA’s effect on the extinction of FPS. a Experimental model. b Average FPS across sets of four CS re-exposures during memory testing (Pre), extinction training (day 25), and extinction testing (day 26) when MDMA (7.8 mg/kg) was administered 20 min prior to extinction training (n = 12/group). c Total FPS in response to CS re-exposure during extinction training when MDMA or vehicle was administered 20 min prior to extinction training. Both groups treated with MDMA exhibited significantly less FPS than vehicle-treated groups regardless of chronic treatment. d Total FPS in response to CS re-exposure during extinction testing. *p < 0.05; ^p < 0.01; #p < 0.001

Discussion

The acute positive effects of MDMA on mood and affiliative behaviors have long driven interest in its usefulness as an adjunct to psychotherapy (Greer and Tolbert 1986). We have hypothesized that the lasting efficacy of MDMA as an adjunct to psychotherapy for PTSD may result in part from its effects on the extinction of powerful fear memories (Mithoefer et al. 2011; Mithoefer et al. 2013). Using both conditioned freezing and FPS as measures of fear memory extinction, we demonstrate that MDMA-induced enhancements of extinction can be impaired by pharmacologically inhibiting 5-HTT. We propose that this effect is mediated by indirect inhibition of 5-HT2A receptors. The results of the present study support the hypothesis that MDMA enhances the extinction of fear memory, and also reveal an important role for serotonergic neurotransmission in this effect.

Our observation that administration of MDMA prior to extinction training prevented the expression of FPS during the extinction test is a critical indicator that MDMA improves extinction learning (Myers and Davis 2002), and supports previous observations where MDMA enhanced the extinction of conditioned freezing (Young et al. 2015). Though both freezing behaviors and the acoustic startle reflex rely on distinct neural pathways, the ability for fear memories to modulate both of these behaviors relies on the amygdala (Myers and Davis 2007). We have previously demonstrated that MDMA facilitates the extinction of conditioned freezing specifically in the amygdala, where it increases BDNF mRNA only after extinction training (Young et al. 2015). Interestingly, although expression of conditioned freezing and FPS follow similar trajectories of magnitude when measured simultaneously during fear memory retrieval, FPS appears to extinguish more slowly (Daldrup et al. 2015). We made similar observations in the present study, where extinction training to 16 CS in saline-treated mice resulted in greater relative reductions in conditioned freezing than in FPS. Therefore, it is important that we demonstrated an enhancing effect of 7.8 mg/kg MDMA on the lasting extinction of both conditioned freezing and FPS. Moreover, validation of MDMA’s effect on the extinction of FPS is important because it is frequently used as an index of conditioned fear expression and extinction in human investigations, where MDMA is believed to have clinical utility (Briscione et al. 2014; Norrholm et al. 2006).

It is interesting to note that, although administering MDMA prior to extinction training significantly reduces both freezing and FPS during extinction training and testing sessions, these reductions in conditioned fear did not predict lasting improvements in extinction learning. Acute psychostimulant effects of MDMA unrelated to extinction learning are likely to mask conditioned freezing and FPS during extinction training via DA and/or NE. Amphetamine, which releases DA and NE in a similar manner as MDMA, can also reduce conditioned fear during the extinction session without persistently improving the extinction of conditioned freezing or FPS (Borowski and Kokkinidis 1998; Carmack et al. 2010). 5-HT is unlikely to be involved in reductions of fear behavior during extinction training, as pre-extinction administration of fenfluramine dose-dependently increased conditioned freezing during extinction training. Although reduced fear expression during extinction training does not predict lasting improvements in extinction, those lasting improvements require that MDMA is on-board during the extinction session (Young et al. 2015). While it has been hypothesized that MDMA improves psychotherapy for PTSD through its acute positive effects on mood, these effects might be mitigated by MDMA’s anxiogenic effects (Bershad et al. 2016; van Wel et al. 2012). The findings here and elsewhere suggest that subjective or behavioral responses to MDMA during extinction training may not predict lasting fear extinction outcomes.

Although systemically administered MDMA potently increases extracellular levels of 5-HT in the brain, 5-HTT-mediated increases in 5-HT alone are unlikely to account for MDMA’s effect on fear memory extinction (Gudelsky and Nash 1996; Murnane et al. 2010). While citalopram treatment itself selectively increases extracellular 5-HT in the brain (Balu et al. 2013), we and others have observed that neither acute nor chronic treatment with citalopram enhances fear memory extinction (Burghardt et al. 2013; Gunduz-Cinar et al. 2016). Even more strikingly, acute treatment with fenfluramine, which potently releases 5-HT through the 5-HTT (Crespi et al. 1997), did not enhance fear memory extinction on its own in our study. On the other hand, SSRIs that have significant binding affinity at other monoamine transporters can enhance fear extinction in rodents when chronically administered between fear conditioning and extinction training (Gunduz-Cinar et al. 2016; Karpova et al. 2011). While disrupting MDMA’s access to 5-HTTs with the highly selective SSRI citalopram (Sanchez and Hyttel 1999) actually blocked MDMA-induced extinction enhancement, the fact that drugs that specifically increase 5-HT do not improve extinction suggests that pharmacodynamic mechanisms beyond acute 5-HT release are required.

MDMA affects a variety of neuromodulatory systems and targets in the brain that have been previously associated with fear memory extinction (Green et al. 2003; Singewald et al. 2015). MDMA increases levels of extracellular NE in the prefrontal cortex (Lanteri et al. 2014), where inhibiting beta-adrenergic receptors in the infralimbic sub-region prior to extinction training impairs the retention of fear memory extinction the following day (Mueller et al. 2008). Conversely, extinction retention is enhanced by direct administration of MDMA into the infralimbic cortex (Young et al. 2015). Given that reboxetine, a selective inhibitor of NE transporters (NET), inhibits some physiological and behavioral effects of MDMA (Hysek et al. 2011), it follows that reboxetine pre-treatment might also inhibit MDMA’s effect on extinction. However, reboxetine did not alter MDMA’s effect on extinction in the current study, nor did it affect extinction learning on its own. Indeed, completely ablating NE signaling does not affect the extinction of cued fear memory (Murchison et al. 2004). MDMA’s continued efficacy could be explained by direct binding of MDMA at beta-adrenergic receptors (Battaglia et al. 1988), blockade of which inhibits some of MDMA’s cardiovascular effects (Hysek et al. 2010). Inhibition of beta-adrenergic receptors has been previously demonstrated to inhibit the effect of NE on prefrontal cortical neurons believed to be essential for extinction learning (Mueller et al. 2008).

Although MDMA also releases significant levels of DA, other transporter-mediated DA releasers such as amphetamine have not been demonstrated to promote extinction learning here or elsewhere (Borowski and Kokkinidis 1998; Carmack et al. 2010). While one study reported enhancement of contextual fear memory extinction by the DA transporter (DAT) inhibitor methylphenidate, this treatment also increases NE via NET inhibition (Abraham et al. 2012; Kuczenski and Segal 1997). RTI-336 was used to block DAT in the current study because of its high selectivity for DAT over 5-HTT and NET (Carroll et al. 2006). However, DAT only accounts for some of the DA released by MDMA. For example, activation of the 5-HT2A receptor by MDMA appears to contribute to increases in DA release in the striatum (Yamamoto et al. 1995). Given that we observed that 5-HT2A receptor inhibition blocked MDMA’s effect on extinction, indirect DA release may in fact play an important role. One target of indirectly released DA that may be important to MDMA’s effect on extinction is the D2 receptor. Although MDMA has comparatively very low affinity for the D2 receptor (Battaglia et al. 1988), the D2 receptor is required for some of MDMA’s euphoric effects (Liechti and Vollenweider 2000a) and does contribute to normal fear memory extinction (Mueller et al. 2010).

Compensatory changes brought on by chronic SSRI treatment are likely to inhibit MDMA’s effect on fear memory extinction because chronic citalopram treatment blocked MDMA-induced extinction enhancements even 48 h after the final treatment. This is expected to be a sufficient amount of time to clear systemic levels of citalopram, which has an estimated half-life of 90 min in mice (Fredricson Overo 1982). Moreover, the dose used in the present study is almost completely eliminated from serum within 6 h (Karlsson et al. 2013). One consistently observed effect of chronic SSRI treatment is the downregulation of 5-HTT availability and function (Benmansour et al. 1999; Benmansour et al. 2002), which could diminish transporter-mediated release of 5-HT by MDMA. However, we also explored mechanisms besides 5-HT release, given that acutely increasing transporter-mediated 5-HT release with fenfluramine failed to enhance extinction. Pharmacological and genetic manipulations of 5-HTT consistently alter 5-HT2A receptor function in the amygdala (Bocchio et al. 2015; Jennings et al. 2008). In addition to its effects on 5-HTT, chronic SSRI treatment downregulates 5-HT2A receptor expression and/or function (Meyer et al. 2001; Sawyer et al. 2012). To determine the effect of our chronic citalopram treatment on 5-HT2A receptor function, we explored its effects on DOI-induced head-twitch behavior, which is mediated specifically through the 5-HT2A receptor (Dougherty and Aloyo 2011). Indeed, DOI-induced head-twitch behavior was significantly reduced 48 h after chronic citalopram treatment. Further supporting the requirement of 5-HT2ARs for MDMA’s effect on extinction, blocking 5-HT2ARs with the highly selective antagonist M100 (Kehne et al. 1996) prevented MDMA’s lasting enhancement of fear memory extinction. This is consistent with studies demonstrating that the 5HT2A receptor is required for MDMA’s behavioral and physiological effects (Liechti et al. 2000; Murnane et al. 2012).

We did not observe lasting impairments in fear memory extinction in response to 5-HT2A receptor antagonist treatment alone, suggesting that 5-HT2A receptors may be sufficient, but not necessary for extinction. While others have observed impaired extinction acquisition in response to 5-HT2A receptor inhibition, they did not explore whether extinction was permanently impaired (Slipczuk et al. 2013; Zhang et al. 2013). Conversely, the extinction of fear memory is consistently enhanced by 5-HT2A receptor agonists (Catlow et al. 2013; Zhang et al. 2013), which also diminish the expression of FPS (Hughes et al. 2012). 5-HT axon terminals and 5-HT2A receptors are densely expressed in the amygdala (Asan et al. 2013; McDonald and Mascagni 2007), where they are particularly important for regulating fear behavior (Bocchio et al. 2015). Moreover, targeting MDMA to the amygdala facilitates fear memory extinction and increases brain-derived neurotrophic factor (Young et al. 2015), the latter of which can be stimulated specifically by 5-HT2A receptors. Besides 5-HT2A receptors, MDMA does bind with notably weaker affinity as a partial agonist at 5-HT1A and 5-HT2C receptor sites (Battaglia et al. 1988). Interestingly, 5-HT1A agonists have been observed to acutely reduce the expression of conditioned fear (de Paula and Leite-Panissi 2016; Risbrough et al. 2003), while antagonists block the psychomotor stimulant and prosocial effects of MDMA in mice (Hunt et al. 2011; Morley et al. 2005). Given the proposed role of therapeutic alliance in the effect of MDMA on PTSD psychotherapy, it is possible that 5-HT1A signaling is important in the clinical context. In contrast, we have previously demonstrated here and elsewhere that fear expression during extinction training is irrelevant to lasting extinction improvements by MDMA (Young et al. 2015). Moreover, 5-HT1A antagonism did not inhibit MDMA’s effect in the amygdala and has limited effects on the cognitive and subjective effects of MDMA in humans (Hasler et al. 2009).

We hypothesize that chronic SSRI treatment blocks MDMA’s effect on fear memory extinction by downregulating 5-HTTs and 5-HT2A receptor signaling. However, it is unclear why acutely administered citalopram also blocked MDMA’s effect, when MDMA’s acute 5-HT-releasing activity alone does not appear to enhance extinction. One potential explanation is rapid de-sensitization of the 5-HT2A receptor response to MDMA following citalopram treatment. Systemic injection of citalopram can increase extracellular 5-HT levels in the brain 3–5-fold within 20 min (Balu et al. 2013), and in vitro studies have demonstrated that increasing extracellular 5-HT diminishes the 5-HT2A receptor response to subsequently administered 5-HT2A receptor agonists within minutes (Berg et al. 2001; Vouret-Craviari et al. 1995). Alternatively, acute and chronic citalopram may disrupt MDMA’s effect through distinct non-serotonergic mechanisms, such as the downregulation of NMDA receptor function in the amygdala by chronically administered citalopram, which would be expected to alter MDMA-induced activity important for extinction (Boyer et al. 1998). Another alternative hypothesis is that the effect of chronically administered citalopram on MDMA’s effect is due to withdrawal effects following cessation of chronic citalopram treatment. However, were this the case, we would expect chronic citalopram treatment alone to alter fear memory expression or extinction, which it did not. The interaction between chronic SSRI treatment and MDMA is of particular importance for translational purposes, given that SSRIs are a commonly employed treatment for anxiety disorders characterized by impaired fear learning (Koen and Stein 2011).

Although several treatments have been observed to enhance the extinction of conditioned freezing and/or FPS, most of these compounds have not had similar efficacy in human clinical populations (Singewald et al. 2015). MDMA, however, has been observed to improve psychotherapeutic outcomes in PTSD patients (Mithoefer et al. 2011). Although these studies used unstructured psychotherapeutic approaches, and not exposure therapy, the observations in the present study support the hypothesis that reduction of PTSD symptoms by MDMA-assisted psychotherapy is facilitated by enhancing the extinction of the fear response to memories for trauma. Indeed, improvements in PTSD symptoms are achieved by exposure-based therapies that rely on principles of extinction learning (Rauch et al. 2015). However, a portion of patients remain unresponsive to that treatment. We speculate that treatment-resistant individuals may achieve improvements in PTSD symptoms with the combination of MDMA and exposure-based therapy, with the important caveat that concurrent SSRI treatment may obstruct efficacy.