Introduction

In stock enhancement, released seedlings often suffer high mortality due to maladaptive behavior towards natural preys and predators [13]. Such behavioral deficiencies in reared fish can possibly be improved before release. Training has been considered one of the options to improve the quality of seedlings [46]. Through feed training before release, released fish can forage more effectively for natural foods in their living environment. Moreover, fish trained to respond adequately to a threat stimulus would be able to avoid novel predators.

Observational learning is the acquisition of behavior through the observation of other individual(s). For instance, nine-spined stickleback Pungitius pungitius learns food patch quality by observing the success of others [7]. Fish can acquire information more effectively by observational learning than no observed learning [8]. The observational learning has drawn attention as a training method for released seedlings in stock enhancement, especially for the conditioning of predator information [9, 10]. In practice, however, it is difficult to train fish by observational learning using a live model because of the limitations of time and space.

In this study, we propose training method by observational learning using video playback model. Video playback can be an effective tool of observational learning because it is easily repeatable in a limited space. Past studies have shown that fish can recognize conspecific and heterospecific fish in video playback as much as live fish in an adjacent tank [1115].

Whereas past studies have found that fish show certain responses to model fish in video playback, to the best of our knowledge, no study has revealed whether fish can acquire the information by observational learning of video model. The present study investigated the observational learning of video model in Pagrus major for feed training (Experiment 1) and avoidance training (Experiment 2). In each experiment, the observational trials were established as the following treatments: (i) live model observation treatment, where the observer fish was allowed to directly observe behavior of a live fish in the adjacent model fish tank; and (ii) video model observation treatment, where the observer fish observed fish behavior on video playback. Their learning processes for these observational treatments were compared with (iii) non-observing control fish in both experiments.

Materials and methods

Experiment 1: Feed training

Fish

Fertilized P. major eggs were purchased from Pacific Trading Co. (Fukuoka, Japan), and the eggs were stocked in four 500-L transparent polycarbonate tanks supplied with filtered seawater at the Maizuru Fisheries Research Station (MFRS) of Kyoto University. After hatching on October 13, 2010, larvae were provided with rotifers Brachionus plicatilis, Artemia sp. nauplii, and dry pellets (N400 and N700, Kyowa Hakko Bio Co., Ltd., Tokyo, and Otohime S1, Marubeni Nisshin Feed Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan), in accordance with growth. The water temperature was maintained at 24 °C using a heater and thermostat. Fish attained about 40 mm standard length (SL) on January 6, 2011. They could feed on enough amounts of pellets at one time at this size and thus suitable for this experiment. Fish SL was 44.6 ± 5.8 mm (average ± standard deviation) at the experiment, and there was no difference of size among live model observer, video model observer and control treatments (ANOVA: F 2, 14 = 1.15, P > 0.05).

Experimental tank

Four transparent polypropylene experimental tanks (length × width × height: 30 × 20 × 20 cm) were set in a room with a 12:12 h light/dark regime. These tanks were covered with black vinyl sheets, and seawater was filled to a depth of 15 cm with circulating filtered seawater. Each tank was used as model fish tank, live model observer tank, video model observer tank or control fish tank. The live model observer tank was located next to the model fish tank, and that of the video model observer faced a 26-inch waterproof monitor (Disign, Inc., Kanagawa, Japan). One of the long sides of each observer tank faced a model fish tank or a video monitor, respectively. The black vinyl sheets between each observer tank and model tank or video monitor were removable, and the sheets were used as blind sheets except for the observational trial.

A grey polyvinyl chloride (PVC) pipe (diameter × height: 3 × 2 cm) covered by a white PVC board was set at the center of each tank as a feeding base (Fig. 1). A grey PVC pipe (diameter × height: 2.5 × 15 cm) was placed on the feeding base. In experiments, three to five pellets (Otohime S1) were dropped on the feeding base through the pipe which prevented feeding of fish before the training. On a training, the pellets on the feeding base were exposed to the fish by removing the PVC pipe.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Schematic drawings of treatments in Experiment 1: a live model observer, b video model observer, and c control. A PVC pipe was placed on a feeding base at the center of the tank. The pipe was removed, and the pellets were presented to the fish

Model fish training

A single fish randomly selected from a rearing tank was introduced into the model fish tank. The fish was trained to forage the pellets on the feeding base. We defined a training trial as a sequence that the fish starts to forage the pellets on the feeding base after the removal of the PVC pipe. Afterwards, the PVC pipe was placed back on the feeding base. Training trials were repeated at 30-min intervals with this procedure the model fish was trained to feed on pellets on the feeding base within 30 s after PVC pipe removal in four consecutive trials.

For the video model, the feeding behavior of the model fish was recorded from the lateral side by a video camera (HDR-CX550, SONY Co., Tokyo, Japan); ten unique events of the model fish performing the task were recorded.

Observational trials

The observational trials were conducted for the live model observer and video model observer treatments (Fig. 2). A single fish randomly selected from a rearing tank was introduced into each model observer and control fish tanks on the day before the experiment. These fish were allowed to acclimatize overnight, and then a few pellets were provided before initiating the experiment. If the fish ate these pellets, the observational trials were started except for control fish. The blind sheet between each observer tank and the model fish tank or the monitor was removed 30 min before the beginning of observational trials; thereby, each observer fish was able to see live model fish or video monitor through the transparent tank wall.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Flowchart of the procedure in Experiment 1. The Experiment 2 was also conducted with the same procedure except for having 20 conditioning trials

The observational trial for the live model observer was a sequence where model fish foraged pellets on the feeding base after removing the PVC pipe in the adjacent model tank. In the video model observer treatment, the observer fish observed the above sequence on the video monitor. An observational trial lasted 1 min, and ten trials were conducted for both observational treatments, with 5-min intervals.

Test and conditioning trials

After the tenth observational trial, blind sheet was set between each observer tank and model fish tank or video monitor, and 30 min later, we tested whether each observer and control fish could respond to the feeding base without pellets as follows. The test trial lasted 60 s following removal of the PVC pipe. If fish pecked on the feeding base within the 60 s, the fish was considered to have learned about the feeding base. If it did not, the fish was considered not to have learned, and was then the fish was conditioned to forage the pellets on the feeding base after the test trial. Conditioning trials were provided in the same manner as the model training. Conditioning trials were repeated four times at 30-min intervals followed by the next test trial. A set of four conditioning trials followed by one test trial was defined as one session. If the fish did not forage the pellets within 30 min, the fish was considered to be under stress and was replaced by a new one. Two sessions were conducted in a day, and the experiment was repeated for a maximum of four consecutive days until the fish met the definition of learning, equivalent to a maximum of nine test trials. At the end of the experiment, fish body length was measured. Five replications were conducted for each observer and control treatments.

Analyses

The proportion of fish to have learned the feeding base was compared among live model observers, video model observers, and non-observing controls from the first to the ninth test trial, using survival analysis. In the survival analysis, the Cox proportional hazard model likelihood ratio test with the Breslow method was performed using the “Survival” package for R statistical software, version 3.0.0 (R Development Core Team 2013).

Experiment 2: Avoidance training

Fish

Hatchery-reared P. major juveniles, hatched on June 10, 2010, were transported from Miyazu National Center for Stock Enhancement to the MFRS. Fish were kept in 500-L transparent polyethylene tanks. The fish were fed as in Experiment 1, until December 26, 2010. Fish SL was 114.2 ± 6.7 mm (average ± standard deviation), and there was no difference of size among treatments (ANOVA: F 2, 17 = 0.05, P > 0.05).

Experimental tank

Eight glass experimental tanks (length × width × height: 60 × 30 × 36 cm) were set in a room with 12:12 h light/dark regime and were allocated for four conditions with duplication as model fish tanks, live model observer tanks, video model observer tanks and control fish tanks. These tanks were covered with black vinyl sheets, and seawater was filled to a depth of 25 cm with circulating filtered seawater. The live model observer tank was located next to the model fish tank, and that of the video model observer faced a 26-inch waterproof monitor (Disign, Inc., Kanagawa, Japan). One of the long sides of each observer tank faced a model fish tank or on a video monitor. The black vinyl sheets between each observer tank and model tank or video monitor were removable, and the sheets were used as blind sheets except for the observational trial.

A half-cut transparent polyethylene case (length × width × height: 15 × 20 × 20 cm) attached to a black polyethylene board (length × height: 30 × 20 cm) with a hole (length × height: 5 × 10 cm) was set in the experimental tank as a shelter (Fig. 3). A black PVC board (length × height: 7 × 30 cm) was set as a door in front of the hole to prevent fish from entering the shelter, before experiment.

Fig. 3
figure 3

A schematic drawing of experimental tank in Experiment 2. A sheltering area was placed at one end of the tank. On a conditioning trial, the fish was chased by a hand net from the opposite end of the tank towards the shelter

Model fish training

A single fish randomly selected from a rearing tank was introduced into the model fish tank. The fish was trained to escape into the shelter when chased by a hand net (length × height: 30 × 30 cm), after removing the door. A training trial was composed of the following sequence: the door was removed, and after 30 s, a hand net was introduced at the opposite side of the shelter and the net was left for 15 s; the hand net was then moved slowly to 22.5 cm from the shelter during the following 15 s. If the fish did not enter the shelter with this, the hand net was moved to 3 cm from the shelter until the fish escaped into the shelter. The escaped fish was allowed to stay inside the shelter for 5 min. If the fish did not go out from the shelter within 5 min, fish was gently forced out using a black polyethylene board. The door was placed back to the entrance for closing. Training trials were repeated at 30-min intervals; the model fish was trained to escape into the shelter from hand net within 30 s after removing the door for at least four consecutive trials.

For the video model, the escaping behavior of the model fish was recorded by a video camera (HDR-CX550, SONY Co., Tokyo, Japan); ten unique trials of the model fish performing the task were recorded. Video playback from the first to the fifth trial was recorded from the lateral side, and a recording from the oblique backward side was conducted from the sixth to tenth trial. This was because the observer fish might have difficulty in understanding the entrance to the shelter in a two-dimensional video monitor.

Observational trials

The same three treatments as in Experiment 1 were conducted. One fish was introduced into the live model observer, video model observer and control fish for each from the stock tank on the day before the experiment. These fish were allowed to acclimatize overnight. The fish were provided with observational trials 30 min after removing the blind sheet except for control fish. The observational trial for the live model observer was a sequence where model fish escaped into the shelter from the hand net within 30 s, in the adjacent model tank. In the video model observer condition, the observer fish observed the above sequence on the video monitor. An observational trial lasted 1 min, and ten trials were conducted for both observational treatments in 5-min intervals.

Conditioning trials

After the tenth observational trial, the blind sheet was resumed, and conditioning trials were started 30 min afterwards. Conditioning trials were conducted in the same manner as the model training.

For each conditioning trial, the duration from removing the door to escaping into the shelter was recorded as the escape latency. Ten conditioning trials were conducted per day with 30-min intervals for two consecutive days. This means that avoidance conditioning consisted of 20 trials for each fish. A single fish was used for one replication, and six replications were conducted for all treatments. Fish body length was measured after the experiment.

Analyses

The escape latency was used to evaluate avoidance learning; latency is expected to decrease as the fish learns how to avoid the hand net by entering the shelter. The escape latency from the first to the 20th trial was analyzed using generalized linear mixed models (GLMM) with the “lme4” package for R statistical software. The error distribution of response variables was fitted to the Poisson distribution, with restricted maximum likelihood parameter estimation. The two fixed factors were “trial” (1 to 20) and “treatment” (live model observer, video model observer, and control). We treated “individual” as random factor since individual fish were repeatedly measured. Tukey’s test was performed for “treatment” by general linear hypotheses (GLHT) using the “multcomp” package.

Results

Experiment 1: Feeding training

For the feed learning, the proportion of trained fish during nine test trials was not significantly different among observational treatments (Cox proportional hazard model likelihood ratio test = 0.03 on 2 df, P > 0.05; Fig. 4). This indicates that neither the live model nor the video model observer improved the learning efficiency compared to control fish which experienced no observation.

Fig. 4
figure 4

The proportion (%) of fish to have learned the feeding base in the course of nine test trials in Experiment 1: control (black square), live model observer (gray diamond), and video model observer (white circle)

Experiment 2: Avoidance training

The escape latency of the control fish was significantly longer than that of the fish trained by live model observer (Tukey’s test by GLHT for GLMM: Z = −13.73, P < 0.001; Fig. 5 ; Table 1) and video model observer (Z = −14.87, P < 0.001). There was no significant difference in escape latency between the fish trained by live model and video model (Z = −1.16, P > 0.05).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Average escape latency (s) in the course of 20 conditioning trials in Experiment 2: control (black square), live model observer (gray diamond), and video model observer (white circle). Bars indicate standard deviation (n = 6)

Table. 1 Tukey’s test by GLHT for GLMM. Pairwise comparison of avoidance learning capability between the treatments: live fish observer, video playback observer, and control

Discussion

In Experiment 1, 45-mm SL P. major juveniles did not improve their feed learning ability through the observation of conspecific individuals feeding, either in an adjacent tank or displayed on a video monitor. Therefore, it was not possible to evaluate the efficiency of video model for observational learning. However, in Experiment 2, the escape latency of 114-mm SL juveniles decreased through the observation of live model and video model, compared to non-observing control fish. This result shows that P. major juveniles can acquire avoidance and sheltering information by observing conspecific fish in video playback. The video model has been reported to work as effectively as a live model for other fish species and innate behavioral aspects [1117]. For example, a male swordtail Xiphophorus helleri shows courting behavior to a female displayed in video playback [16], and conspecific model in video playback would induce aggression behavior in Betta splendens [17]. In addition to these studies, the present study indicates that watching a video model can work for observational learning of avoidance information.

Past studies revealed that by observing a predation event on a live conspecific a fish can acquire information about predator threat without risking themselves [1820]. Watching a predation event on a video model, observer fish may be able to learn anti-predator behavior. Indeed, Johnson & Basolo [21] found that X. helleri recognized a predation event on a conspecific in video playback, and their mating responses were altered after watching the video. Observational learning for a predation event in a video playback should be studied to develop a practical training technique. Furthermore, the duration of such memory also has a high priority for further study to improve the efficiency of training in hatchery-reared fish.

The size of fish may have induced the different results of observational learning between Experiments 1 and 2. Our previous studies showed that learning capability in fish changes ontogenetically and between conditioned stimuli [22, 23]. We also found that the ontogenetic change of observational learning in T. japonicus coincides with that of social interaction [24]. Further studies using juveniles in several developmental stages are required to evaluate observational learning through video model on fish feeding behavior.

For establishing observational learning in fish, the appearance of model would be important [25, 26]. Using animation techniques, it is possible to manipulate the model appearance, e.g., size, color, and motion in video model. Fishes are reported to react to animated fish in video model just as to live models [27, 28]. Such image manipulation may play an important role in furthering investigations on the mechanisms of observational learning, and thus for the application of this technique in the practice of stock enhancement.