Introduction

Primary progressive aphasia (PPA) describes an uncommon syndrome primarily affecting language. The concept of PPA was first described by Pick and Sérieux in the 1890s and re-introduced in the modern literature by Mesulam [1]. It is part of an evolving understanding of neurodegenerative conditions that identifies a range of clinical syndromes affecting cognitive functions other than memory [2]. The diagnosis of PPA requires that an insidiously progressive language impairment be the primary cognitive deficit for approximately 2 years after symptom onset [1]. There should be minimal difficulty in other cognitive functions, including memory, visuospatial skills, and executive abilities. Non-neurodegenerative etiologies, such as tumor or stroke, must be ruled out.

Three subtypes of PPA are currently recognized: semantic variant PPA (svPPA), logopenic variant PPA (lvPPA), and nonfluent/agrammatic variant PPA (navPPA) [3]. These are summarized in Table 1. The classification of PPA into these three subtypes is a relatively recent development, and the terminology and criteria for these subtypes are still somewhat inconsistent across institutions. Clinical evaluations of aphasic patients are challenging because most instruments of cognitive assessment rely extensively on language. Furthermore, differences in testing methods across institutions create an obstacle to making comparisons across studies. Yet the reliability of these clinical diagnoses is of vital importance as the underlying pathologies of PPA subtypes are being elucidated and treatments for these conditions are emerging. The urgent need for more uniform diagnostic methods is underlined by a recent summary of clinical-pathologic correlations in 145 autopsied PPA patients from seven experienced, international institutions. The accuracy of predicting pathology on the basis of a clinical syndromic diagnosis is disappointingly low [4••]. A new consensus on terminology, diagnostic recommendations, and suggested testing procedures is soon to be published (Gorno-Tempini et al., Unpublished data), and hopefully, this will help resolve some of the discrepancies in the descriptions of these patients across institutions. The naming conventions used here reflect this consensus, and the currently recognized characteristics of these conditions are summarized in Table 1. Mesulam et al. [5] have recently proposed a quantitative algorithm for subtyping PPA that may be useful in standardizing diagnoses. Additionally, clinical diagnoses of PPA syndromes can be supplemented with the use of multimodal diagnostic criteria that include combinations of cognitive assessments, genetic biomarkers, cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers, and brain imaging [4••, 6]. In this review, we discuss the three PPA variants and important recent findings for each.

Table 1 Characteristics of PPA variants

Semantic Variant PPA

SvPPA is also referred to as “semantic dementia” (SD). This is a fluent variant of PPA associated with frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD). The hallmark of svPPA is impaired performance on measures that depend on intact semantics. Thus, patients with svPPA have difficulty with picture and object naming, single word comprehension, category naming fluency, and knowledge of the uses and features of objects [7•]. The impairment is greater for less familiar words and objects. Difficulty with semantics is also evident in spontaneous speech production, which frequently contains indefinite terms such as “that” or “thing” where more precise and meaningful words would be appropriate [8]. Comprehension of speech is negatively affected as well. In an investigation of diagnostic features, Kertesz et al. [9] identified the questioning of word meanings in conversation (e.g., “What is stapler?”) as the primary diagnostic feature of svPPA. In reading, these patients often exhibit surface dyslexia, pronouncing words strictly as they are spelled on the basis of letter-sound correspondence rules (eg, plaid is pronounced as played). In addition to language problems, patients may exhibit behavioral changes similar to those in the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia, although these symptoms often have a later onset [9]. Despite these problems, svPPA patients do relatively well on measures of grammar, phonology, episodic memory, visuospatial skills, and number knowledge [7•, 10].

There is disagreement over whether or not svPPA should be considered identical to SD, a variant of frontotemporal dementia. Mesulam et al. [11] make a distinction between svPPA and SD. Both syndromes have similar language deficits; the critical difference, according to these authors, is the presence of a visual recognition deficit for objects and faces (associative agnosia and prosopagnosia) in SD that is not prominent in svPPA. Some have pointed out, however, that svPPA patients exhibit mild visual agnosia when sensitive testing methods are used [12]. Furthermore, svPPA patients invariably progress to clear presentations of what Mesulam et al. [11] designate as SD, and both syndromes share a common pathology [13]. Therefore, most authors [3, 4••] have not made a distinction between svPPA and SD.

The defining cognitive deficit for svPPA (and SD) appears to be a deterioration of semantic knowledge [7•, 14]. Investigations of this impairment have important implications for theories of semantic memory, yet the exact nature of the impairment in svPPA is not fully understood. Given the apparent global effects of the semantic deficit in svPPA, some propose a single semantic system that supports conceptual representations in all modalities (eg, verbal, visual) and is referred to as “amodal” [14]. Localized damage to this amodal semantic system would explain the profound semantic impairment in svPPA. However, others point out that svPPA patients exhibit dissociations across conceptual categories. For instance, spared representations of number knowledge and music knowledge are observed [10, 15, 16], demonstrating that if an amodal semantic system exists, it does not support these conceptual domains. Additionally, some find that in svPPA, knowledge of concrete concepts with visual feature associations is often more impaired than knowledge of abstract concepts. Taken together with the relative sparing of number and music knowledge, it is argued that degradation of the visual feature representations of concrete concepts may be the primary semantic deficit in svPPA [17, 18•]. Many of the testing procedures for assessing semantic knowledge in svPPA rely on visual stimuli (eg, picture naming) or the ability to understand words with strong visual associations. The reliance on visually weighted information may hide a relative sparing of semantic information in other categories (eg, abstract concepts). This interpretation is consistent with a theory of semantic memory that is widely supported in the functional neuroimaging literature, which proposes that concrete concepts rely in part on modality-specific feature representations [19]. However, the visual concept relative to abstract concept impairment in svPPA has not been consistently observed [20].

Structural imaging analyses demonstrate that svPPA patients have cortical atrophy affecting ventral and lateral regions of the anterior temporal lobes along with the anterior hippocampus and amygdala [3, 18, 21]. This atrophy is often more pronounced in the left hemisphere than the right. Longitudinal imaging shows atrophy beginning in anterior left temporal cortex, spreading to more posterior and superior temporal regions, and often appearing in anterior right temporal cortex [22, 23]. Those who support the hypothesis that svPPA is characterized by an amodal semantic deficit propose that atrophy of the anterior temporal lobes affects a component of the semantic system that is necessary for integrating conceptual information and that atrophy in this region accounts for the deficit in svPPA [14], whereas those who hypothesize dissociations across conceptual categories in svPPA propose that atrophy of visual association regions in ventral temporal cortex degrades visual feature representations and is critical to the semantic impairment in svPPA [17, 18•]. Consistent with this, a recent study found a correlation between atrophy of ventral temporal cortex and an impairment for words with visual feature associations [18•]. Longitudinal progression of disease may eventually compromise auditory association cortex in dorsal regions of the temporal lobe as well.

Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) can reveal damage to white matter tracts that may not be observable in standard structural MRI. DTI analyses of svPPA patients have found evidence for damage to the white matter tracts connecting anterior temporal cortex with other brain regions: the inferior longitudinal, arcuate, and uncinate fasciculi [24, 25]. These findings suggest that damage to connections between regions of the language network may contribute to the language impairment in svPPA.

In Grossman’s [4••] summary of the autopsy-confirmed pathology of 145 recent cases of PPA, 36 were diagnosed as having the svPPA phenotype (Table 1). As is found for each of the PPA variants, there is no one-to-one mapping of clinical syndrome to underlying pathology. The most frequent finding in svPPA was FTLD with inclusions immunoreactive to ubiquitin and TDP-43 (FTLD-TDP), observed in 25 of the 36 cases (69%). Alzheimer’s disease (AD) pathology was identified in nine cases (25%), and a minimal number of cases exhibited FTLD with tau-positive immunoreactivity (FTLD-tau; n = 2). In recent imaging investigations, prominent left anterior temporal atrophy in svPPA has been associated with FTLD-TDP pathology at autopsy [21, 26, 27].

Logopenic Variant PPA

LvPPA, also referred to as “logopenic progressive aphasia” or “progressive mixed aphasia,” is characterized by slowed spontaneous speech output with frequent word-finding pauses and phonemic paraphasias. Grammar and motor control of speech remain intact [3, 28••]. These patients perform poorly on measures of auditory-verbal short-term memory, including sentence repetition, digit span, word span, and letter span. Comprehension of speech is impaired and does not improve for speech that is syntactically simple. LvPPA is frequently associated with the clinical characteristics of AD. Thus, these patients tend to have poorer episodic memory than patients with the other PPA variants. However, whereas AD patients have difficulty with single word meaning, it is claimed that lvPPA patients, early in the course of their condition, perform well on single word comprehension and other measures of semantics. The core impairment in lvPPA appears to be to the phonological loop function, a function of auditory-verbal short-term memory [28••]. This is said to account for the difficulty with repetition and speech comprehension, despite preserved grammar and single word comprehension. Nevertheless, as these patients progress, deficits with single word comprehension and sentence-level grammar do emerge; thus the term “progressive mixed aphasia” has been used to characterize these individuals [4••, 5].

The lvPPA subtype was first characterized in detail by Gorno-Tempini et al. [3]. Although lvPPA cases had previously been reported [1, 29], the Gorno-Tempini et al. [3] study initiated the consistent use of the term “logopenic” and established the identification of lvPPA (referred to at that time as “logopenic progressive aphasia”) as a third subtype of PPA. The lvPPA classification may correspond to earlier reports of progressive aphasia, including PPA with no specified subtype [30] and nonfluent variant PPA [31, 32]. As pointed out by Gorno-Tempini et al. [28••], the discrepancies in identifying and labeling lvPPA and similar syndromes stem partly from discrepancies in how the labels “fluent” and “nonfluent” have been used in the literature. Whereas lvPPA patients often fall into the “fluent” category in formal aphasia tests owing to spared articulation and grammar, clinicians not relying on formal aphasia tests may label them as “nonfluent” given their slowed speech and frequent word-finding pauses. Consistency in the PPA literature can be improved by reserving the label “nonfluent/agrammatic” for patients with speech sound production and grammar deficits and using the label “logopenic” (meaning “lack of words”) for patients with slowed speech but few speech errors or grammar deficits, as in the current recommendations (Gorno-Tempini et al., Unpublished data).

Several investigations have now demonstrated that lvPPA is associated with atrophy of posterior perisylvian and inferior parietal regions, typically more pronounced in the left hemisphere [3, 4••, 28••, 33]. This pattern of atrophy is consistent with the hypothesized impairment of phonological-loop functions, which are thought to be carried out in inferior parietal cortex [28••, 34].

Of the autopsy-confirmed cases summarized by Grossman (Table 1) [4••], 24 cases were diagnosed as having the lvPPA phenotype. The most frequent finding in lvPPA at autopsy was AD pathology, observed in 12 of 24 cases (50%). FTLD-TDP pathology was identified in nine cases (38%), and a small number of cases exhibited FTLD-tau pathology (n = 3). Consistent with the common association with AD pathology, many of the clinical features of lvPPA resemble language impairments observed in AD [35].

Nonfluent/Agrammatic Variant PPA

NavPPA is also referred to as “nonfluent progressive aphasia” or “progressive nonfluent aphasia.” This syndrome is most often associated with FTLD and marked by dysfluent and effortful speech, with hesitations, retakes, and errors in the production of speech sounds [3, 36, 37]. Speech rate (words per minute) in navPPA is one third to one half that of healthy speakers [37, 38••]. The average length of utterances is reduced in the connected speech of navPPA patients because their sentences are often less complex in structure, with a reduction in the use of dependent clauses and phrases compared with healthy speakers. NavPPA patients also make grammatical errors. For example, they may omit required determiners, and they may fail to produce appropriate subject-verb agreement [39]. Dysfluency may also stem in part from dysarthria and speech production errors known as apraxia of speech (AOS), which is defined as an impairment of articulatory planning [40, 41]. Over time, patients may reach the point of producing little more than minimally connected content words; eventually they may become mute. They also have difficulty with grammatical comprehension, primarily for complex grammatical constructions [42, 43], which, considered along with their poor grammatical production, suggests that a grammar deficit may be central to the syndrome of navPPA. Difficulty in naming is a feature commonly found in navPPA [37, 38••], although this is not as severe as in the semantic and logopenic variants of PPA, and it may be related to difficulty in the phonologic assembly of words [44]. Executive resources, notably working memory, may be impaired. However, single word and object comprehension are relatively spared [45].

NavPPA has many similarities to Broca’s aphasia, a syndrome resulting from stroke in the left hemisphere and characterized by effortful, dysfluent speech. However, agrammatism and phonological processing are typically worse in Broca’s aphasia than in navPPA. Moreover, unlike navPPA, executive resources are often spared in Broca’s aphasia [46]. Differences between these nonfluent syndromes may be related in part to differences in their underlying pathologies. Stroke produces an abrupt and focal loss of tissue that also significantly injures white matter areas; this contrasts with the insidiously progressive tissue loss affecting more extensive regions of gray matter in neurodegenerative disease.

NavPPA has some features in common with lvPPA, including impaired repetition of phrases and sentences, speech sound errors, and spared single word comprehension and object knowledge. Little work has investigated whether there are qualitative differences between navPPA and lvPPA in the nature of the difficulties encountered during sentence repetition and speech-sound error production. Descriptions of the two syndromes consistently remark on the presence of agrammatism and simplified grammatical structures in the speech of navPPA, contrasted with the relative sparing of grammar and the impaired word retrieval exhibited in lvPPA. Some authors group logopenic patients under the navPPA classification [37], but currently most view the logopenic and nonfluent variants as distinct syndromes. This differentiation is supported to the extent that a differentiation of underlying pathology has been established (Table 1).

There are differing views on whether the language difficulties in navPPA primarily reflect a motor impairment from AOS or a nonmotor language impairment. In a recent study of speech errors in navPPA, the large majority of speech sound errors (82%) were substitutions, insertions, deletions, or transpositions, all of which contain actual speech sounds of American English and likely represent incorrect retrieval of the desired sound [38••]. Only the remaining 18% of errors consisted of sounds that were not well-formed phonemes and thus are likely to represent a motor impairment in which the speech articulators are not optimally positioned or activated to produce a proper speech sound of English. Neither type of error correlated in frequency with speech rate or syntactic complexity, but both types of error correlated with grammatical correctness, consistent with other reports [37]. These findings suggest that AOS may occur within navPPA, but it is not the central cause of the slowed, effortful speech that characterizes this syndrome. Rather, a nonmotor language impairment, perhaps in phonological retrieval, seems to play a greater role in navPPA patients’ dysfluency. This may accompany a syntactic deficit affecting the sentence construction aspect of fluency.

Cortical atrophy in navPPA occurs predominantly in the left hemisphere in an anterior perisylvian distribution, involving inferior, opercular, and insular regions [3, 42]. As the disease progresses, atrophy extends to left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and left superior temporal cortex. It extends medially to involve orbital and anterior cingulate regions and spreads posteriorly along the sylvian fissure into the parietal lobe [6]. The impaired grammatical comprehension that is characteristic of navPPA has been found to correlate with atrophy in left inferior frontal cortex, in accordance with considerable evidence that this brain region contributes to sentence processing [42, 47]. Impaired fluency in terms of words per minute of spontaneous speech has been associated with atrophy in left interior frontal and superior temporal cortex [39, 47]. This association may reflect the role of a syntactic deficit in impairing the flow of speech. Speech errors associated with AOS in stroke patients have been localized to the anterior portion of the left insula in one study [48]. A contrasting study found a correlation of AOS with damage to left Broca’s area in acute stroke patients [49]. However, these findings in stroke patients may not be generalizable to a neurodegenerative condition such as PPA.

Of the autopsy-confirmed cases summarized by Grossman (Table 1) [4••], 85 cases were diagnosed as having the navPPA phenotype. The most frequent finding in navPPA was FTLD-tau, observed in 44 of the 85 cases (52%). AD pathology was found in 21 cases (25%), and FTLD-TDP was identified in 16 cases (19%). A minimal number of cases exhibited dementia with Lewy bodies (n = 2), dementia lacking distinct histopathology (n = 1), or mixed pathology (n = 1). Inferior frontal and superior temporal atrophy is seen in navPPA with tau-positive disease confirmed by autopsy [6, 21]. Parietal cortical atrophy on MRI and reduced functioning in parietal cortex on positron emission tomography or single photon emission computed tomography are seen in navPPA patients who have AD pathology [50].

Conclusions

The classification of PPA into semantic, logopenic, and nonfluent/agrammatic variants is now recommended for PPA diagnosis (Gorno-Tempini et al., Unpublished data). In the past, these subtypes of PPA have often been recognized under different names (Table 1). The systematic identification and investigation of these variants will be improved through adoption of the new recommendations and nomenclature.

Much work has already demonstrated that the PPA variants have distinct clinical profiles. Despite this, the accuracy of predicting pathology on the basis of clinical diagnosis is disappointingly low. In a recent summary of clinical-pathologic correlations, even the strongest correlation—between svPPA and FTLD-TDP pathology—was only 69% [4••]. The ability to accurately predict pathology will be necessary as trials for etiology-specific treatments become available. To this end, further work is needed on the development of multimodal diagnostic methods, combining neuroimaging, neuropsychological measures, and molecular markers from biofluids. These methods have the potential to significantly improve pathology prediction. For example, recent work demonstrates that combining neuroimaging with quantitative neuropsychological measures yields a powerful multimodal predictor for AD pathology in nonfluent forms of PPA [6].

In addition to characterizing PPA phenotypes, investigations of PPA have considerable impact on theories of language function. The nature of the semantic impairment in svPPA has important implications for theories of semantic memory. Similarly, investigations of navPPA inform our understanding of syntactic, phonological, and motor processes in the brain, and lvPPA patients may be an informative population for investigations of auditory working memory functions. There are still many open questions about the nature of the language impairments in each of the PPA variants. Future work can provide a more detailed understanding of these language impairments and their relationships to the underlying disease processes.