A recent review of epidemiological studies reported that the prevalence rates of depression in girls and boys between 13 and 18 years of age are 5.9% and 4.6% respectively (Costello et al. 2006). Not only is adolescent depression relatively common, it also is associated concurrently with high rates of comorbid psychiatric disorders (Kovacs 1996), significant psychosocial impairment (Birmaher et al. 1996), and increased risk of suicide (Rao et al. 1993). Adolescent depression is, moreover, predictive of a wide range of long-term psychosocial impairments including recurrent depressive disorders in early adulthood (Aalto-Setälä et al. 2002; Lewinsohn et al. 1999). The prevalence rates of adolescent depressive disorders likely underestimate the extent of the problem posed by depressive conditions because significant numbers of adolescents experience subdiagnostic depressive symptomatology (Roberts et al. 1990) and available evidence suggests that these youth present with many of the same social, clinical, and behavioural problems as do those who meet diagnostic criteria (Gotlib et al. 1995; Pine et al. 1999). These adolescents with subdiagnostic depressive symptomatology also are at substantially increased risk of developing depressive disorders (Gotlib et al. 1995; Pine et al. 1999). The prevalence and impairment associated with adolescent depressive symptoms clearly highlight the importance of research addressing mechanisms that place youth at risk.

One factor that has been consistently linked to depression in youth, and may play a causal role, is the level of depressive symptoms in parents. In an early review, children of depressed parents were found to be six times more likely to be diagnosed with major depressive disorder than children of non-depressed parents (Downey and Coyne 1990). Similar findings have been obtained in community samples of adolescents. For example, using a longitudinal design, Beardslee et al. (1996) found the duration of parental major depressive disorder, along with the parents’ total number of nonaffective disorders and the adolescents’ total number of prior diagnoses, to be the strongest predictors of the onset of affective disorders in the adolescents. When compared to depressed adolescents of non-depressed parents, depressed adolescents with depressed parents also have been shown to have higher suicidal ideation, and more recurrent depressive episodes (Essau 2004). This association between the depressive symptoms of parents and their offspring also has been observed in nonclinical samples (Hops 1992; Ge et al. 1994; Ge et al. 1995).

Evidence suggests that the intergenerational transmission of depressive syndromes is attributable, in part, to disruptions in interactions between parents and youth associated with depressive symptomatology (Gotlib and Goodman 1999; Radke-Yarrow and Klimes-Dougan 2002). This is not to minimize the genetic and contextual risks (Goodman and Gotlib 1999) that are shared by parent and child, or to ignore evidence that children with behavioural or emotional difficulties place a burden on parents that may contribute to their distress (Gartstein and Sheeber 2004; Patterson et al. 1982). Nonetheless, relative to nondepressed parents, depressed parents have been shown to exhibit a pattern of parent–child interaction that is less positive, less engaged, harsher, and more critical (Lovejoy et al. 2000; Radke-Yarrow and Klimes-Dougan 2002). These findings are largely consistent across age of child, measurement approach, and operationalizations of depression (Hammen et al. 1990; Gotlib and Goodman 1999; Lovejoy et al. 2000). Of particular relevance to this paper is evidence that depressed parents are rejecting and critical, and low in warmth when interacting with their depressed adolescents (Burge and Hammen 1991; Hammen et al. 2004; Muris et al. 2001). Notably, a meta-analysis (Lovejoy et al. 2000) indicated that the relationship between depressive symptoms and parenting style is not moderated by diagnostic status. This suggests that adverse parenting approaches are associated with parental distress, whether or not that distress reflects clinical levels of pathology.

In this paper, we focus on aspects of the parent–child relationship as mediators between parental and adolescent depressive symptoms. Several studies have indicated that the link between parenting variables and youth depression may be stronger in girls than in boys (Sheeber et al. 2002). For example, using a sample of 134 high-school students, Gamble and Roberts (2005) found that adolescent gender interacted with adolescents’ self-report of critical parenting experience to predict dysfunctional attitudes and negative attributional styles in the youth, with stronger relations among girls than boys. Similarly, Hale et al. (2005), using a large community sample of 1,329 adolescents, found that the links between perceived parental rejection and adolescent depressive symptoms were stronger for females than for males. In sum, from these studies, negative parenting attitudes emerge as one possible pathway through which parental and adolescent depressive symptoms may be linked, particularly for female adolescents. Unfortunately, given that these studies only assessed parenting from the adolescents’ perspective, it is unclear whether the observed relations, including the greater association observed between parenting variables and outcome for girls, could be due to source effects, with girls reporting more adverse parenting as well as more depressive symptoms and cognitions. Studies that include independent reports of parenting are thus needed to further clarify these findings.

One aspect of parenting that has remained largely unexplored in understanding the transmission of depressive symptoms from parents to adolescents is the role of parental attributions for adolescents’ behaviour. Attributions are the causal explanations that parents generate for their adolescents’ behaviour, and include the parents’ views of aspects of the adolescent’s behaviour such as the locus of control, the stability, and the controllability of behaviour (Weiner 1986). In much of the research on parental attributions, causal reasoning along these various attributional dimensions has been broadly categorized as negative or child-blaming versus positive or child-crediting (e.g., MacBrayer et al. 2003; Snyder et al. 2005). Overall, when parents make attributions regarding their children’s behaviour that are more negative (e.g., seeing misbehaviour as caused by factors internal to the child, controllable, and stable over time), they are more likely to respond with coercive parenting (Dix et al. 1986; Smith Slep and O’Leary 1998), and negative affect (Hakim-Larson et al. 1999; Smith and O’Leary 1995), and these parenting responses are associated with poorer outcomes in the offspring (Joiner and Wagner 1996; Miller 1995; Mills and Rubin 1990).

Numerous studies show associations between negative parental attributions and externalizing behaviour problems in young children, both in cross sectional and longitudinal designs (e.g., Johnston et al. 2006; Hastings and Rubin 1999; Nix et al. 1999). However, little research has explored how parental attributions may be associated with internalizing disorders or the role of parental attributions in adolescence. In this study, we focus on parents’ attributions for the behaviours of adolescents, and how these attributions may be related to depressive symptoms experienced by both parents and their adolescent children. From previous studies using both community and clinic-referred samples of young children, we know that depression in parents is associated with more negative attributions for child misbehaviour (Bolton et al. 2003; Leung and Smith Slep 2006). Specifically, Bolton et al. (2003) conducted interviews with 61 mothers of children between 4 and 11 years of age, and coded the causal explanations that mothers made for their children’s problem behaviours. These authors found that mothers with higher depression scores were more likely to perceive the cause of their children’s negative behaviours as internal to and controllable by their children. Similar results were obtained by Leung and Smith Slep (2006) in both mothers and fathers.

Although most previous studies have focused on child externalizing problems, a recent study of 48 mothers of 12–14 year old adolescents (Heatherington et al. 2007) revealed that mothers’ negative attributions for adolescents’ behaviour were related to increased mother–adolescent conflict. In particular, mothers who perceived their adolescents’ negative behaviours as resulting from more global and pervasive causes also reported having more conflicts with their adolescents. This finding, combined with results showing significant associations between negative parental attributions and negative parenting in families of children with externalizing problems, makes it reasonable to hypothesize that adolescent-blaming attributions, associated with maternal depressive symptoms, are related to depressive symptoms in the adolescent offspring. As a first step, therefore, we postulate that negative parental attributions are related to both parent and adolescent depressive symptoms, and, as one aspect of disrupted parent–youth interactions, provide one mechanism underlying the intergenerational transmission of depression.

The first goal of this study, therefore, is to examine whether negative parental attributions for adolescent behaviour are related to adolescent and parental depressive symptoms. Given the highly familial nature of depressive symptoms, we sought to determine whether negative parental attributions would be related to adolescent depressive symptoms after controlling for parental depressive symptoms, and further, whether negative parental attributions mediate the relationship between parental and adolescent depressive symptoms. Although most research in this area has focused exclusively on mothers, a previous study conducted on the larger sample from which the sample for the current study was drawn, has shown that depressed adolescents and those with subdiagnostic levels of depression experienced less supportive and more conflictual relationships with both their mothers and fathers compared to nondepressed adolescents (Sheeber et al. 2007). Furthermore, the variance accounted for by mother and father behaviour in predicting adolescent depression was unique, highlighting the need to consider the role of fathers in understanding adolescent depression. Therefore, in this report we include attributions made by both mothers and fathers to explore the potential effect of parent gender on the relations between parental and adolescent depressive symptoms.

Consistent with evidence reviewed earlier that the links between parenting and adolescent depression may be particularly strong for girls, the second goal of this study was to examine whether adolescent gender moderates the links between parental attributions and adolescent depressive symptoms. The gender difference in the prevalence of adult depression is well established, and this difference appears to emerge between early and middle adolescence (Hankin and Abramson 2001). One theory that has been proposed to account for this phenomenon suggests that girls begin to value interpersonal intimacy more than boys during adolescence as each gender becomes increasingly socialized into gender-specific roles, and as a result, adolescent girls are more vulnerable to the negative effects of stressful interpersonal events (Cyranowski et al. 2000). Given that parental attributions reflect aspects of the interpersonal relationships between parents and adolescents, it is likely that adolescent girls are both more attuned to the negative attributions of their parents, and more sensitive to the negative parent–youth interactions that may result from them. In sum, we hypothesized that because adolescent girls are more sensitive to interpersonal stress than adolescent boys, parental attributions that are hostile or negative toward adolescents will be more strongly associated with depressive symptoms in adolescent girls than boys. A visual representation of our hypotheses is provided in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Hypothesized relationships between parental depressive symptoms, parental negative attributions, and adolescent depressive symptoms. Dashed lines indicate that the relationships are predicted to be moderated by adolescent gender

Method

Participants

Description and Inclusion Criteria

Participants were 124 adolescents (76 girls and 48 boys) and their parents, selected from a larger sample of families participating in a study of family process associated with adolescent depression (N = 243; Sheeber et al. 2007). To be included in the larger investigation, adolescents had to be between 14 and 18 years old, live with at least one parent or permanent guardian, and meet research criteria for placement in one of three groups: depressed, subdiagnostic, or healthy. In order to provide a clear description of the sample selection and recruitment process, operational definitions of these designations are included in this report. However, it should be noted that diagnostic group is not a variable in this study, in which, as described below, depressive symptoms were treated as a continuous variable.

The depressed group included adolescents who both scored above an adolescent appropriate cut-off score on the CES-D (≥26 for males and ≥30 for females) during an initial school-based screening and met the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association 1994) diagnostic criteria for a current unipolar depressive disorder in a subsequent assessment. The subdiagnostic group included adolescents who scored above the cut-off on the CES-D scale at screening, but who did not meet diagnostic criteria for current or lifetime affective disorders, or current nonaffective disorders. The healthy group included adolescents who scored below the cut-off scores on the CES-D scale at screening, did not meet criteria for current or lifetime diagnoses, and had no history of mental health treatment.

Due to resource limitations, attributional coding was only completed on a subset of the larger sample. Because we were interested in examining the relations between attributions and depressive symptoms in both mothers and fathers, only two-parent families in which both parents participated were included. This approach enabled us to ensure that mothers and fathers were making attributions about the same child’s behaviour. Additionally, of families meeting these inclusion criteria, 21 were excluded because audiotaped attribution data were unavailable due to problems in taping. Twenty-seven percent of the adolescents included in our subsample were from the original depressed group, 36% were from the subdiagnostic group, and 37% were from the healthy group.

Recruitment Procedures

For the larger study from which our subsample was drawn, families were recruited and selected using a two-gate procedure consisting of an in-school screening and an in-home diagnostic interview. Students from area high schools participated in the school screening which was conducted during class time. To facilitate recruitment of a representative sample of students, we used a combined passive parental consent and active student consent procedure to inform families about the project and request their participation in the school screening (Biglan and Ary 1990; Severson and Ary 1983). Approximately 75% of eligible students (N = 4,495) participated. Participating students completed the screening CES-D, a demographic information form, and a contact form.

Research staff telephoned families of those adolescents selected based on CES-D scores obtained from the school screening and invited them to participate in an informational meeting in the families’ homes. At these meetings, staff described the project and obtained active informed consent from the adolescents and their parents. Following the consent procedure, interviewers (bachelor to doctoral level research staff) conducted the diagnostic interviews with the adolescents (Schedule of Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia—Children’s Versions [K-SADS]; Orvaschel and Puig-Antich 1994). Of families invited to participate in the diagnostic interview, approximately 80% (N = 537) consented. Of those subsequently eligible to participate in the full family assessment, approximately 94% (246) agreed to do so. Rates of decline did not vary as a function of CES-D score, group status, age, race, or gender. Greater detail on recruitment procedures is available in Sheeber et al. (2007).

Measures

Parent and Adolescent Depressive Symptoms

The CES-D is a widely-used, self-report measure that has respectable psychometric properties for use in both adolescent and adult populations (Radloff 1991; Radloff and Teri 1986; Roberts et al. 1990). It has been shown to discriminate between psychiatric inpatient and healthy control groups, and to correlate moderately with clinician ratings of severity of depression (Radloff 1977). It also has been demonstrated to have adequate sensitivity and specificity for detecting current episodes of depression and dysthymia in an adolescent sample (Roberts et al. 1990).

The CES-D was administered on two occasions. As noted above, adolescents first completed the CES-D during the school screening which served as the first-gate of the selection procedure. Adolescents and their parents subsequently completed the CES-D during the lab-based family assessment in order to provide a continuous measure of depressive symptoms. Data from this second assessment was used as the outcome variable in this report. The CES-D scores were continuously distributed and demonstrated adequate internal consistency for mothers (α = 0.93), fathers (α = 0.92), and adolescents (α = 0.94).

Video-mediated Recall Procedure

Each family engaged in two 10-min family problem-solving interactions (PSIs) in which they discussed and tried to resolve two identified areas of conflict (e.g., fighting with siblings; helping out around the house). The topic of greater conflict (as indicated by parent and adolescent report of frequency and intensity) was discussed second. Immediately subsequent to the PSIs, mothers and fathers participated separately in a video-mediated recall procedure (VMR). For this procedure, each parent simultaneously watched and responded to a videotaped replay of the second problem-solving interaction in private. We conducted the VMR immediately subsequent to the second interaction so that participants would have the clearest possible recall of the interaction. The video replay was paused automatically every 20 s and parents responded to two questions regarding their adolescent’s behaviour during the observed section of the interaction: (1) What is your child doing? and (2) Why was he/she doing that? Parents’ responses were audiotaped and coded for parental attributions. The video-mediated recall procedure has been employed in previous research to assess parental cognitions (e.g., Dadds et al. 2003; Johnston and Freeman 1997; Sanders et al. 1992), and has consistently shown sensitivity to group differences in parental attributions. Although the method is somewhat less standardized than the traditional approach of eliciting parental attributions from vignettes depicting child behaviours, it has the advantage of being more ecologically valid as it is based on actual behaviour displayed by the parents’ own child.

Attribution Coding System

Mothers’ and fathers’ responses to the questions “what is your child doing” and “why is he/she doing that” were transcribed verbatim. As parents often included attributions in their response to the question of “what is your child doing” (e.g., she was just not trying), the decision was made to code all instances of causal attributions in the responses to both questions. Causal attributions were defined as explanations that parents generated for their adolescents’ behaviour. In addition to reading the transcribed responses, coders listened to the audiotape of the parents’ response during coding in order that tone of voice and affect could be considered in coding the attributions.

The coding system used in this study was based on the systems previously used by MacBrayer et al. (2003) and Snyder et al. (2005). In this study, positive attributions were defined as attributions that gave the adolescent credit for positive behaviours, or excused or removed blame from the adolescent for negative behaviours. These included: (1) attributions that minimized a negative adolescent behaviour (e.g., she just got confused); (2) attributions that implied positive intentions or characteristics in the adolescent (e.g., he’s trying to understand our point of view); and (3) attributions that invoked external circumstances, the parent’s own behaviours, or the adolescent’s lack of knowledge to explain negative adolescent behaviours (e.g., she is still young; he stopped listening because I wasn’t clear). Negative attributions were defined as attributions that blamed the adolescent for negative behaviours or did not give the adolescent credit for positive behaviours. These included (1) attributions that implicated negative personality traits (e.g., she is lazy); (2) attributions that suggested the adolescent acted with hostility, indifference, defiance, or a significant lack of concern (e.g., she’s not willing to pay attention; he knows he can make his brother mad by saying that); and (3) attributions that invoked external circumstances or the parent’s own behaviours to explain positive adolescent behaviours (e.g., he’s only listening because that’s what he’s been told to do). Attributions that could not be unambiguously classified as either positive or negative were coded as neutral. For each interval, coders indicated the presence or absence of each type of attribution (positive, neutral, and negative), or coded the interval as “no attribution” if the parent did not offer any causal attributions in their response.

The team of coders consisted of the first and second authors, and three senior undergraduate students in psychology. Coders were trained in the use of the manual until they agreed on 80% of the intervals in practice transcripts. Once coding commenced, weekly meetings were held to review the coding manual and to discuss disagreements between coders. All coders were blind to the level of depressive symptoms in adolescents and parents. Forty-four percent of the transcripts were coded independently by two coders to evaluate reliability. To calculate the intraclass correlations, the attributions in each category were summed across intervals to yield four total scores. Intraclass correlation coefficients were computed between coders and were 0.90, 0.93, 0.96, and 0.99 for the positive, neutral, negative, and no attribution categories respectively. To control for differences in the absolute number of attributions that parents provided, percentage scores were computed by dividing the category total (e.g., total of positive attributions summed across intervals) by the grand total of attributions (i.e., total of all attributions summed across intervals). These percentage scores were used in all subsequent analyses. In this sample, the mean percentages of positive, negative, and neutral attributions made by mothers and fathers were 0.18, 0.16, 0.66, and 0.19, 0.16, and 0.65 respectively. Given that our hypotheses concerned only the relationship between parental levels of depressive symptoms, parents’ negative attributions, and adolescent outcome, only the percentage of parents’ negative attributions was included in the analyses.

Results

Preliminary Analyses

The means and standard deviations for demographic variables, adolescent depression scores, and mothers’ and fathers’ attributions are presented in Table 1. The adolescent CES-D score was missing for one subject. Inspection of distributions revealed that the CES-D scores for the current sample of mothers, fathers, and adolescents were all continuous. The adolescents’ CES-D scores had a skewness statistic less than 1.0. There was, however, a positive skew for mothers’ and fathers’ negative attributions, as well as mothers’ and fathers’ depression scores, with the skewness statistic exceeding 1.0. Squareroot transformations were therefore applied to these four variables. The transformations successfully decreased the skewness statistic for mothers’ negative attributions (from 1.10 to −0.16), fathers’ negative attributions (from 1.19 to 0.03), mothers’ depression scores (from 1.82 to 0.27), and fathers’ depression scores (from 1.74 to 0.34). These transformed scores were used in subsequent analyses.

Table 1 Means (SD ) of Demographic Variables, Depression Scores, and Parental Attributions, and Test Statistics for Gender Differences in These Variables

Preliminary analyses were conducted to investigate possible gender differences in demographics, adolescent depression scores, and mothers’ and fathers’ attributions. Boys and girls did not differ in their mean ages, parental level of education, or depression scores (all ps > 0.05). Similarly, there were no significant differences in the proportions of negative attributions that parents made for boys’ vs. girls’ behaviour (mothers t (122) = −0.49, ns; fathers, t (122) = 0.39, ns.) Mothers and fathers also did not differ significantly in their levels of depressive symptoms, t (123) = 0.84, ns. In addition, adolescent age was not significantly correlated with adolescent level of depressive symptoms, r (123) = −0.11, p = 0.21.

Correlations Between Parental Attributions and Adolescent Depressive Symptoms: Adolescent Gender as Moderator

Multiple regressions were computed to examine our hypothesis that the relationship between negative parental attributions and adolescent depressive symptoms would be moderated by adolescent gender. Regressions for mothers and fathers were computed separately. In these regressions, parental negative attributions were first centered to reduce multicollinearity with the interaction term. At step 1 of the regressions, the centered parents’ negative attributions and adolescent gender were entered as predictors of adolescent depressive symptoms. The interaction term was entered at step 2. At each step, we tested whether the model was significant and, at step 2, whether the incremental change in R 2 was significant. Results of the two regression models are presented in Table 2. For both mothers and fathers, there was a significant main effect of parental negative attributions and a significant interaction between parental negative attributions and adolescent gender. Specifically, for mothers, the full model was significant, R 2 = 0.13, F (3, 119) = 5.70, p < 0.01. At step 1, mothers’ negative attributions accounted for significant variance in adolescent depressive symptoms, R 2 = 0.09, F (2, 120) = 5.87, p < 0.01. At step 2, the interaction term between mothers’ negative attributions and adolescent gender accounted for significant incremental variance, R 2 change = 0.04, F change (1, 119) = 4.96, p < 0.05. For fathers, the full model was significant, R 2 = 0.17, F (3, 119) = 7.96, p < 0.001. At step 1, fathers’ negative attributions accounted for significant variance in adolescent depressive symptoms, R 2 = 0.11, F (2, 120) = 7.58, p < 0.01. At step 2, the interaction term between fathers’ negative attributions and adolescent gender accounted for significant incremental variance, R 2 change = 0.06, F change (1, 119) = 7.85, p < 0.01. Similar results were obtained from simple slopes analyses, with the slopes for mothers’ and fathers’ negative attributions being significant for depressive symptoms in adolescent girls (t = 3.77, β = 0.42 and t = 4.57, β = 0.47 respectively, p < 0.001) but not boys (t = 0.24, β = 0.03 and t = −0.25, β = −0.04 respectively, ns). These results confirmed our predictions and indicated that the relation between parental negative attributions and adolescent depressive symptoms was indeed stronger for female adolescents. We therefore conducted all subsequent analyses for boys and girls separately.

Table 2 Regression Analyses Examining the Moderating Effect of Adolescent Gender

Correlations Among Parental Depressive Symptoms, Parental Negative Attributions and Adolescent Depressive Symptoms

Tables 3 and 4 shows the bivariate correlations between parental depressive symptoms, adolescent depressive symptoms, and parental negative attributions. For girls, at the bivariate level, mothers’ depression scores correlated significantly with adolescent depression scores as well as with mothers’ negative attributions. Fathers’ depression scores correlated significantly with adolescent girls’ depression scores, but not with fathers’ negative attributions. For boys, the only significant correlation was between mothers’ and adolescents’ depression scores.

Table 3 Bivariate Correlations Between Parental Depression, Adolescent Boys’ Depression, and Parents’ Negative Attributions
Table 4 Bivariate Correlations Between Parental Depression, Adolescent Girls’ Depression, and Parents’ Negative Attributions

Predicting Adolescent Depressive Symptoms Based on Parental Depressive Symptoms and Negative Attributions

Given the lack of significant correlations between parental attributions and adolescent boys’ depressive symptoms, subsequent analyses were performed on data from families of adolescent girls only. Hierarchical regressions were used to examine whether parents’ depressive symptoms and negative attributions each contributed independently to the prediction of adolescent girls’ depressive symptoms. Hierarchical regressions were performed separately for mothers and fathers, with adolescent girls’ depressive symptoms as the outcome variable. At step 1 of the regressions, parent’s level of depressive symptoms was entered. At step 2, parental negative attribution was entered. At each step, we tested whether the model was significant and, at step 2, whether the incremental change in R 2 was significant. The beta weights for each variable at step 2 reflect the independent contribution of that variable, controlling for the other. The statistics for the two models are presented in Table 5. For mothers, the full model was significant, R 2 = 0.27, F (2, 74) = 13.23, p < 0.001. At step 1, mothers’ depressive symptoms accounted for significant variance in adolescent girls’ depressive symptoms. At step 2, mothers’ negative attributions accounted for significant incremental variance, R 2 change = 0.06, F change (1, 72) = 5.42, p < 0.05. Hence, both mothers’ depressive symptoms and mothers’ negative attributions contributed uniquely to the prediction of adolescent girls’ depressive symptoms. For fathers, the full model was significant, R 2 = 0.27, F (2, 74) = 12.95, p < 0.001. At step 1, fathers’ depressive symptoms accounted for significant variance in adolescent girls’ depressive symptoms. At step 2, fathers’ negative attributions accounted for significant incremental variance, R 2 change = 0.19, F change (1, 72) = 18.72, p < 0.001. Hence, both fathers’ depressive symptoms and fathers’ negative attributions contributed uniquely to the prediction of adolescent girls’ depressive symptoms.

Table 5 Regression Analyses Predicting Adolescent Girls’ Depression

Is the Association Between Parental and Adolescent Depressive Symptoms Mediated by Parental Negative Attributions?

Finally, the bootstrap procedure (Shrout and Bolger 2002) was used to test whether mothers’ attributions mediated the association between maternal and adolescent girls’ depressive symptoms. The bootstrap method uses a resampling approach to generate an empirical distribution for further significance testing. Thus, the bootstrap makes fewer assumptions regarding the distribution of the indirect or mediated effect and has been shown to produce more accurate confidence intervals (MacKinnon 2007; MacKinnon et al. 2004; Shrout and Bolger 2002).

With 1,000 bootstrap resamples, the 95th percentile bias-corrected confidence interval for the mediational effect ranged between 0.007 and 0.107. Given that this confidence interval did not include zero, the mediation effect was interpreted as statistically significant at the p < 0.05 level (Shrout and Bolger 2002). Of note, the direct effect of maternal depressive symptoms on adolescent depressive symptoms while reduced, remained strong (β = 0.25, t = 4.62, p < 0.01), indicating partial mediation of this effect. Similar results were obtained from repeated bootstrap analyses using 2,000, 3,000, and 4,000 resamples. Mediation analysis was not conducted for fathers given the lack of significant correlation between paternal depressive symptoms and paternal negative attributions.

Discussion

Extending the literature on family processes and adolescent depressive symptoms, this study takes a first step in examining the relationship between parents’ negative attributions for youth behaviour and adolescents’ internalizing problems. Specifically, we investigated whether parents’ negative attributions were related to adolescent depressive symptoms, whether this relationship was moderated by adolescent gender, and whether attributions mediated the link between parental and adolescent depressive symptoms. Our results indicated that, although parents’ negative attributions were related to adolescents’ symptoms of depression as predicted, this association was only significant for girls. This study also revealed differences in the pattern of results for mothers and fathers of adolescent girls. Specifically, whereas mothers’ negative attributions partially mediated the relationship between mothers’ and adolescent girls’ depressive symptoms, fathers’ negative attributions, although related to adolescent girls’ depressive symptoms, were not significantly associated with fathers’ level of depressive symptoms.

Turning first to the effects of adolescent gender, our results are consistent with previous findings showing that adolescent girls may be more vulnerable to the effects of interpersonal stress relative to adolescent boys. Previous studies have found not only that girls experience more interpersonal stress than boys during adolescence, but also that they become more emotionally distressed than boys in the face of interpersonal disharmony (Goodyer and Altham 1991; Rudolph 2002; Shih et al. 2006). Indeed, having a more positive self-perception in the domain of interpersonal relationships has been shown to serve as a protective factor against depression for adolescent girls (Eberhart et al. 2006). Given that parents of boys and girls did not differ significantly in the percentage of observer-rated negative attributions for adolescent behaviour that they generated, the fact that parents’ negative attributions were significantly associated with depressive symptoms only in adolescent girls’ suggests that the same negative parental cues are differentially related to child outcome in adolescent boys and girls. Although the smaller sample size of adolescent boys compared to girls in this study could have contributed to lower statistical power for detecting significant associations, the small magnitude of the correlations found for boys suggests that this is an unlikely cause for the lack of significant findings. It is important to note that, given the cross-sectional design of this study, the directionality of the associations cannot be discerned from these data. Thus, although these results are consistent with the hypothesis that the higher levels of depressive symptoms in adolescent girls resulted from their greater sensitivity to their parents’ negative attributions, one cannot rule out other causal possibilities.

Consistent with our hypotheses, mothers’ negative attributions partially mediated the link between maternal and adolescent girls’ levels of depressive symptoms. The fact that mothers’ levels of depressive symptoms were related to their negative attributions for adolescent girls’ behaviours is consistent with the results of previous studies conducted with younger children (Bolton et al. 2003; Leung and Smith Slep 2006), and contributes to the literature by highlighting maternal negative attributions as an important aspect of parenting that may serve to link parental and adolescent depressive symptoms. The results of our regression model show that, controlling for mothers’ depressive symptoms, mothers’ negative attributions accounted for 6% of the variance in their adolescent daughters’ depressive symptoms. Although this effect may appear modest, it is important to keep in mind that the significant correlation between maternal depressive symptoms and negative attributions renders this a conservative test of the potential influence of negative attributions. Furthermore, our results suggest that not only are mothers’ negative attributions related to their adolescent daughters’ depressive symptoms, these negative interpretations of the adolescent girls’ behaviours may constitute one of the pathways through which the transmission of depressive symptoms from mothers to daughters occurs.

With respect to fathers, our results indicate that, controlling for fathers’ depressive symptoms, fathers’ negative attributions accounted for 19% of the variance in their adolescent daughters’ depressive symptoms. That fathers’ attributions appear to be more strongly associated with adolescent depressive symptoms than were mothers’, likely reflects the unanticipated finding that fathers’ depressive symptoms were unrelated to their negative attributions, and hence, made a unique contribution. The absence of a correlation between fathers’ attributions and their depressive symptoms is in contrast to the results of Leung and Smith Slep (2006). One major difference between the two studies is that the parents in Leung and Smith’s study were making attributions for a much younger group of children (i.e., 3 to 7 years of age). Our impression, based on coding of the attributions, is that fathers’ attributions for the adolescents’ behaviour were related to general attitudes towards youth (e.g., the appropriateness of various adolescent behaviours) and were more closely linked to negative affective states of anger than to depressive symptoms (e.g., “he is arguing just to be arguing”; “so he can hopefully get us to shut up”; “she is trying to avoid what she needs to do by bringing it back to her sister’s issues”). Thus, although fathers’ negative attributions were found to be related to the levels of depressive symptoms in their adolescent daughters, fathers’ negative attributions were unrelated to their own levels of depressive symptoms, and may instead be related to a different set of parental characteristics than were mothers.

There are a number of shortcomings in this study. As noted above, the cross-sectional design does not allow us to draw conclusions regarding the directionality of the associations. Future longitudinal studies are needed to elucidate the directionality of these associations. In addition, the majority of the families included in this study were European-American, and of middle class, which limits the generalizability of the findings. Finally, we used a dimensional approach to assess both adolescent and parental depressive symptoms in this study, and further research would be needed to examine the extent to which these findings hold in groups based on adolescent and/or parental diagnostic status.

Nonetheless, this study makes an important contribution to the extant literature by investigating the link between parents’ attributions and internalizing problems in adolescents—an area of functioning and an age group that are both relatively unexplored in the literature on parental attributions. Furthermore, the use of an observational rather than self-report measure of parents’ attributions enhances the interpretability of the results as it reduces the potential confounding effects of method variance. Finally, given the dearth of studies on fathers and child psychopathology (Phares et al. 2005), the inclusion of both mothers and fathers in this study is a considerable strength. In hopes of guiding future research in this area, we highlight the need for studies to examine which paternal characteristics may better predict fathers’ negative attributions and which parental cognitions or behaviours may be more closely linked to depressive symptoms in adolescent boys. The findings of this study also point to the need to further examine the mechanisms that may underlie the link between parental negative attributions and adolescent girls’ depressive symptoms. For example, there is a need to explore the extent to which the influence of parental negative attributions is mediated by harsh and critical parenting behaviour and/or by adolescents’ perceptions of their parents’ attitudes and behaviours. Previous studies have shown, for example, that children who perceive their parents as more rejecting have more maladaptive beliefs about their own ability to produce desired outcomes (Rudolph et al. 2001). In addition, there is a need to explore other potential mechanisms that may underlie the findings of gender differences, such as whether adolescent girls may be more vulnerable to depression because they, more so than boys, internalize negative parental attributions or because they are more likely to adopt a negative cognitive style through modelling of parents’ negative cognitions. Bruce et al. (2006) have speculated, for example, that internalization may be one mechanism by which children develop negative self-concepts from negative parental feedback. Such internalizations of negative attributions or cognitive styles may thus serve as more proximal risk factors for the development of depression in adolescent females.

Clinically, these results highlight the importance for clinicians working with depressed adolescent girls to attend to the levels of depression in their parents, as well as to assess how the adolescents’ depression may be affecting or affected by their parents’ perceptions of their behaviours. Given that both mothers’ and fathers’ negative attributions were related to their adolescent daughters’ levels of depressive symptoms, it is important to assess both parents’ attributions whenever feasible. Clinicians should explore parents’ understanding of their adolescents’ behaviour, and be mindful of the degree to which parents are making child-blaming interpretations. The fact that parent–adolescent relationships have significant implications for adolescent depression is acknowledged by a number of current treatment approaches which incorporate intervention modules that address this issue. These include cognitive–behavioural (e.g., Wells and Albano 2005) and interpersonal (e.g., Mufson and Dorta 2000) interventions as well as treatments that are primarily family focused (e.g., Diamond et al. 2003). Attending to parental attributions for adolescent behaviour would thus be relevant in the context of these intervention sessions. Finally, as more information on the mechanism underlying the link between parents’ negative attributions and depression in adolescent girls emerges, it may provide the foundation for interventions designed to alter parental attributions in hopes of reducing adolescents’ depression and decoupling the transmission of depression from mothers to adolescent girls.