Introduction

Recent years have seen an onslaught of cases relating to corporate fraud and scandals, inflicting great pain on organizations and society. Against this background, an increasing amount of attention has been paid to the topic of ethical leadership in organizational research (Brown and Treviño 2006; Treviño et al. 2014). Being defined as “the demonstration of normatively appropriate conduct through personal actions and interpersonal relationships, and the promotion of such conduct to followers through two-way communication, reinforcement, and decision making” (Brown et al. 2005, p. 120), ethical leadership has been found to be positively related to a wide range of beneficial outcomes, including task performance, citizenship behaviors, ethical conducts, and so forth (for systematic reviews, see Bedi et al. 2015; Ng and Feldman 2015). Given these positive outcomes, both researchers and practitioners have engaged in identifying the antecedents of ethical leadership and thus have accumulated a considerable body of evidence (den Hartog 2015; Treviño and Brown 2014). Among several streams of research, there is one suggesting a “trickle-down” effect of ethical leadership, which argues that ethical leadership of high-level leaders could cascade downward and influence the ethical leadership of middle-level supervisors (Mayer et al. 2009; Ruiz et al. 2011a, b; Schaubroeck et al. 2012).

However, these accumulated studies on the trickle-down effect of ethical leadership are limited in a critical way. Although almost all studies have relied on social learning theory to explain the mechanisms underlying the effect, none of them has used social learning-related constructs to test this account. As noted in a recent article by Sumanth and Hannah (2014, p. 46), “in investigating the effects of ethical leadership of higher level organizational leaders on lower level leaders…though frequently theorized, we are not aware of any research that has actually tested social learning as a mediator in the cascading process.” The lack of such research results in an unclear understanding of the intermediate process through which the trickle-down effect occurs. Given this research gap, the current study intends to continue the momentum of research on the trickle-down effect of ethical leadership and advance further to examine its underlying mechanism, aiming to provide a better understanding of how ethical leadership cascades from high-level managers to middle-level supervisors within an organization.

Specifically, we propose ethical efficacy expectation and ethical outcome expectationFootnote 1 as mediators to disentangle the cascading process of ethical leadership. Social learning theory emphasizes the importance of individual’s cognition in the regulation of human behaviors and posits that “most external influences affect behavior through intermediary cognitive processes” (Bandura 1977, p. 160). In particular, the changes in efficacy expectation and outcome expectation are the most important intermediate cognitive processes that link external influences to behavioral changes (Bandura 1977; Manz and Sims 1981). Given that our focus is on the ethical realm, we concentrate on ethical efficacy expectation and ethical outcome expectation in the current research. Ethical efficacy expectation (i.e., ethical efficacy) refers to “individuals’ belief in their ability to mobilize the motivation, cognitive resources, and courses of action necessary to execute ethical behavior” (Mitchell and Palmer 2010, p. 92); ethical outcome expectation represents “individuals’ perception of the likelihood that an [ethical/unethical] behavior will lead to a particular [reward or punishment] outcome” (Ashkanasy et al. 2006, p. 450). Social learning theory holds that an individual’s efficacy expectation and outcome expectation are susceptible to the influence of role models (e.g., leaders) through a vicarious learning process (Manz and Sims 1981). Thus, it is reasonable to suggest that, the ethical leadership of high-level managers, who oftentimes are regarded as role models, will exert an influence on the ethical efficacy expectation and ethical outcome expectation of middle-level supervisors, which will in turn elicit their ethical leadership behaviors.

We tested our theoretical model using a sample from a large banking firm. Results generally provided support to our hypotheses. By conducting this research, we contribute to the extant literature in three ways. First, this study advances the existing research on the trickle-down effect of ethical leadership by delineating the underlying mechanisms. By considering ethical efficacy expectation and ethical outcome expectation as mediators, the present study provides a direct examination of the trickle-down effect of ethical leadership from the social learning perspective. Second, this study extends the research on ethical efficacy by providing further insights into its nomological network. In the early 1990s, Bandura (1991) suggested that moral/ethical beliefs could be applied to ethical situations, suggesting the existence of ethical efficacy. It is only recently, however, that a few studies have formally conceptualized and operationalized this construct and studied its relevance to ethics (Arnaud and Schminke 2012; Hannah and Avolio 2010). Even so, research on ethical efficacy still remains largely in its infancy, and thus warrants further investigation (Hannah et al. 2011). This said, the present study addresses this call and enriches our understanding of ethical efficacy. Third, this study also highlights the role of ethical outcome expectation and advances the literature by contextualizing it in the ethical leadership context (Ashkanasy et al. 2006; Treviño and Youngblood 1990), illuminating another important psychological mechanism that could explain the function of ethical leadership. Taken together, using social learning theory as a theoretical framework, our research aims to answer the question how ethical leadership of high-level managers trickles down to influence the ethical leadership of middle-level supervisors. Our research model is depicted in Fig. 1 as shown below.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Research model

Theory and Hypotheses

Manager Ethical Leadership and Supervisor Ethical leadership

Social learning theory is based on the idea that an individual learns by paying attention to and emulating the attitudes, values, and behaviors of role models (Bandura 1977). As to the learning of ethical conducts in organizations, although individuals can rely on organization’s formal regulations or norms to gain ethics-related knowledge, they can also learn ethical conducts via a modeling process, in which they look for role models in the organization and emulate their ethical attitudes, values, and behaviors (Brown et al. 2005). This modeling process not only applies to followers who regard their leaders as role models, but also applies to leaders themselves, who also learn from their role models. Indeed, as Brown and Treviño (2006, p. 600) highlighted, “followers are not the only ones who learn from models. Leaders learn from models too. By observing an ethical role model’s behavior as well as the consequences of their behavior, leaders should come to identify with the model, internalize the model’s value and attitudes, and emulate the modeled behavior.” Then it comes to the question who would be leaders’ ethical role models. A few studies have answered this question and suggested that top managers or top executives in an organization could serve as ethical role models for middle-level supervisors (see examples in Mayer et al. 2009; Ruiz et al. 2011a). The reasons supporting supervisors as ethical role models for frontline employees, as suggested by existing studies (Brown et al. 2005; Brown and Treviño 2006), could also be extended to the dyadic relationships between high-level managers and middle-level supervisors, in which supervisors become followers of high-level managers. Just like supervisors, having power, status, and the control over the reward/punishment system, as well as having personal characteristics such as being honest and trustworthy, high-level managers are legitimate, attractive, and credible to be regarded as ethical role models, from whom supervisors could emulate the ethical conducts (Bandura 1977). Based upon the above reasoning as well as the findings in several existing studies (e.g., Mayer et al. 2009; Ruiz et al. 2011a, b; Schaubroeck et al. 2012), we propose the trickle-down effect of ethical leadership of high-level managers on their immediate supervisors. By using high-level managers, we refer to managers whom middle-level supervisors work closely and communicate frequently with, but not those distant top executives. Our rationale is that ethical role modeling is a “side by side” phenomenon (Weaver et al. 2005), and middle-level supervisors have more chances to observe and imitate ethical behaviors of their intimate leaders (i.e., high-level managers), rather than those of top or executive managers (Brown and Treviño 2014). We propose that:

Hypothesis 1

High-level managers’ ethical leadership relates positively to middle-level supervisors’ ethical leadership.

The Mediating Effect of Ethical Efficacy Expectation

As an ethical cognition (Schaubroeck et al. 2012), ethical efficacy expectation (i.e., ethical efficacy) represents individuals’ confidence in their ability to execute ethical behaviors (Hannah and Avolio 2010; Mitchell and Palmer 2010). Unlike the trait-like general efficacy expectation which is more stable, ethical efficacy expectation is more state-like, which indicates that it could be influenced to change. Existing research has found that ethical efficacy expectation is susceptible to external influences (Fischbach 2015; Hannah and Avolio 2010; May et al. 2013) and could play an important role in ethics-relevant situations (Arnaud and Schminke 2012; Lee et al. 2015). In the present study, we consider ethical efficacy expectation as a crucial factor that contributes to the cascading of high-level managers’ ethical leadership. We analyze its role from two aspects as follows.

In one aspect, given that an ethical role model plays a critical role in developing and strengthening one’s ethical efficacy beliefs (Bandura 1991), we believe that high-level managers’ ethical leadership would exert a positive influence on middle-level supervisors’ ethical efficacy expectation. According to the social learning theory, expectations of efficacy are based on four sources of information: performance accomplishments, vicarious experience, verbal persuasion, and emotional arousal, and these four sources could be further influenced by different modes of induction (Bandura 1977). Bandura presented a framework in which many external factors induce sources of efficacy, which in turn impacts efficacy. For instance, he argued that participant modeling, performance desensitization, performance exposure, and self-instructed performance could lead to individuals’ performance accomplishments, which in turn increase their efficacy, while suggestions, exhortation, self-instruction, and interpretive treatments are inducements of verbal persuasion, which in turn impacts efficacy. That said, these four sources are not sub-dimensions of efficacy, rather, they are more like the determinants of efficacy and the mechanisms through which external factors influence efficacy. In our research, based on this “mode of induction → sources of efficacy → efficacy” framework and the function of ethical leadership as the mode of induction (e.g., participant modeling, suggestions), we theorized that ethical leadership would induce individuals’ performance accomplishments, vicarious experience, verbal persuasion, and emotional arousal, which would ultimately increase their ethical efficacy.

First, high-level managers’ ethical leadership can enhance middle-level supervisors’ ethical efficacy expectation through performance accomplishments. Ethical managers set high moral standards and hold their followers accountable for ethical issues through using proactive and effective measures (e.g., using reward systems, conveying ethics-related messages) (Brown et al. 2005). These tactics can ultimately help supervisors to achieve ethical performance and thus enable them to have more opportunities to obtain personal attainment and success in an ethics-related domain. Second, supervisors’ ethical efficacy expectation can be enhanced through their own vicarious experience (Bandura 1991). In other words, supervisors can strengthen their ethical efficacy expectation by vicariously observing what others do when being faced with an ethics-related issue. Ethical managers, given their legitimacy, attractiveness, and credibility, of course are the role models whom supervisors would learn from when facing an ethical issue (Brown et al. 2005). Third, verbal persuasion from ethical managers can ameliorate supervisors’ ethical efficacy expectation. Ethical managers usually would discuss business ethics or values openly with their immediate supervisors and encourage them to ask “what is the right thing to do” when making decisions (Brown et al. 2005). Through this communication process, ethical managers verbally convey the message to their immediate supervisors that ethical behaviors are appropriate and should be encouraged, hence enhancing supervisors’ ethical efficacy expectation (Bandura 1977). Fourth, ethical managers define success not only by outcomes, but also by the ways how the outcomes are achieved. This philosophy helps alleviate followers’ affective concerns (e.g., anxiety or stress) that usually relate to outcomes and thus enables followers to pay more attention to the ethicality of their approaches in achieving goals (Lee et al. 2015; Walumbwa et al. 2011). Additionally, ethical managers expect, appreciate, and support their immediate supervisors’ ethical behaviors. Such expectations, appreciation, and support may make supervisors feel proud and emotionally delighted when exhibiting ethical behaviors, thus enhancing their ethical efficacy expectation. Apart from the above four sources, Hannah and Avolio (2010) recently added another one, which involves providing and articulating organizational support measures that could equip leaders with the confidence to take ethical actions. By setting the ethical tone of the work unit, stressing the importance of ethical behaviors and using a reward system to incentivize ethical behaviors, ethical managers can establish a social environment that provides the necessary support for their immediate supervisors to adhere to ethical standards and engage in ethical behaviors. Such an ethical social environment could boost supervisors’ confidence to behave ethically, thus enhancing their ethical efficacy expectation (Schaubroeck et al. 2012).

In the other aspect, we propose that enhanced ethical efficacy expectation will motivate middle-level supervisors to exhibit ethical leadership behaviors. According to Bandura (1997), how people behave is better predicted by their beliefs about their capabilities rather than by their actual capabilities. The same logic has also been extended to the realm of moral behavior regulation by Bandura (1991), who contended that ethical efficacy expectation is a critical cognitive process for moral thoughts and behaviors. Ethical efficacy expectation instills in supervisors a sense of confidence in their capabilities to organize and mobilize the cognitive resources to attain moral performance and regulate their behaviors to meet inner moral standards, thus ensuring that they will engage in ethical behaviors even in an ethically adverse situation (Hannah and Avolio 2010; Mitchell and Palmer 2010). Extending to the case of middle-level ethical supervisors, given that they bear the “moral manager” responsibility to hold followers accountable for their ethical/unethical behaviors (Brown et al. 2005), ethical efficacy expectation will be more necessary for supervisors’ exhibition of ethical leadership behaviors.

Summarizing the above two aspects of theorizing, we conjecture that ethical efficacy expectation would serve as an intermediate factor linking high-level managers’ ethical leadership and their immediate supervisors’ ethical leadership. We propose that:

Hypothesis 2

The relationship between high-level managers’ ethical leadership and middle-level supervisors’ ethical leadership is mediated by middle-level supervisors’ ethical efficacy expectation.

The Mediating Effect of Ethical Outcome Expectation

Outcome expectation is the other intermediate cognition in social learning theory. Distinguished from efficacy expectation, which represents one’s perceived ability to execute a behavior, outcome expectation is one’s judgment concerning the likelihood of outcomes resulting from a specific behavior (Manz and Sims 1981). According to Bandura (1977), the consequences of a behavior, which inform individuals about the benefits of an appropriate conduct and the costs of an inappropriate conduct, can facilitate individuals’ learning from their role models. Extending this notion to the ethical leadership area, we consider ethical outcome expectation as a key factor in the trickle-down process of ethical leadership and argue that high-level managers’ ethical leadership can promote their immediate supervisors’ ethical leadership through shaping their expectations of ethical outcomes.

According to the social learning perspective, individuals form their ethical outcome expectations through two ways: (1) being rewarded for their own ethical behaviors or punished for their own unethical behaviors (direct learning) and (2) observing or hearing about the consequences of others’ ethical or unethical behaviors in the workplace (vicarious learning). The latter way, which stresses the role of models in the process of learning, is the primary learning approach (Bandura 1977). Having said that ethical leaders would serve as role models for supervisors to learn ethical conducts and given the above arguments on ethical outcome expectations, we reason that ethical managers would play a crucial role in affecting immediate supervisors’ ethical outcome expectations. First, being ethical role models for their immediate supervisors, ethical managers would lead them to believe that managers’ ethical leadership has been rewarded in the past. Indeed, as Bandura suggested, which was also noted in Weiss (1977), a role model’s (i.e., leader) attributes such as status, power, or perceived competence would lead observers (i.e., followers) to believe that the model’s behaviors have been rewarded in the past or are appropriate in the situation, and this information would affect observers’ expectations that engaging in similar behaviors would lead to eventual rewards. Second, ethical managers will shape immediate supervisors’ ethical outcome expectations through rewarding those who behave ethically and punishing those who behave unethically in the workplace (Detert et al. 2007). When supervisors observe the consequences of ethical behaviors and unethical behaviors, they will learn what is considered appropriate and inappropriate concerning ethical issues in the organization (Davis and Luthans 1980; Manz and Sims 1981). In other words, these social cues would convey a message to supervisors that behaving ethically is appropriate and will be rewarded, whereas behaving unethically is inappropriate and will be punished (Brown et al. 2005; Brown and Treviño 2006). Accordingly, these outcome expectations would facilitate supervisor’s learning in an anticipatory ethical manner and influence them to engage in more ethical leadership behaviors.

To summarize, the reasoning above suggests that the relationship between high-level managers’ ethical leadership and middle-level supervisors’ ethical leadership could be mediated by the ethical outcome expectations of middle-level supervisors. To examine this mediating mechanism in more depth, we further differentiate ethical outcome expectation into two types: ethical behavior–reward expectation, which refers to the perceived likelihood of getting rewards for ethical behaviors; and unethical behavior–punishment expectation, which refers to the perceived likelihood of getting punished for unethical behaviors. We propose that:

Hypothesis 3

The relationship between high-level managers’ ethical leadership and middle-level supervisors’ ethical leadership is mediated by middle-level supervisors’ ethical behavior–reward expectation.

Hypothesis 4

The relationship between high-level managers’ ethical leadership and middle-level supervisors’ ethical leadership is mediated by middle-level supervisors’ unethical behavior–punishment expectation.

Method

Sample and Procedure

We collected multilevel and multi-source data from a large banking company in China. This banking company has 120 sub-branches and 976 employees (including front desk clerks and back office staff) in total. At the most basic level of the bank, each sub-branch runs independently as a standard operating team that consists of one supervisor (i.e., middle-level supervisor in the organizational hierarchy) and several subordinates. Each sub-branch is under the control of a high-level manager from the corresponding regional branch of the bank. We chose to conduct the research in the banking industry for a few reasons. First, it provides a suitable context for researchers to do ethics-related research. The banking industry is more likely to induce employees to conduct unethical behaviors than other industries because it is characterized by higher stress and less regulation (Ruiz-Palomino et al. 2013). Although ethics in this industry has garnered tremendous public attention and urged government to adopt strict regulation measures, the number of fraud and scandals is still on the rise (Boatright 2010). As ethical leadership has been recognized as an effective way to restrain unethical conduct and elicit ethical behaviors in the banking industry (e.g., Ruiz-Palomino et al. 2011, 2013; Sims and Brinkman 2002; Walumbwa and Schaubroeck 2009), it would be meaningful to examine why and how ethical leadership behavior cascades downward to shape a collective ethical leadership culture in such an industry (Treviño et al. 2003). Second, although most banking firms have ethics-related policies, norms, or codes that regulate supervisors’ behaviors (Treviño et al. 2003), the boundaries between ethical and unethical behaviors in many banks are still quite vague, which makes supervisors more likely to learn from their direct leaders (i.e., high-level managers) about what is right and what is wrong in the organization. This said, the cross-level influences of leadership on employees’ ethics become very relevant a phenomenon in banking firms. Third, most banking firms have a typical hierarchical structure in which managers in charge of higher-level branches have control over resource allocation, goal setting, and reward/punishment system, while supervisors in charge of lower-level branches report directly to these managers. This difference in authority between higher-level managers and lower-level supervisors, along with the frequent interactions between them, meet the prerequisite of social learning process, making the trickle-down effect of ethical leadership possible in the context of banking firms.

Through personal social network and alumni contacts, we approached 79 sub-branches. Our research assistants communicated with the branch directors by phone and explained the purpose of the survey in the hope of obtaining their support. Three sub-branches refused to participate, resulting in 76 sub-branches that participated. To ensure the effectiveness of the survey, with the branch supervisor’ help, we distributed and collected the questionnaires in person. Date collection was conducted during the regular early meeting before business hours. Before completing the questionnaires, all respondents were informed of the research purpose and assured of confidentiality. To reduce common method bias, we administered two different sets of questionnaires, one for sub-branch supervisors and the other one for their subordinates. Specifically, sub-branch supervisors reported their perceived ethical leadership of high-level managers, their own ethical efficacy expectation and ethical outcome expectation, whereas subordinates reported their perceived ethical leadership of sub-branch supervisors. After completing the survey, each respondent forwarded his or her questionnaire (enclosed in a sealed envelope) to the researchers.

After excluding responses from seven sub-branches with only one or two subordinates, we finally collected a sample of 69 sub-branch supervisors (57.5 % of all sub-branch supervisors) and 381 subordinates (39.0 % of all employees). In this matched sample, the average number of participating subordinates per sub-branch supervisors was 5.52. Of the 69 supervisors, 47.8 % were women, 81.2 % had a college degree, the average age was 39.61 years (SD = 5.97), and the average tenure as a sub-branch supervisor was 2.33 years (SD = 1.22). Among the subordinates, 61.9 % were women, 79.0 % had a college degree, the average age was 31.49 years (SD = 6.37), and the average dyadic tenure with the supervisor was 2.32 years (SD = 1.14).

Measures

Except for ethical outcome expectation, all other measures in the study were originally in English. Applying the standard translation and back-translation procedure (Brislin 1986), we translated the English scales into Chinese versions. Unless otherwise indicated, the measures were rated by the respondents on a five-point Likert-type scale. The anchors for the scale were from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5).

Manager Ethical Leadership and Supervisor Ethical Leadership

To capture leadership in the eyes of beholders, we asked followers to rate the ethical leadership behaviors of their immediate leaders. Specifically, high-level manager’s ethical leadership was rated by middle-level supervisors (i.e., sub-branch supervisors); middle-level supervisor’s ethical leadership was evaluated by their immediate subordinates. This approach has been used in a number of leadership studies (e.g., Li and Sun 2015; Liu et al. 2012). We used the six-item version (Detert et al. 2007) of the ethical leadership scale (ELS; Brown et al. 2005) in this study (see Appendix). We chose to use this short version instead of the original 10-item version as we found that some of the items (e.g., has the best interests of employees in mind, can be trusted) in the original 10-item scale are redundant and have obvious content overlapping with some other constructs such as consideration and trust, and this has been pointed out by a few recent studies (Mayer et al. 2012; Yukl et al. 2013). We concurred with these studies and believed that we should use the most representative items for ethical leadership in our current study. Sample items for the six-item scale include “My leader defines success not just by results but also by the way that they are obtained” and “My leader disciplines employees who violate ethical standards.” As supervisor ethical leadership scores were essentially aggregated based on subordinates’ ratings in the multilevel analyses, we calculated the ICC(2) and mean of R wg for supervisors’ ethical leadership, which were .84 and .91, respectively, and were above the suggested cutoff point (Bliese 2000; James et al. 1993), supporting the aggregation for supervisor ethical leadership in the multilevel analyses. The Cronbach’s αs were .89 for manager ethical leadership and .93 for supervisor ethical leadership.

Ethical Efficacy Expectation

Ethical efficacy expectation was measured with a five-item moral efficacy sub-scale from the Moral Potency Questionnaire (MPQ) developed by Hannah and Avolio (2010).Footnote 2 Sample items include “I am confident that I can confront others who behave unethically to resolve the issue” and “I am confident that I can determine what needs to be done when I face moral/ethical decisions.” In the present study, the Cronbach’s α of the scale was .89.

Ethical Outcome Expectation

Since there was no existing scale of ethical outcome expectation in the organizational context, we developed the items based on the theory. Specifically, ethical behavior–reward expectation was measured through three items, including “In our work unit, those who behave ethically will be rewarded,” “In our work unit, those who follow ethical standards will be given priority in promotion,” and “In our work unit, those who follow ethical principles will be respected by others,” Unethical behavior–punishment expectation was measured via another three items, including “In our work unit, those who behave unethically will be punished,” “In our work unit, it will be difficult for those who violate ethical standards to get promoted,” and “In our work unit, those who violate ethical principles will be despised by others.” The anchors for both measures ranged from 1 (very unlikely) to 5 (very likely). The Cronbach’s αs were .90 and .73, respectively. To check the construct validity, we performed exploratory factor analysis using the principal component method with the number of factors not specified. The results based on oblimin rotation revealed the emergence of two distinct dimensions of ethical outcome expectation, with items loaded nicely on the two proposed factors, without any cross-loadings. The two factors explained 77.22 % of the total variance in ethical outcome expectation.

Control Variables

We controlled for middle-level supervisor’s age, gender, education, and position tenure. Age and position tenure were measured by the number of years. Gender was coded as 1 = male and 2 = female. Education was coded into four categories (1 = high school or lower, 2 = associate’s degree, 3 = bachelor’s degree, 4 = master’s degree or higher).

Analysis Strategy

Given the nested nature of the data, to account for potential non-independence effects, we conducted multilevel modeling to test the hypotheses. Specifically, we tested Hypothesis 1 through hierarchical linear modeling using HLM 6.08 (Raudenbush and Bryk 2002). Following the recommendations of Hofmann and Gavin (1998), we centered the predictor according to its grand mean in performing these analyses to control for multicollinearity.

To test the cross-level mediation effects (Hypotheses 2–4), we conducted multilevel path analyses using Mplus 7.0 (Muthén and Muthén 1998–2012). Specifically, we followed the method suggested in Zhang et al. (2009) and estimated the indirect effects based on a 2–2–1 path-analytical model (2 refers to variables at level 2, while 1 refers to variables at level 1). To examine the significance of each indirect effect we estimated, we followed Selig and Preacher’s (2008) method and conducted a Monte Carlo simulation (i.e., a form of parameter bootstrapping) with 20,000 replications, which provided an estimate of the confidence interval (CI) for each effect.

Results

Discriminant Validity Tests

Before hypotheses testing, we conducted a series of confirmatory factor analyses to examine the distinctiveness of the four supervisor-reported variables (manager’s ethical leadership, ethical efficacy expectation, ethical behavior–reward expectation, and unethical behavior–punishment expectation). As shown in Table 1, the theorized four-factor model provided an acceptable fit to the data (χ 2 = 186.32, df = 113, CFI = .93, TLI = .91, SRMR = .08) and showed a significantly better fit than the three-factor model (\(\Updelta \chi_{(3)}^{2}\) = 73.46, p < .01), the two-factor model (\(\Updelta \chi_{(5)}^{2}\) = 179.51, p < .01), and the single-factor model (\(\Updelta \chi_{(6)}^{2}\) = 432.75, p < .01). Given these results, the theorized four-factor model was superior in fit to all alternative models, and therefore, we can continue to examine these variables as distinct constructs.

Table 1 Results of confirmatory factor analyses

To provide further support for the discriminant validity of the constructs, we also computed the square root of average variance extracted, another widely used index for establishing the discriminant validity of constructs (Fornell and Larcker 1981). As was shown in Table 2, the square root of AVEs for all major constructs in our study was above .81, which was higher than any of the inter-construct correlations, suggesting that all constructs have good discriminant validity.

Table 2 Means, standard deviations, and correlations among variables

Descriptive Statistics

The means, standard deviations, and correlations among variables are presented in Table 2. As shown in the table, high-level managers’ ethical leadership was positively related to middle-level supervisors’ ethical efficacy (r = .56, p < .01) and unethical behavior–punishment expectation (r = .38, p < .01). Additionally, middle-level supervisors’ ethical efficacy (r = .62, p < .01) and unethical behavior–punishment expectation (r = .51, p < .01) were positively related to their ethical leadership (aggregated from subordinates’ ratings). In what follows, we develop a multilevel model to test the hypotheses.

Hypotheses Testing

Hypothesis 1 predicted that high-level managers’ ethical leadership would be positively related to the middle-level supervisors’ ethical leadership. Before testing this hypothesis, we examined whether there was significant between-group variance in subordinates’ perceived ethical leadership of middle-level supervisors. The results revealed a significant between-group variance [σ 2 = 0.21, τ 00 = 0.19, χ 2 = 404.69, p < .01; ICC(1) = .48, indicating that 48 % of the variance can be attributed to level 2], justifying the appropriateness of the use of hierarchical linear modeling to test the hypotheses.

To test Hypothesis 1, we estimated an intercepts-as-outcomes model in HLM 6.08, in which we used high-level managers’ ethical leadership (level 2 predictor) to predict the intercept of middle-level supervisors’ ethical leadership (level 1 outcome). As shown in Table 3, after controlling for middle-level supervisors’ age, gender, education, and position tenure, high-level managers’ ethical leadership related positively to middle-level supervisors’ ethical leadership (B = 0.40, p < .01). Thus, Hypothesis 1 was supported.

Table 3 Results of cross-level regressions

Hypotheses 2–4 proposed that middle-level supervisors’ three ethical expectations (level 2) would mediate the relationship between high-level managers’ ethical leadership (level 2) and middle-level supervisors’ ethical leadership (level 1). We tested these three mediation effects simultaneously in the same multilevel path-analytical model, in which there were three level 2 paths linking high-level managers’ ethical leadership and middle-level supervisors’ three ethical expectations (paths a 1, a 2, and a 3, respectively), and another three level 1 paths linking middle-level supervisors’ ethical expectations and ethical leadership (paths b 1, b 2, and b 3, respectively). The proposed mediation effects were examined by estimating the three indirect effects linking high-level managers’ ethical leadership and middle-level supervisors’ ethical leadership: a 1 b 1, a 2 b 2, and a 3 b 3, respectively. As shown in Table 4, the indirect effect of high-level managers’ ethical leadership on middle-level supervisors’ ethical leadership through middle-level supervisor’s ethical efficacy expectation was 0.13, with a 95 % Monte Carlo CI being [0.01, 0.26], which did not include zero, suggesting that the indirect effect was significant, supporting Hypothesis 2. The indirect effect through unethical behavior–punishment expectation was also significant (estimate = 0.09, 95 % Monte Carlo CI [0.01, 0.18]). Thus, Hypothesis 4 was supported. However, the indirect effect through ethical behavior–reward expectation was not significant (estimate = −0.02, 95 % Monte Carlo CI [−0.07, 0.01]), which did not support Hypothesis 3.

Table 4 Results of the mediating effects of ethical expectations

Discussion

Although past research has recognized the trickle-down effect of ethical leadership, the underlying mechanism delineating the cascading process still remains largely unclear. Building upon social learning theory, which suggests that a role model influences an observer’s behaviors through shaping his or her cognitive expectations (Bandura 1977; Manz and Sims 1981), we proposed and tested an integrative dual-process model in which the trickle-down effect of high-level managers’ ethical leadership on middle-level supervisors’ ethical leadership was mediated by middle-level supervisors’ cognitive expectations about their ethical efficacy and the potential outcomes of their ethical or unethical behaviors. Results from a survey study of 69 middle-level supervisors and 381 subordinates from a large banking company largely supported the model we proposed. We discuss the implications, limitations, and future directions of our findings as follows.

Theoretical Implications

First, through exploring the mediating roles of ethical expectations, we disentangled the intermediate psychological processes underlying the trickle-down effect of ethical leadership. Resonating with several recent studies on ethical leadership (Mayer et al. 2009; Ruiz et al. 2011a, b; Schaubroeck et al. 2012), our study also revealed a positive relationship between high-level managers’ ethical leadership and middle-level supervisors’ ethical leadership, providing further support for this trickle-down effect of ethical leadership. Going beyond that, the significant mediating effects of ethical expectations as we found in the current study clearly delineated the underlying mechanism of how ethical leadership cascades downward in an organization. Specifically, we found that high-level managers’ ethical leadership could facilitate middle-level supervisors’ exhibition of ethical leadership behaviors through influencing their ethical efficacy expectation and unethical behavior–punishment expectation. According to social learning theory, efficacy expectation and outcome expectation are the two major cognitive processes influencing human functioning (Bandura 2012; Manz and Sims 1981). Our research extended the expectation-related perspectives of social learning theory into the ethical leadership context and thus provided a novel and meaningful understanding of the social learning process of ethical leadership. As theorized in the current study, ethical efficacy expectations instill followers with a “can-do” motivation for their ethical behavior regulations, while ethical outcome expectations instill them with a “reason-to-do” motivation. The findings of our research, to some extent, provided direct support for the well-recognized social learning account of the trickle-down effect of ethical leadership.

Second, we contributed to the growing, yet still limited, body of literature on ethical efficacy. To explain an individual’s ethical behaviors, earlier studies have highlighted the importance of moral judgment (Blasi 1980; Kohlberg 1969). The association between moral judgment and ethical behaviors, however, remains weak and inconsistent. This is also called as the “judgment-action gap” (Jennings et al. 2015; Walker 2004). As such, an individual’s judging a situation as ethical or unethical and understanding of what should be done and what should not be done does not necessarily translate into ethical behaviors. In this sense, understanding how to motivate ethical behaviors is critically important (Treviño et al. 2006). Defined as individuals’ beliefs about their abilities to execute ethical behaviors, ethical efficacy is a relevant construct that could help address the “judgment-behavior gap” (Hannah and Avolio 2010; Hannah et al. 2011; Mitchell and Palmer 2010; Sweeney et al. 2015). However, despite its close relevance with ethical behaviors, insofar only a few studies have investigated the role of ethical efficacy in the ethics domain. For instance, Schaubroeck et al. (2012) found that ethical leadership could influence followers’ ethical efficacy through ethical culture. Lee et al.’s (2015) recent research found that followers’ ethical efficacy could mediate the effect of ethical leadership on followers’ moral voice behaviors. By providing support for the intermediary role of ethical efficacy in the trickle-down process of high-level managers’ ethical leadership, the current research added to the ongoing exploration of ethical efficacy in the organizational ethics context, advancing the knowledge of ethical efficacy’s antecedents and consequences. However, it should also be noted that the research on ethical efficacy is still in its infancy, and thus more research is needed to further the understanding of this construct.

Third, we contributed to the research on ethical outcome expectation by extending it to the area of ethical leadership. Despite its important role in the regulation of moral behaviors (Ashkanasy et al. 2006; Treviño and Youngblood 1990), expectations of ethical outcomes received little attention from researchers. Drawing on earlier work by Treviño and Youngblood (1990), we took a pioneering step to differentiate two important types of ethical outcome expectations: the expectation of getting rewards for ethical behaviors and the expectation of getting punished for unethical conducts. Furthermore, we found evidence supporting the intermediary role of middle-level supervisor’s unethical behavior–punishment expectation in the cascading process of high-level managers’ ethical leadership, which suggested that ethical leaders could influence their followers to become ethical leaders through shaping their expectations that behaving unethically would be punished. However, the study lent no support to the mediating effect of ethical behavior–reward expectation. We argue that there might be three possible reasons for this unsupported effect. First, prior research suggested that, in the short term, using rewards to elicit ethical behaviors may be frustrating. As posited by Treviño and Brown (2004, p. 79), “Can we really reward ethical behavior? In the short term, we probably cannot. For the most part, ethical behavior is simply expected, and people don’t expect or want to be rewarded for doing their jobs the right way.” Second, it has been posited that a transactional leadership approach, characterized by managing by exception and contingent reinforcement, is more likely to draw followers’ attention to oughts, duties, and losses (Kark and van Dijk 2007), thus making them more sensitive to behaviors that they should not do. Given that ethical leaders usually use a transactional approach to hold followers accountable (Brown et al. 2005; Treviño and Brown 2014), it could be reasoned that supervisors working under ethical managers might pay more attention to losses, such as punishments for behaving unethically, rather than to gains, such as rewards for behaving ethically. In indirect support of our finding, Shao et al. (2011) suggested that ethical leadership is more likely to induce employees’ ethical prevention focus, a construct that to some extent is similar to unethical behavior–punishment expectation. Finally, the unsupported mediating role of ethical behavior–reward expectation might also be attributed to the measurement of ethical leadership in the current research. Although Brown et al. (2005) contended that ethical leaders use rewards and punishments to hold followers accountable for their conducts, the ELS (Brown et al. 2005; Detert et al. 2007) we used only captured the “punishment” component (e.g., disciplining employees who violate ethical standards), yet neglected the “reward” component.

Fourth, as a reply to a recent study by Wo et al. (2015), the present study defended the legitimacy of using social learning theory to explain trickle-down effects in organization studies. Wo et al. explored the mechanisms underlying the cascading process of interactional justice perception through three perspectives: social learning, social exchange, and displaced aggression. Through two studies, they found no support for the social learning perspective, thereby casting doubt on its validity in explaining the trickle-down effect. Responding to their call for a re-evaluation of the social learning perspective, we used ethical efficacy expectation and ethical outcome expectation, two core variables rooted in social learning theory, to examine the validity of this theory in explaining the cascading effect of ethical leadership. Based on our findings, the current research defended the legitimacy of using social learning theory in explaining the trickle-down effect. Indeed, different theories may have different power in explaining the trickle-down process of an organizational phenomenon. According to our observations, when studying the trickle-down effect of behaviors (e.g., leadership behaviors, citizenship behaviors, creativity), researchers primarily used social learning theory (e.g., Jaussi and Dionne 2003; Li and Sun 2015; Liu et al. 2012; Yaffe and Kark 2011); when explaining the trickle-down effect of exchange relationships (e.g., leader–member exchange, perceived organizational support) or justice perceptions (e.g., Aryee et al. 2007; Tepper and Taylor 2003; Venkataramani et al. 2010), social exchange theory was mostly used; the displaced aggression perspective is mainly adopted to account for the cascading effect of negative leaderships (e.g., abusive supervision) and injustice perception (e.g., Aryee et al. 2007; Hoobler and Brass 2006; Tepper et al. 2006). Taken together, it could be reasoned that social exchange theory and displaced aggression perspective might have stronger power than social learning theory in explaining the trickle-down effect of justice perceptions, as examined in Wo et al.’s study; yet when it comes to the cascading effect of ethical leadership behaviors, social learning theory might be a more suitable perspective.

Finally, from the results of our data analyses, we found some effects relating to the control variables of our study interesting and we believe these small yet interesting findings could indicate potential research questions to be examined in the future. As was shown in Table 3, supervisor gender was positively (B = .27, p < .05) associated with their ethical efficacy expectation, such that females tend to have higher ethical efficacy scores than males. Given that existing research suggested that women should be more likely to conduct ethical behaviors and avoid unethical behaviors than men (see Franke et al. 1997; Kish-Gephart et al. 2010; Pan and Sparks 2012 for meta-analytical reviews) and men are more than two times as likely as women to engage in actions regarded as unethical (Betz et al. 1989), the finding that women have higher belief in their ability to execute ethical behaviors is not surprising. Besides, supervisor’s level of education was negatively (B = −.36, p < .05) related to supervisors’ ethical behavior–reward expectation yet positively (B = .22, p < .05) associated with supervisors’ unethical behavior–punishment expectation. A possible explanation could be that those highly educated people, due to their higher levels of morality or their higher levels of conscience, may view conducting ethical behaviors as something normal in their life. Under this circumstance, they may not expect any rewards for their good deeds and sometimes, they might even dislike the rewards given for good deeds. However, in terms of unethical behaviors, highly educated individuals might be more likely to have a strong belief in norms or principles, which essentially requires a punishment for unethical behaviors.

Practical Implications

This research also has practical implications for organization management, especially for the ethics management in the banking industry where ethical scandals have happened so frequently and have caused severe consequences. First, consistent with other explorations of trickle-down effects of leadership, the present study highlighted the importance of the role modeling of leadership and indicated that high-level leaders should set examples for middle-level supervisors in the practice of ethical leadership. Moreover, supervisors can act either as transmitters of ethical behaviors or as inhibitors of unethical behaviors due to their role as linking-pins in the organization. Therefore, the role of middle-level supervisors in promoting the trickle-down effect of ethical behaviors should deserve greater attention in organizational ethics management. Second, our findings of the intermediate mechanisms underlying the trickle-down effect offered deeper insights into how the trickle-down effect occurs, providing practical recommendations for facilitating the trickle-down effect. For instance, high-level managers should serve as ethical role models for middle-level supervisors to bolster their perceived abilities to execute ethical behaviors and to shape their ethical outcome expectations, which would in turn transform those supervisors into ethical leaders.

Limitations and Future Directions

Our research is not without limitations. First, although we have theoretically delineated the causal relationships among variables, the cross-sectional data used in the current study still limited us in making causal inferences. The use of multi-source ratings in the current study could help alleviate the common method variance to some extent, yet future research could use more advanced design to provide more conservative and robust support for the proposed relationships among variables. For example, a multi-wave longitudinal design with the ratings for ethical leadership and the mediators separate in time would help establish more robust findings. Second, in this study, we only collected our sample from the banking industry, the generalizability of our findings to other contexts remains to be further investigated. Although ethical leadership research does not primarily focus on any specific sector or generate conclusions that were only applicable to any given sector, future research should still consider the generalizability of the trickle-down effect to other research settings. Third, in theorizing on the proposed mechanisms linking manager’s ethical leadership and followers’ ethical efficacy expectation, we relied heavily on social learning theory and suggested that the four sources of efficacy, as suggested originally by Bandura (1977), to be the focal determinants of ethical efficacy. However, due to the lack of a valid scale, we did not measure the four sources of efficacy directly in the current study, which is an obvious limitation of our study. This said, future research should develop a reliable and valid scale to measure the four sources of efficacy.

The fourth limitation concerned with the use of self-developed measure of ethical outcome expectations. As suggested by Bandura (1977), outcome expectation is very specific to the research context. Bearing this in mind, we indeed tried to find an existing measure for ethical outcome expectations. Although there have been a few studies that operationalized ethical outcome expectations (Ashkanasy et al. 2006; Treviño and Youngblood 1990), the measurement approach used in these studies, either designed for an experimental setting or based on a general assessment (i.e., not differentiating two types of expectations), did not fit well with the context and the purpose of our research. Given the lack of an appropriate scale to measure ethical outcome expectations, we considered the alternative of using a self-developed measure instead. We realized that, however, many researchers have developed measures according to their research purposes. For instance, Yuan and Woodman (2010) developed three items to assess employees’ expected positive performance outcomes from innovative behaviors; Lian et al. (2011) developed a five-item scale to assess employees’ perceptions of the likelihood that abusive behaviors would be rewarded; Schaubroeck et al. (2016) recently used a self-developed three-item scale to evaluate employees’ beliefs about the benefits of engaging in desired customer service behaviors. In light of these studies, we built upon our theorizing of ethical outcome expectations and developed a six-item measure for the current study. Although the self-developed measure has demonstrated good reliability and validity, we still encourage future research to further develop and validate a rigorous measure for ethical outcome expectations.

Finally, we examined the trickle-down effect of ethical leadership and its “black box” solely from the social learning perspective, leaving other potential perspectives uncharted. The theories of ethical leadership and the earlier work on the trickle-down effect of leadership in combination suggested that there may be more than one reason explaining why ethical leadership cascades in organizations. For instance, according to social exchange theory and the observation that “imitation is the sincerest form of flattery” (Liden et al. 2014, p. 1436), middle-level supervisors are expected to feel a sense of indebtedness to high-level ethical managers because of their trustworthy, humane, and fair nature (Mayer et al. 2012), and such a sense of indebtedness might drive middle-level supervisors to reciprocate ethical managers by emulating their ethical leadership behaviors. Besides social exchange theory, social identity theory may also be an alternative explanation. This theory holds that followers who identify with their leaders are more sensitive to and active in meeting leaders’ expectations (Aron 2003; Li and Sun 2015; Pratt 1998). In this sense, it could be possible that high-level managers’ ethical leadership could elicit middle-level supervisors’ personal identification with managers (e.g., Walumbwa et al. 2011), which would in turn influence them to display behaviors that are consistent with high-level managers’ ethical expectations. This said, future research should continue investigating alternative explanations for the trickle-down effect of ethical leadership, which could help provide a more comprehensive understanding of this phenomenon.