Introduction

The peripheral and central vestibular systems are bilaterally organized in three major planes of action: the horizontal yaw plane, the sagittal pitch plane, and the frontal roll plane. Plane-specific disorders are determined by ocular motor, postural, and perceptual signs. Consequently, central vestibular disorders due to acute unilateral lesions manifest with signs and symptoms in one or several of these spatial planes. Vestibular tone imbalances in the roll plane are the most frequent. They are caused by a unilateral damage of graviceptive pathways, which mediate input from the otolith organs and vertical semicircular canals [1,2,3]. Typical roll plane signs are tilts of the perceived visual vertical, ocular torsion, skew deviation, and tilts of the head and body. This complete eye–head synkinesis is called the ocular tilt reaction (OTR). It was first observed during electrical stimulation of the rostral midbrain tegmentum in the monkey [4, 5] and was later described also in patients with unilateral meso-diencephalic lesions [6]. Various clinical studies have shown that tilts of perceived verticality are highly sensitive vestibular signs of unilateral peripheral [7,8,9,10] and central lesions of the vestibular circuitry in the brainstem [3, 11,12,13,14] as well as the vestibular thalamus [15,16,17] and the parieto-insular vestibular cortex [14, 18]. As regards the frequency of all components of the OTR in unilateral brainstem lesions, SVV tilts occurred in up to 80–90%, ocular torsion followed with 80%, skew deviation with 40%, and complete OTR with 20% [13, 16].

Human lesion studies have allowed clinically relevant interpretations on which the following topographic diagnostic rules are based: (1) complete OTR or its ocular motor components occur only with unilateral lesions from the labyrinth to the rostral midbrain tegmentum, including the INC and the rostral interstitial nucleus of the medial longitudinal fascicle (riMLF). Thus, reflexive control of eyes (vestibulo-ocular reflex and vestibulo-ocular tone imbalance), head, and body (vestibulo-spinal reflexes and tone imbalance, head tilt and body lateropulsion) is mediated at the brainstem/cerebellar level [19]. (2) Unilateral graviceptive tone imbalances caused by lesions of the centromedial or posterolateral vestibulo-thalamic subnuclei or the parieto-insular vestibular cortex (PIVC) manifest only with perceptual tilts and are determined by deviations of the perceived visual vertical. (3) All unilateral peripheral and caudal pontomedullary brainstem lesions (including the vestibular nuclei, VN) manifest with ocular motor and perceptual tilts that are ipsilateral. All unilateral lesions within the pontomesencephalic brainstem cause contralateral tilts due to a crossing of graviceptive pathways in the pons at a level above the vestibular nuclei. (4) Unilateral lesions at thalamic or cortical level manifest with either ipsilateral or contralateral tilts of the SVV [2, 15, 16]. The direction of the tilts is constant in the single patient (intraindividual consistency), but it varies interindividually, with about 50% ipsilateral and 50% contralateral. (5) The amount of the SVV tilts in degrees is less in thalamic and in cortical lesions (thalamus: mean 3°–5°; cortex: mean 4°–6° compared to brainstem lesions, mean 8°–14°) (Fig. 1; Table 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Two alternative mechanisms for SVV tilt after unilateral vestibular lesion. a OTO model (see text). Lower row: body position of the patient, dark red cross indicates lesion side. Middle row: peripheral processing: unequal numbers of hair cells with left and right preferred directions on the intact utricle cause a utricular bias (dark red arrow), which is added to the remaining saccular and utricular discharge (light blue arrows) caused by gravity to yield the otolith vector (dark blue arrow). Top row: central processing: vectorial addition of normalized otolith vector (dark blue arrow) and idiotropic bias (light blue arrow) leads to the SVV estimate (red line). The resulting SVV is equal for left-ear-down and right-ear-down positions. b OTO model prediction for SVV tilt due to asymmetry of utricular hair cell proportion. The predicted SVV deviation at 90° is equal for ipsi- or contralateral lesions. c SCC model (see text). Lower row as in a. Middle row (peripheral): independent of body position, a torsional semicircular canal bias (dark orange arrow) rotates the otolith vector away from the affected ear. Top row: summation with idiotropic bias as in a. d Right: SCC model prediction for SVV tilt due to semicircular canal imbalance. At 90°, prediction for ipsi- and contralateral lesions differs by approximately the same amount as in the upright position. In c and d, the SVV deviation in the upright position has been set to about 12.5° corresponding to the data of [29]. In case of an asymmetry of utricular hair cells b the difference at 90° right-ear-down or left-ear-down positions (dashed vertical line) is close to zero. In contrast, the model with canal bias predicts a sustained difference for right-ear-down and left-ear-down positions

Table 1 SVV tilts in acute unilateral vestibular lesions at different lesion sites from labyrinth to cortex (ipsilateral tilt = i; contralateral tilt = c)

The last two findings are still unexplained. Why do unilateral lesions manifest at the thalamic or cortical level with perceptual tilts in either ipsi- or contralateral direction? Why are these deviations from true vertical quantitatively smaller than in brainstem and peripheral vestibular lesions? There is a partial crossing of the ascending graviceptive pathways in the rostral midbrain that provides the thalamus and the cortex with input from graviceptive sensors of both labyrinths [20]. This allows the right and left thalamocortical networks to operate separately (there is no direct interconnection between the two thalami) and mediate vestibular information from both labyrinths [21].

Another explanation for the difference between periphery/brainstem versus thalamus/cortex lesions could be based on different neuronal coding principles. In a recent study based on the clinical experience that caudal vestibular brainstem lesions cause rotational vertigo, whereas more rostral vestibular lesions cause non-rotational dizziness or only imbalance without vertigo. We mathematically simulated the different manifestations of vestibular disorders in the yaw plane using a neural network model of the head direction cell system [22]. This model was based on experimentally documented differences in neuronal coding for angular head velocity and head direction [23]. It explained and confirmed the clinical experience of vestibular vertigo syndromes in the horizontal semicircular canal plane. However, graviceptive vestibular function in the roll plane was not included in that model, although the majority of patients presented with roll plane signs.

We here hypothesize that the differential effects of unilateral graviceptive lesions in either the brainstem or thalamocortical vestibular circuitry can also be explained by a dysfunction of a graviceptive cell system that uses coding principles for spatial orientation in the roll (and pitch) planes similar to those exhibited by the head direction cell system for the horizontal yaw plane [23]. We therefore extended our model approach to the graviceptive frontal roll plane. Findings in a recent study on macaque monkey of the gravity-orientation tuning in anterior thalamic neurons [24] encouraged us to develop the following model.

Methods

Clinical study on published SVV tilts

A total of 15 published studies on acute unilateral peripheral or central vestibular lesions were reviewed as to the direction and amount of SVV tilts [2, 3, 7,8,9, 12,13,14, 16, 18, 25,26,27,28]. A statistical meta-analysis was not applicable since the methods used to determine the visual vertical differed across the studies, as did the lesion size and the grouping of the patients (complete or incomplete peripheral lesions; analyses of the entire brainstem versus lower and upper brainstem segments in central lesions). The data were obtained in upright body position. The mathematical model approach in the current study, however, also requires patient data on SVV measurements in tilted body positions, which are comparatively rare. We took into account two studies in which patients with peripheral vestibular lesions were tested in right-ear-down (RED) and left-ear-down (LED) positions [29, 30].

Model approach

In the following, only lateral tilt and perception of verticality in the roll plane are considered for clarity. Systems-level models for dynamic three-dimensional gravity or tilt perception have been published elsewhere [31,32,33,34,35,36,37 and other publications not cited here]. In contrast to these models, which focused on the mechanisms required to solve the problem of perception of verticality on a relatively abstract level, the current work posits a hypothesis as to how these mechanisms might be implemented at the neuronal level. Firing rate neurons (that is, simulated neurons that have firing rates as input and output) were used throughout, since these model neurons simplify the task of designing a network-level model implementing gravity perception with the aim of simulating lesions of this network.

Peripheral sensory input

The otolith organs, the sensory organs for gravity, decompose the gravity vector into one component aligned approximately with the interaural axis (utricle) and a second component aligned with the body-longitudinal axis (saccule). The primary otolith afferents are tuned to the angle of tilt in a sinusoidal manner. Since otolith hair cells on one otolith macula are polarized in both directions (e.g., left–right for the utricle and the roll plane), there are four populations of hair cells (two utricular and two saccular populations) for each side of the head. For example, the left utricle contains hair cells with preferred direction for leftward tilt, but also for rightward tilt. In addition to the sinusoidal tuning, each afferent fiber has a baseline firing rate. Taken together, the afferent fibers from each of the populations thus yield a population response with a baseline firing rate approximately proportional to the number of fibers [38].

In the following, we propose two mechanisms of how peripheral lesions might lead to roll tilt SVV deviations in the upright position and formulate them as computational models.

Otolith (OTO) model (Fig. 1a): the predominant explanation for SVV tilt after peripheral lesions is that the SVV bias is caused by the effect of the lesion on otolith afferents. To model this assumption, we assumed that hair cells on both sides of the striola (having opposite directions) are not equal in number (Ewald’s law for otolith organs) [39]. We refer to this model assumption as the “OTO model”. In a healthy system, the central fusion of utricular afferents from the left and right labyrinths and from both sides of the striola ensures an unbiased response. However, after a peripheral lesion, the unbalanced input from the remaining side causes an SVV deviation in the upright position (see Appendix for mathematical derivation).

Semicircular canal (SCC) model (Fig. 1c): an alternative explanation for SVV tilt following peripheral lesions is based on semicircular canal input. Gravity perception requires that the so-called tilt-translation ambiguity be solved [31,32,33,34,35,36,37, 40, 41]. The predominant view is that semicircular canal input is used by a central estimation mechanism to disentangle otolith input caused by tilt from that caused by translational acceleration. Under normal circumstances, canal input is transient and has no influence on static body positions. However, a peripheral unilateral lesion causes a continuous imbalance of semicircular canal afferent firing, since input from the lesioned side is missing. This imbalance is centrally interpreted as a head rotation, which manifests as vertigo and spontaneous nystagmus. However, it should also have an effect on the estimation of the direction of gravity. For this model, we assumed a basic gravity estimation mechanism proposed previously (2-D: [40]; 3-D: [31, 33, 34]). This dynamic model consists of a 3-D differential equation system whose static solution predicts an SVV deviation in the upright position that is approximately proportional to the semicircular canal bias (see Appendix). Since it is currently unknown how or where the brain implements gravity estimation at the neural level, we assumed for the SCC model a simple fusion of otolith and semicircular canal input (see Appendix) below the thalamus, for example, at the brainstem or cerebellar level (see for example [42, 43]).

Central processing

Otolith afferent fibers from both sides are fused (probably in the vestibular nuclei) via commissural connections, so that overall four populations of neurons can be assumed: those tuned for 2-D leftward, rightward, upward, and downward acceleration. While the components of gravitational acceleration in the periphery and supposedly in brainstem pathways are coded in sensory coordinates, central neurons show a tuning that is no longer restricted to the two directions of otolith organs. In central neurons [24], tuning is distributed over various directions similar to that of the head direction cell system [23, 44]. We thus implemented a tuning curve population model [45]. To implement the well-known bias of perception of the direction of gravity (Aubert effect, see for example [46]), we assumed a so-called rate change model, in which tuning curves are not homogeneous but depend on the direction of tuning ([45], Fig. 2 left), and from which the SVV direction (Fig. 2, right) is computed via population decoding. The tuning curve population model receives appropriately weighted input from utricular and saccular otolith afferents (in the case of the SCC model, after fusion with canal input). The perceptual bias implemented by the population of gravity-tuned neurons acts in a very similar way as Mittelstaedt’s idiotropic vector, which was applied to the normalized otolith organ vector [46]. The normalization, which causes the SVV to be approximately independent of gravity magnitude, is here implemented by transforming the vectorial representation of the gravity estimate (utricular and saccular components) to a population representation that codes the direction but not the magnitude of the vector.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Left: simulated neuronal tuning functions for graviceptive pathways above midbrain implementing rate change tuning. Right: subjective visual vertical derived from a tuning curve population model with neurons tuned to gravity direction as shown on the left. The typical Aubert effect (underestimation of tilt for larger angles) can be clearly seen (blue line)

Results

Clinical data on SVV tilts

Unilateral peripheral lesions of the labyrinth or the eighth nerve caused ipsilateral SVV tilts in upright head and body position, which were maximal with complete vestibular loss due to neurectomy or labyrinthectomy [9, 10, 25] (Table 1; Fig. 3). Pontomedullary lesions of the vestibular nuclei also caused ipsilateral tilts of about the same amount. Above the crossing of graviceptive pathways in the lower pons (Table 1; Fig. 3), the tilts were contralateral up to the rostral midbrain tegmentum (INC) and showed somewhat less degrees of deviation from true vertical in the pons [3, 13, 14]. One exception from that rule has been described for anteromedian pontomesencephalic lesions close to and within the medial lemniscus which manifested with isolated ipsilateral SVV tilts of up to 6.6° [28]. Two studies have shown that lesions of the vestibular thalamic subnuclei caused either ipsilateral or contralateral SVV tilts had a lesser degree of up to 5° [15, 16]. The same is true for unilateral lesions of the parieto-insular vestibular cortex [1, 2]. For better understanding, the directions and ranges of the mean data have been schematically depicted in a simplified way (Fig. 3). This allowed us to demonstrate the special features of vestibular lesions in the thalamus and cortex, i.e., the smaller tilts to the ipsi- or contralateral side.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Schematic graviceptive pathways together with the amount (in deg) of SVV deviation for ipsi- and contralateral lesions depending on the level of acute unilateral vestibular damage. The range of the mean values was calculated from a total of 15 published studies (see Table 1). The four major messages are as follows. (1) In peripheral and pontomedullary brainstem lesions SVV tilts are ipsilateral. (2) In pontomesencephalic vestibular pathway lesions up to the INC, SVV tilts are contralateral. (3) In vestibular thalamic and cortical lesions, SVV tilts may be either ipsilateral or contralateral with an intraindividual consistency and an equal distribution interindividually. (4) The amount of SVV tilt is maximal in complete peripheral lesions, followed by tilt in brainstem lesions and less in lesions of the vestibular thalamus and cortex

Simulation results

Simulation of the intact network shows the typical Aubert effect on perception of verticality, that is, the underestimation of tilt observed for large tilt angles (Fig. 2, right). No particular effort was made to fit the response to experimental data so that the result shown in Fig. 2 should be considered an approximate response illustrating the principle. Note that the idiotropic vector [46], which can be interpreted in Bayesian terms as prior expectation for upright orientation [47,48,49], is implicitly realized by inhomogeneous tuning curves. Thus, there is no neuron or neuronal population implementing the idiotropic vector; instead, it is coded in the synaptic weights of the network.

For peripheral lesions, we simulated two different models. As described in “Methods”, the OTO model assumes that the SVV deviation in the upright position is caused by unequal numbers of hair cells on the utricular macula, an assumption found in the majority of the literature (for example, [29, 30]). The SCC model assumes that the SVV deviation is caused by a torsional bias of the vertical semicircular canals mediated by the central gravity estimator. Figure 4 shows simulation of both models in comparison to the data of [29], obtained after unilateral vestibular nerve section. Both models have been adjusted for the value reported in the upright position (12.4°): in the OTO model, the hair cell ratio was set to 0.436; in the SCC model the roll angular velocity bias was set to 2.8°/s, which, assuming a torsional VOR gain of 0.5, would approximately correspond to a hardly recognizable torsional nystagmus of 1.4°/s. Both models also predict some static ocular torsion due to utricular bias for the OTO model and the effect of sustained semicircular canal input for the SCC model [51].

Fig. 4
figure 4

SVV data from [32] obtained in patients with unilateral vestibular nerve section (blue, error bars denote SD) and model simulations of the SVV adjustments (red and yellow). The OTO model (yellow) assumes unequal distribution of hair cells with opposite tuning on the utricular macula (Ewald’s law for otolith organs); the SCC model (red) assumes that the afferent input of vertical semicircular canals is processed centrally by the gravity estimation mechanism. After a lesion, the semicircular canal bias causes a perceptual error of gravity direction that becomes visible as SVV tilt

While both models can simulate the SVV deviation in the upright position, the model predictions differ for tilted positions such as right-ear-down and left-ear-down positions (see Fig. 1b, d). As shown in Fig. 4, the SCC model clearly corresponds better to the patient data obtained in tilted head positions.

A unilateral lesion of the central population network was simulated by randomly removing half of the neurons in the network, that is, by setting their output firing rate to zero. To estimate the amount of the resulting SVV deviation, the lesion simulation was repeated 100 times (larger numbers yield similar results). With the present network, the average absolute SVV tilt was 5.0° for the upright position (Fig. 5); this is very close to the clinical findings at the thalamo-cortical level (see Fig. 3).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Average subjective visual vertical after simulated unilateral central lesion of the tuning curve population (red region shows standard deviation of the simulated lesion). To simulate the lesion, approximately 50% of the neurons have been removed randomly. For the upright position, the simulated mean SVV tilt is 0° ± 6.2° corresponding to an average absolute deviation of 5.0°

Fig. 6
figure 6

Simulated cosine tuning functions for population response of utricular afferents in the roll plane (blue: left utricle; yellow: right utricle). For each utricle, two functions corresponding to ipsi- and contralaterally polarized hair cells are shown. It is assumed that hair cell proportions are unequal so that both response gain and offset depend on hair cell polarization

Discussion

Modeling unilateral peripheral vestibular lesions

To explain the effect of unilateral peripheral lesions on SVV deviation, we considered two alternatives: for the OTO model unequal proportions of utricular hair cells with opposite tuning direction were assumed; for the SCC model we took into account that the SVV depends on tilt-translation disambiguation and that the central gravity estimator operates with semicircular canal input. Thus, SVV tilt following a peripheral lesion could be caused by an imbalance of utricular firing rate for ipsi- and contralesionally tuned hair cells, or by the well-known imbalance of semicircular canal input. Such an imbalance would act as rotational input to a central gravity estimator, which in turn leads to a deviation of the SVV.

While both models are able to generate an SVV deviation in the upright position, the model predictions for tilted body positions differ. The OTO model predicts that the difference of the SVV setting for right- and left-ear-down positions vanishes, since in both positions the SVV is determined only by utricular input independent of its amount (Fig. 1a, b). In contrast, the SCC model predicts that the SVV is rotated by an approximately constant amount to the ipsilateral side manifesting as a constant shift of the SVV independently of body position (Fig. 1c, d).

From our comparison of the predictions of the two models with data of the SVV adjustments in patients with unilateral vestibular lesions in left-ear-down and right-ear-down body positions [29], we propose that the SVV deviation in the upright position is indeed predominantly caused by the effect of semicircular canal bias on the gravity estimator and is not due to an utricular hair cell asymmetry. Corresponding to our assumption that SVV tilt is caused by semicircular canal imbalance, we previously have shown that static ocular torsion observed during prolonged galvanic vestibular stimulation can be fully explained by semicircular canal activation [50]. Damage to the gravity estimator has also recently been implicated in the generation of persistent horizontal positional nystagmus [51].

We also considered patient data from Müller et al. [30], who measured SVV in patients with unilateral peripheral vestibular damage in different body positions. However, due to the smaller SVV tilts found in their study, the differences between model predictions were not as conclusive as for the data from Böhmer and Rickenmann [29] (see Appendix Fig. 7). Müller and co-workers [30] also tested the hypothesis that SVV tilt due to complete vestibular nerve lesion should differ from that of a lesion of the superior branch, because the latter would spare the saccular nerve. The difference between both types of lesions in their data was negligible. An evident explanation compatible with our model is that SVV tilt with peripheral lesions is not caused by otolith organ dysfunction, but by the lesion of the vertical semicircular canal afferents, which are affected in both cases (even though only the superior canal is affected with superior nerve lesion).

Fig. 7
figure 7

Data from [30] (blue, complete vestibular nerve lesion, error bars denote SD) compared to SCC model prediction (red) for SVV tilt (left, due to semicircular canal bias) and OTO model prediction (right, due to utricular haircell asymmetry). Model parameters have been adjusted to approximately match the data. Due to the small bias in the upright position, the difference between model predictions is within the variability of the data, even though it is in the same direction as in Fig. 4 (asymmetric errors for the OTO model, right panel)

To further clarify the relative contribution of the otolith or canal biases to pathological SVV deviations after unilateral lesions, SVV data from patients not only in the upright position but also in tilted positions are required. Such data acquired in patients with brainstem or midbrain lesions might also help to elucidate where otolith–canal fusion for gravity estimation takes place—already in the cerebellum [42, 43] or higher at midbrain levels, thus comparable to the generation of the head direction cell signal [22, 23].

Modeling central vestibular lesions at thalamic level

For the central lesions, we proposed a tuning function network that codes gravity direction in a distributed fashion similar to the coding of heading direction in the head direction cell network [22, 23]. The recent finding of gravity-tuned neurons in the thalamus [24] prompted us to propose that neuronal coding of gravity in the thalamus, and possibly in regions below and above, follows similar principles as known for the head direction cell system [23]. The effect of a unilateral lesion of such a network depends on its implementation and read-out mechanisms. Here, we assumed that thalamic gravity-encoding neurons converge onto a cortical population-tuning network with similar coding properties; this is followed by standard population decoding to derive the gravity direction encoded in the network activity. In addition, we proposed that the perceptual bias in tilted positions, i.e., the Aubert effect, which has been explained as idiotropic vector or, in a Bayesian framework, as prior expectation for the upright position, is implemented at the neural level as inhomogeneous tuning functions via synaptic weights. Interestingly, the transformation from vectorial coding (utricular/horizontal and saccular/vertical components) to population coding also implements what is [50] called normalization, that is, representation of the angle with respect to gravity independently of the amplitude of the otolith vector. To achieve normalization in a vectorial coding scheme would require the implementation of non-linear mathematical operations, specifically division, which is difficult to realize neuronally. However, the transformation from a vector code to a population code proposed here implements the normalization as emergent property and without needing mathematical division.

Unilateral central lesion of this model network showed that in the upright position, a random bias with an average deviation of 5° emerges. The direction of the bias depends on the distribution of neurons (or neuronal assemblies) on both sides of the thalamus. A perfectly equal distribution would not cause any bias after a unilateral lesion. We thus hypothesize that the SVV tilt observed after unilateral thalamic or cortical lesions, which is independent of the lesion side, is due to the population coding of the direction of gravity and the relative distribution of neuronal assemblies in the population. The experimentally observed distribution of lateral deviations is unlikely to be related to hemispheric vestibular dominance, since this would imply a dependence of tilt magnitude on the side of the lesion, which was not found.

Instead of representing gravity direction using a feedforward network, we could as well implement the gravity-tuned population model by an attractor network with recurrent connectivity as supposed to exist for head direction cells [22, 23, 44]. The results of such an attractor network concerning the effect of unilateral lesions is, according to our simulations, similar, but the implementation as feedforward network used here is simpler and therefore preferable to illustrate our main hypotheses.

However, an attractor solution would have several additional advantages:

  1. 1.

    Since an attractor network implements a short-term memory, during short periods of weightlessness, such as while jumping down from a rock, the perception of the direction of gravity would be sustained despite zero input from the otolith organs. In a pure feedforward network, it would rapidly decay.

  2. 2.

    During longer periods of weightlessness, the subjective perception of verticality does not vanish but aligns with the body midline [52, 53]. This is exactly what happens in an attractor network with inhomogeneous tuning curves due to drift of the bump of activity toward the region with maximal tuning. Again, in a feedforward network, the perception of verticality would simply vanish during zero g.

  3. 3.

    As mentioned above, all systems-level models for gravity estimation and tilt-translation disambiguation ([31,32,33,34,35,36,37] and others) posit that an internal estimate of gravity is updated not only by otolith input, but also by angular velocity signals derived from semicircular canal input. While we assumed here that gravity estimation is performed below the level of population coding, for example by cerebellar circuits [43], an attractor network could solve this updating using a similar integration mechanism as applied in the head direction cell network to update head direction while turning [22].

Conclusions

The presented model explains the clinical data summarized in Fig. 3: (1) ipsilateral tilt of perceived verticality in unilateral peripheral lesions, which are compatible with SVV tilts measured in upright or head-tilted positions; (2) ipsi- or contralateral tilts in vestibular lesions of the thalamus or cortex; (3) SVV tilts in degree are about 50% smaller in thalamus and cortex lesions compared to a peripheral deficit. The pathophysiology of the latter two clinical findings has been hitherto unexplained. Our model approach to coding of the direction of gravity in central areas is well in line with animal data that suggest a 3-D ‘brain compass’ comprising head direction and gravity direction cells [44]. Furthermore, comparison of our model simulations with patient data in the right-ear-down and left-ear-down positions suggests that static deviations of the SVV after unilateral peripheral lesions may not be caused by otolith organ imbalance as suggested previously, but may be a consequence of a vertical semicircular canal imbalance acting upon the central gravity estimator.