Introduction

Brain size for a given body size varies appreciably among mammalian species (e.g. Striedter 2005) and relates to intellectual or cognitive performance (Deaner et al. 2007; Reader et al. 2011). These enhanced cognitive abilities engender general behavioural flexibility (Fernandes et al. 2014; Borrego and Gaines 2016; Burkart et al. 2016), and therefore provide numerous benefits in both the social (e.g. Byrne and Whiten 1988; Barrett and Henzi 2005; Emery et al. 2007; Dunbar and Shultz 2017) and ecological domain (e.g. Parker and Gibson 1977; Sol 2009; van Woerden et al. 2012; Benson-Amram et al. 2016; Heldstab et al. 2016b; Navarrete et al. 2016; DeCasien et al. 2017; Powell et al. 2017). However, the benefit of enhanced cognitive abilities is counterbalanced by the energetic demands of larger and hence metabolically more expensive brains. Brain tissue is among the most metabolically expensive tissues in the body to maintain and grow (Holliday 1986; Rolfe and Brown 1997; Niven and Laughlin 2008; Bauernfeind et al. 2014; Kuzawa et al. 2014) and benefits from a supply of energy at all times (Mink et al. 1981; Lukas and Campbell 2000). Thus, one would predict that species can only evolve relatively larger brains than their ancestors if they can constantly sustain the high energetic costs of brain development and maintenance, and if these costs do not outweigh the fitness benefits of superior cognitive abilities.

The expensive brain hypothesis (Isler and van Schaik 2009a) postulates that the increased costs of an evolutionary brain enlargement can be paid for by two complementary mechanisms, for which comparative studies of mammals, birds, amphibians and fish have provided extensive support (Clutton-Brock and Harvey 1980; Fish and Lockwood 2003; Isler and van Schaik 2006b, 2009a, 2009b; van Woerden et al. 2010, 2012, 2014; Barton and Capellini 2011; Navarrete et al. 2011; Kotrschal et al. 2013; Weisbecker et al. 2015; Heldstab et al. 2016a, 2018b; Luo et al. 2017; Genoud et al. 2018; Yu et al. 2018). The first, trade-off pathway is a redirection of energy allocated to other body functions such as locomotion (Isler and van Schaik 2006b; Navarrete et al. 2011; Heldstab et al. 2016a), rate of development (Isler and van Schaik 2009a; Barton and Capellini 2011; Yu et al. 2018) and fertility (Isler and van Schaik 2009a, 2009b; Kotrschal et al. 2013). The second pathway of providing the increased energy needs of larger brains consists of a stable net increase in energy input. This has been supported in mammals in general (particularly so in Eutheria, but not in Metatheria) where the basal metabolic rate (BMR), a proxy of the net energy input, is positively correlated with brain size (Isler and van Schaik 2006a; Genoud et al. 2018). The increased energy input could be achieved by improved diet quality (Clutton-Brock and Harvey 1980; Fish and Lockwood 2003), or by avoiding periods of starvation in both mammals (van Woerden et al. 2010, 2012, 2014; Weisbecker et al. 2015) and frogs (Luo et al. 2017). Hibernators, whose intake varies most dramatically (e.g. Lovegrove et al. 2014), are also found to have smaller brains (Veitschegger 2017; Heldstab et al. 2018b).

The broad support for the expensive brain hypothesis suggests that organisms have a brain as large as they can sustain energetically. The expensive brain hypothesis should therefore also predict effects of energy inputs during brain growth due to allomaternal care by the breeding male (paternal care) or non-breeding helpers (alloparental care). Brains benefit from high and stable energetic input especially during the growth and differentiation phase (Holliday 1986; Bauernfeind et al. 2014; Kuzawa et al. 2014) because developing brains cannot be starved temporarily without permanent cognitive damage (Lukas and Campbell 2000). This energetic constraint on brain development is expected to be relieved by energy subsidies to breeding females and immatures, allowing the evolution of bigger brains in species with allomaternal care, including humans (Burkart et al. 2009; Hrdy 2009; van Schaik and Burkart 2011; Isler and van Schaik 2012). However, empirical evidence for this prediction is mixed. A large comparative study across more than 400 mammals found support for this hypothesis. Isler and van Schaik (2012) found a positive correlation between brain size and the amount of allomaternal care. Likewise, cooperatively breeding mammals and most altricial birds do not show the steep decline in rmax (maximum rate of population increase) with brain size found among independent breeders, suggesting that allomaternal care enables species to increase their brain size without compromising their demographic viability (Isler and van Schaik 2009b). In contrast, two other studies, one in the parvorder Corvida (Iwaniuk and Arnold 2004) and one in cichlid fishes (Reddon et al. 2016) found no relationship between brain size and cooperative breeding.

Some of the ambiguity in results of previous studies may have arisen because most studies did not or only partially separate care according to the identity of the carer, although benefits and costs, and therefore the reliability of allomaternal care may differ between the male breeder and alloparents. For instance, Isler and van Schaik (2009b, 2012) did not distinguish between the contribution by fathers or non-parents to offspring care and hence the positive correlation between brain size and allomaternal care could be driven by paternal care, by alloparental care or both. Similarly, in the cichlid fish study, Reddon et al. (2016) compared cooperatively and independently breeding species but the independently breeding species comprised species with biparental and uniparental care (usually maternal care). And finally, Iwaniuk and Arnold (2004) solely tested for a difference in brain size between independently and cooperatively breeding species but did not test for a difference in encephalisation between species with uniparental and species with biparental care. We therefore reassess the relationship between brain size and allomaternal care by separating the effect of allomaternal care provided by males (paternal care) from that provided by other group members (alloparental care).

The goal of this paper is to re-evaluate the effects of energy inputs on brain size in light of the expensive brain hypothesis. As mentioned above, organisms cannot opportunistically respond with brain enlargement to highly fluctuating increases in energy supply. Thus, selection should only favour increased brain size if the increase in energy available to the female is predictable and constant. We therefore predict the following pattern. In species with male care (in addition to the care of the breeding female) or cooperative breeding (in which the breeding male also always participates in caring), the care provided by the breeding male is reliably present, being a stable trait of such species. Indeed, even if the male reduces his contribution in response to the activity of helpers (Price 1992; Rothe et al. 1993; Santos et al. 1997; Bales et al. 2000), there is nonetheless a stable amount of extra energy available to the breeding female. Thus, the expensive brain hypothesis predicts that natural selection will favour females responding to the reliable presence of male care by producing larger-brained offspring.

In contrast, the care provided by helpers is much more variable depending on various factors such as the age of the parents, group composition (number of helpers available) and variation in environmental conditions. Thus, new breeding pairs usually have no helpers or only young inexperienced helpers who tend to provide less help than experienced older caregivers (Tardif et al. 1992; Heinsohn and Cockburn 1994; Woxvold et al. 2006; Rymer and Pillay 2014). Moreover, whereas the help provided by the breeding male is unaffected by his body condition or food abundance, other non-breeding group members generally adjust their helping efforts in relation to their body condition and according to food availability (Harrington et al. 1983; Creel and Creel 2002; Nichols et al. 2012; Marshall et al. 2016). Finally, subordinates can also start to breed themselves, in which case their help to the dominant female could end abruptly or be minimal to begin with (Clutton-Brock et al. 2002; Young et al. 2005; Brouwer et al. 2011; Zöttl et al. 2013). In sum, energy inputs due to alloparental help should be less reliable and therefore less likely to allow for a selective change in the energy allocation to the growing brain during development. Instead, we suggest that these more fluctuating energy inputs are allocated to reproduction. It is well known that females respond to increased food availability with increased fertility (Tyler 1987; Wauters and Lens 1995; van Noordwijk and van Schaik 1999; Heesen et al. 2013; Arlet et al. 2015). The positive effect of the amount of alloparental care on fertility is well established in birds (Russell and Rowley 1988; Mumme 1992; Komdeur 1994; Klauke et al. 2013; Dixit et al. 2017) and mammals (Moehlman 1979; Fairbanks 1990; Solomon 1991; Koenig 1995; Garber and Leigh 1997; Mitani and Watts 1997; Ross and MacLarnon 2000; Russell et al. 2003).

In this paper, we therefore test this modified prediction of the expensive brain hypothesis (Fig. 1): if the level of steady energy inputs relates to a species’ brain size, the presence and amount of paternal care by the breeding male (in addition to maternal care), being reliable and steady, should be correlated with increased brain size, whereas the presence and amount of allomaternal care by non-breeding helpers (alloparental care) should not be and rather be correlated with increased fertility. We test these predictions in a large sample of 478 mammalian species using a new approach that clearly distinguishes care provided by the breeding male (paternal care) from care by non-breeding helpers (alloparental care).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Predicted pattern of correlated evolution between different sources of allomaternal care and brain size or fertility. We expect paternal care, which is predictable in amount and time, to be associated with large relative brain size, but alloparental care, which is less predictable, to be related to higher fertility rates

Material and methods

Brain size, body mass and fertility

Data on brain size and body mass were mainly retrieved from Isler and van Schaik (2012) and supplemented with data from published compilations (Isler et al. 2008; Isler and van Schaik 2009b; Heldstab et al. 2016a; Matějů et al. 2016). Where available, we used female values to reduce error due to sexual dimorphism. Annual fertility of an average female was defined as the product of the number of offspring per litter times the number of litters per year as in Isler and van Schaik (2009a). Data on the number of offspring per litter and litters per year were taken from Myers et al. (2006); Jones et al. (2009); Rowe and Myers (2011); Santos et al. (2015); and West and Capellini (2016), building on the large datasets compiled by Isler and van Schaik (2009a, 2012).

Allomaternal care behaviours

To quantify the continuous amount of allomaternal care, we complemented the database of Isler and van Schaik (2012) and Heldstab et al. (2017) which includes the frequency of occurrence of the following care behaviours: provisioning (separating provisioning aimed at offspring from provisioning aimed at the mother), carrying, protection, and a variable that comprises other energetically influential care behaviours such as huddling, communal nesting and pup retrieval. As scores for provisioning the mother were restricted to the order Carnivora in the dataset of Isler and van Schaik (2012), we added additional data on this behaviour from West and Capellini (2016) for other orders. Communal nursing (allonursing) was excluded in this study because lactating mothers have not been shown to derive any energetic benefits from it (Baldovino and Di Bitetti 2008; MacLeod et al. 2015; Heldstab et al. 2017). To distinguish the effects of allomaternal care provided by males (paternal care) from that provided by other group members (alloparental care), we summed up the frequency of occurrence of all allomaternal care behaviours separately for the father and other group members, as in Heldstab et al. (2017).

To investigate whether the results reported in this study are robust with respect to different coding schemes of allomaternal care, we additionally conducted all analyses by using a binary classification of paternal and alloparental care with 1 indicating the presence and 0 the absence of the trait. We scored the presence of paternal care or alloparental care if the frequency of occurrence of care was higher than 5%. We additionally only scored the presence of paternal care if it comprised more than just defence of territory or protection against predators or infanticide. Such paternal care is probably not energetically significant, and such species are not generally categorised as having male care in studies employing a binary coding scheme (e.g. Woodroffe and Vincent 1994; Lukas and Clutton-Brock 2013; West and Capellini 2016).

In total, data on allomaternal care behaviours and brain size and/or fertility were available for 478 mammals across all major orders. Bats and cetaceans were excluded from our study because reliable data on allomaternal care of both cetaceans and bats are notoriously difficult to obtain. The full dataset is available in the Supplementary Material (ESM 1). Figure 2 shows the distribution of the various care categories in the dataset.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Distribution of the various care categories in the dataset (N = 478 species)

Covariates

Living in social groups (gregariousness), diet quality, diurnality and substrate use have been shown to correlate with brain size in mammals (e.g. Harvey et al. 1980; Gittleman 1986; Bernard and Nurton 1993; Pérez-Barbería and Gordon 2005; Kirk 2006; Dunbar and Shultz 2007; DeCasien et al. 2017; Powell et al. 2017). Although it is less clear how these variables should be related to allomaternal care or fertility, our large sample size allowed us to include these potentially confounding variables into our analyses. Data for these covariates were collated from the literature (data were from Russell 1974; Gittleman 1989; Myers et al. 2006; Jones et al. 2009; van Woerden et al. 2010; Rowe and Myers 2011; Kuznetsova et al. 2013; Lukas and Clutton-Brock 2013; Wilman et al. 2014; Heldstab et al. 2016a; DeCasien et al. 2017). Gregariousness was classified as follows: solitary (or mother with infants) (0); usually solitary, but occasionally seen in pairs or groups, or facultative group denning (0.5); pairs (with infants) (1); usually in pairs, but gregarious at times or in part of the geographic range (1.5) and permanently gregarious (the group comprises more adults than just the parents) (2). To control for diet quality, species were divided into five categories based on their main diet: aquatic faunivore or piscivore (1), frugivore/folivore or granivore (2), frugivore/faunivore or omnivore (3), herbivore or folivore (4) and carnivore, faunivore or insectivore (5). A binary coding was used for activity period, with (1) for nocturnal, cathemeral or crepuscular species and (2) for diurnal species. For substrate use, each species was assigned to one of four substrate use categories: aquatic or semi-aquatic (1), fossorial or semi-fossorial (2), terrestrial or semi-arboreal (3) and arboreal (4).

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were done in JMP™ 13.0 (SAS Institute Inc. 1989–2016) and in R3.4.1 (R Core Team 2017). Fertility, brain size and body mass values were loge transformed before analysis to reduce the skew of their distribution. Because the phylogenetic signal lambda (λ) was always close to 1, the use of methods to control for phylogenetic non-independence was required (Pagel 1999). We therefore built phylogenetic generalised least-squares regressions (PGLS) models (Freckleton et al. 2002) using the ‘caper’ package (Orme 2013) in R. The phylogeny was based on an updated version (Fritz et al. 2009) of the mammalian supertree (Bininda-Emonds et al. 2007) and is given in Fig. S1 (Supplementary Material ESM 2). We used PGLS models with either brain size or fertility as dependent variables, and paternal or alloparental care, female body mass and all possible confounding variables (gregariousness, diet quality, diurnality and substrate use) as independent variables.

We also tested for an additive effect of care by including paternal care, alloparental care and combined care (paternal and alloparental care) as independent variables into the same model. In most species that exhibit alloparental care, paternal care is also observed (see Fig. 2), potentially resulting in collinearity problems in this particular statistical analysis. To assess potential multicollinearity between paternal, alloparental and combined care in the additive model, we generated variance inflation factors (VIF) (Quinn and Keough 2002; Dormann et al. 2013) using non-phylogenetic generalised linear models and the function ‘vif’ (‘car’ package: (Fox and Weisberg 2011)) in R. VIFs quantify how much the variance of an estimated model parameter is increased because of multicollinearity between predictors. The VIF for alloparental care and combined care was higher than 4, which indicates a problematic amount of covariance among predictors (Rogerson 2001; Hair et al. 2006). To solve this, we categorised species into having either paternal care only (no alloparental care), alloparental care only (no paternal care) or combined care (paternal and alloparental care). After this, the VIFs of all independent variables were less than 2, which indicates an acceptable amount of covariance among predictors (Supplementary Material ESM 3, Table S1). To choose the best fitting from a set of models, we used AIC values (Akaike Information Criterion Akaike 1974).

We additionally also performed multi-model averaging as in Heldstab et al. (2017, 2018a) with brain size or fertility as dependent variables, and paternal care only, alloparental care only, combined care, female body mass and all possible confounding variables (gregariousness, diet quality, diurnality and substrate use) as independent variables to test whether our results are robust with respect to different statistical approaches.

We performed information-theoretic model selection based on AICc across all possible models built with the independent variables mentioned above. As the AICc did not clearly distinguish the most highly ranked models, we accounted for uncertainty by using multi-model averaging (Grueber et al. 2011) in the candidate model set, which included all models with ∆AICc < 3. ∆AICc is the difference in AICc between the focal model and the AICc of the best-fitting model in the candidate model set. Estimates of each parameter were averaged across the candidate models, with means weighted by the Akaike weight of a given model. The relative importance of a predictor was obtained by summing the Akaike’s weights of the models in the candidate model set including the focal predictor (Symonds and Moussalli 2011). The method to perform model averaging with the PGLS function in the ‘caper’ package (Orme 2013) is described by Garamszegi and Mundry (2014) and the corresponding material is available at http://www.mpcm-evolution.org.

A phylogenetically informed comparative approach to analyse the combined mammalian dataset as described above was preferred over analysing different clades or orders separately. First, the larger sample size of a combined dataset allows to test models with several covariates. Second, in a phylogenetic analysis with lambda close to 1, grade shifts between clades are represented in the values of one node, and thus are neither neglected nor exert overdue influence. Third, care behaviours are evolutionary relatively stable within clades, while ecological influences on brain size evolution likely show more variation in the tip nodes of the phylogenetic tree. We therefore expect that patterns are much weaker within clades or may even be obscured completely.

Results

The results confirmed our two main predictions. In a comprehensive sample of more than 400 mammals, paternal care, the more reliable sort of allomaternal care, shows positive correlated evolution with relative brain size (Table 1, Fig. 3a). Alloparental care had an additive effect on brain size, but only if it was associated with reliable paternal care (combined care) (Table 1). Furthermore, paternal care always had a stronger effect on brain size than alloparental or combined care (Table 1). In contrast, alloparental care, which is more variable in amount and time, showed a significant or nearly significant positive relationship with fertility (Table 2, Fig. 3b). This positive correlation between fertility and alloparental care is mainly driven by the positive effect of alloparental care on fertility in species with combined care (paternal and alloparental care); indeed, species with alloparental care only did not show increased fertility (Table 2). Increases in brain size or fertility rate seem to be largely independent selective responses as paternal care was correlated with brain size but not fertility, and alloparental care only had an additive effect on brain size when paternal care was already present (Tables 1 and 2). Our results are robust with respect to different coding schemes of allomaternal care (binary or continuous) (Supplementary Material ESM 3, Tables S2, S3). We also found no difference in the results when we controlled for various possible confounding variables (gregariousness, diet, diurnality and substrate use) (Supplementary Material ESM 3, Tables S4-S7), suggesting that these findings are not spurious by-products of other correlations. Our results are also strikingly similar for different statistical approaches (simple model selection vs multi-model averaging) (Supplementary Material ESM 3, Tables S8-S15).

Table 1 Allomaternal care and brain size: continuous classification of allomaternal care. Phylogenetic generalised least squares models with log (brain size) as the dependent variable and paternal care, alloparental care, combined care and log (body mass) as independent variables (N = 476). The best-fitting model is highlighted in italics
Fig. 3
figure 3

Relative brain size is positively correlated with the amount of paternal care (a). Fertility (corrected for body mass) shows a positive relationship with alloparental care (b). Details of phylogenetic models are shown in Tables 1 and 2. Species values are listed in the Supplementary Material (ESM 1). For a graphical representation of the same correlations but with suborder mean values, see Fig. S2 (Supplementary Material ESM 4)

Table 2 Allomaternal care and fertility: continuous classification of allomaternal care. Phylogenetic generalised least squares models with log (fertility) as the dependent variable and paternal care, alloparental care, combined care and log (body mass) as independent variables (N = 443). The best-fitting model is highlighted in italics

Discussion

Reproduction is energetically very expensive, especially in taxa with fast life histories (e.g. Zenuto et al. 2002; McNab 2006; Speakman 2008). The expensive brain hypothesis therefore postulates that one major pathway toward the evolution of relatively larger brains is reduced allocation to reproduction (Isler and van Schaik 2009a). One way for females to achieve such a reduction in reproductive costs is by distributing these costs over other individuals such as the breeding male or non-breeding group members. However, studies investigating the relationship between brain size and allomaternal care have so far produced inconclusive results. We suspected that this happened because they did not separate between care provided by the breeding male (paternal care) and care by non-breeding helpers (alloparental care). By separating these two care types, we found that allomaternal help by the breeding male was correlated with the presence of larger brains, but not with fertility. Allomaternal help by others was correlated with the presence of higher fertility and only correlated with larger brains in species with combined care (where breeding males also help). Allomaternal help by others in the absence of male care had no influence on either fertility or brain size.

Allomaternal care and brain size

In accordance with the expensive brain hypothesis, we found that an additional influx of energy in the form of predictable paternal care toward the mother and the offspring is associated with an evolutionary increase in brain size, whereas care provided by non-breeding group members, which is more variable in amount and time, only had an influence on brain size if reliable paternal care was also present. Indeed, abundant studies show that the identity of the caretaker, be it the breeding male or other helpers, influences the predictability of care received by the breeding female and her offspring. For instance, in banded mongoose (Mungos mungo) (Nichols et al. 2012; Marshall et al. 2016), adult males (potential fathers) maintained or even increased their investment in care as food supply decreased, whereas non-breeding group members helped less when food was scarce. Similarly, in wolves (Canis lupus) and African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus), prey availability affected the ability or willingness of non-breeding pack members to care for the pups whereas the help of the breeding male remained mostly constant (Malcolm and Marten 1982; Harrington et al. 1983). In regions where prey was scarce, wolf pairs produced more surviving pups than did larger packs with additional potential helpers (Harrington et al. 1983), showing that additional potential helpers can even hinder pup survival. Indeed, in wolves and wild dogs, helpers are sometimes fed at the den by other individuals or raid caches near the den, thus intercepting food potentially available for the pups (Haber 1977; Malcolm and Marten 1982; Murie 2011). Furthermore, young helpers in wild dogs, such as yearlings, failed to regurgitate during periods of food scarcity and thus stopped helping (Malcolm and Marten 1982; Creel and Creel 2002). Together, these studies suggest that the amount of alloparental help and the probability of its expression depend on prevailing environmental conditions influencing food availability (see also Macdonald and Moehlman 1982).

Besides ecological conditions, the ability of subordinate helpers to reproduce independently is also expected to influence an individual’s investment in alloparental care. In cooperatively breeding meerkats (Suricata suricatta) (Clutton-Brock et al. 2002), cichlid fish (Neolamprologus pulcher) (Zöttl et al. 2013) and paper wasps (Polistes dominulus) (Tibbetts 2007), dominant individuals stay around and help to raise offspring, whereas subordinates reduce investment in helping behaviours such as babysitting, pup feeding, digging, cooperative foraging or predator defence shortly before dispersing. This resembles patterns found in some other social vertebrates such as prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) (Lonstein and De Vries 2000, 2001), Californian mice (Peromyscus californicus) (Gubernick and Laskin 1994) and Damaraland mole-rats (Fukomys damarensis) (Zöttl et al. 2018), which reduce the amount of alloparental care with increasing chances of independent reproduction. Thus, alloparental help is unpredictable and variable in amount and timing as helpers in various species adjust their caring effort depending on both food availability and future reproduction. Our result that the presence and frequency of paternal care (in addition to maternal care) were much more strongly correlated with brain size than alloparental care (in addition to maternal care) supports the idea that selection favours larger brains whenever females experience a predictable and constant increase in energy.

An additional reason why alloparental care was less important for brain size in our study might also be that the quality of paternal and alloparental care differs. In a huge variety of mammal and bird species, breeding males contribute more to offspring care than alloparents. Examples include African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus) (Malcolm and Marten 1982), banded mongooses (Mungos mungo) (Gilchrist and Russell 2007), striped mice (Rhabdomys pumilio) (Schubert et al. 2009), golden lion tamarins (Leontopithecus rosalia) (Siani 2009), chestnut-crowned babblers (Pomatostomus ruficeps) (Browning et al. 2006), long-tailed tits (Aegithalos caudatus) (MacColl and Hatchwell 2003), white-fronted bee-eaters (Merops bullockoides) (Emlen and Wrege 1991), apostlebirds (Struthidea cinerea) (Woxvold et al. 2006) and laughing kookaburras (Dacelo novaeguineae) (Legge 2000) (but see as counterexamples Bennett and Faulkes 2000; Clutton-Brock et al. 2004). This is likely the case because alloparents, in contrast to breeding males, are often young and unexperienced (Lancaster 1971; Hrdy 1976; Roberts et al. 1998) and did not improve infant survival (Harrington et al. 1983; Jaquish et al. 1997). In agreement with our study, all these results suggest that paternal care is more important for females than the help of others. Finally, a recent comparative study (Heldstab et al. 2017) showed that reproducing females in species with any sort of allomaternal care can afford to reduce reliance on fat reserves, but also that care provided by the breeding male was more important than the help of other non-breeding group members.

The results of our study are also consistent with those of earlier comparative studies. Studies in birds and non-primate taxa found that brain size is associated with an increase in paternal care (West 2014) or biparental care and pair-bonding (Shultz and Dunbar 2007, 2010). The authors of these studies argue that pair-bonding species are more encephalised due to the higher degree of coordination and cooperation that is necessary to maintain stable pair-bonded relationships. Here, we provide an additional or alternative explanation for larger brains in monogamous species: predictably available paternal care, which is particularly common among species with socially monogamous mating systems (Lukas and Clutton-Brock 2013), allows for a constant extra energy input during brain growth, making the evolution of larger brains possible.

As discussed by Isler and van Schaik (2012), our explanation does not require a special explanation for primates, as the extra energy may be used to increase fertility instead of enlarging brain size (see below). Obviously, it is possible to coordinate close pair bonds and cooperative care even with relatively small brains. Finally, our results also explain why the study of corvids (Iwaniuk and Arnold 2004) and cichlid fishes (Reddon et al. 2016) failed to find any effect on brain size: they compared species with male care with those with male care and care by helpers.

In conclusion, our results are consistent with the expensive brain hypothesis, and therefore strongly suggest that brains tend to be as large as the species can afford it energetically. Thus, constraints on brain size, imposed by its energetic costs, may predict brain size better than the fitness benefits of improved cognitive abilities per se.

Allomaternal care and fertility

If allomaternal care is variable and unsteady in amount and timing and therefore does not allow a constant high energy supply for brain growth, we expect that selection will favour mothers who invest the consequent load reduction into the production of a higher number of offspring, rather than in larger-brained offspring. As expected, we found that alloparental care resulted in higher fertility rates, but only in species with combined care. Previous studies in mammals support this finding, showing that females of cooperative breeders commonly have larger litters and shorter interbirth intervals than non-cooperative social species (Garber and Leigh 1997; Mitani and Watts 1997; Moehlman and Hofer 1997; Ross and MacLarnon 2000; Lukas and Clutton-Brock 2012; West and Capellini 2016).

We found that species with alloparental care where breeding males do not help (alloparental care only) show no increase in fertility. One explanation for this finding is that in these species, alloparental care is less systematic than in species with combined care, such as cooperative breeders. For instance, the frequencies of occurrence of alloparental care behaviours in species where males provide no care are relatively low. For instance, we see only 1% alloparental carrying in black-and-white ruffed lemurs (Varecia variegata), 8.4% in Venezuelan red howlers (Alouatta seniculus), 10.3% in patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas), 5% alloparental provisioning in red slender lorises (Loris tardigradus), spectral tarsiers (Tarsius tarsier) or 10% babysitting in ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Furthermore, some attempts to provide alloparental help have been shown to be detrimental to mothers and infants (Hrdy 1976; Silk 1980; Malcolm and Marten 1982; Sommer 1989; Maestripieri 1994). In our dataset, this was also found in various species with alloparental care only, namely guerezas (Colobus guereza) (Wooldridge 1969), Lowe’s monkeys (Cercopithecus lowei) (Bourlière 1970), vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus aethiops) (Gartlan 1969) and patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas) (Zucker and Kaplan 1981).

In our study, paternal care (in addition to maternal care) showed no correlation with fertility. There is some controversy in the literature as to whether paternal care is related to increased fertility in mammals. The comparative studies that find higher fertility (larger litters: (Lukas and Clutton-Brock 2013) or reduced birth intervals (West and Capellini 2016)) did not distinguish between male care only and male care plus alloparental care (cooperative breeding), making it impossible to disentangle their separate effects. Stockley and Hobson (2016) made this distinction, and separately analysed species with paternal care only (without cooperative breeding). However, they used a very conservative definition of cooperative breeding which led, among many others, to the classification of some callitrichids, e.g. Saguinus labiatus and Cebuella pygmaea, as non-cooperative breeders. In this sample, and only for provisioning but not for other helping behaviours, they found higher fertility in species with paternal care only. In conclusion, in our study, we found that fertility was increased in species with alloparental care but mainly due to species with combined care showing higher fertility rates. However, future studies should examine in more detail whether the positive effect of paternal care on fertility found in previous studies is solely driven by species with combined care.

Allomaternal care and human evolution

Humans have the largest relative brain size across the whole animal kingdom (Jerison 1973; Striedter 2005). The costs of sustaining such a large brain are extremely high as humans spend about 20–25% of their resting metabolism on the brain (neonates even up to 60%) (Mink et al. 1981). Among hunter-gatherers, we also see intensive paternal care, mainly in the form of provisioning (male contribution to subsistence) (Hewlett 1993; Marlowe 1999, 2000; Quinlan 2007; Dyble et al. 2016).

Humans also stand out among apes by having a relatively high reproductive output. The main underlying difference is our system of cooperative care for infants and mothers (Hrdy 2005; Burkart et al. 2009; Burkart and van Schaik 2010). Allomaternal care among human foragers is provided by both reproductive men and non-reproductive group members of both sexes and of kin and non-kin, comprising help in the form of food provisioning, carrying, protecting and babysitting the infants (Hawkes et al. 1998; Hill and Hurtado 2009; Hrdy 2009; Dyble et al. 2016; Jaeggi et al. 2016). Humans therefore resemble other mammalian species with combined care (where the male and other non-breeding group members help) in this respect by having higher reproductive rates compared to other species, including other apes, with bi-parental care or maternal care only. However, in contrast to other mammals, where the amount of alloparental care is highly variable depending on various factors such as the age of the parents, group composition and variation in environmental conditions, alloparental care provided by humans is very stable and reliable, at least among foragers. Universal notions of fairness, equity and social punishment, e.g. through excluding uncooperative individuals from future help, sustain cooperation among foragers, including alloparental care (e.g. Hill et al. 1993; Gurven 2004). As a result, mothers are even provided with more food than they actually need to cover the maximum additional costs of gestation or lactation (Kaplan et al. 2000; Butte and King 2005; Sellen 2007). We therefore suggest that the unusual stability of energy input due to alloparental care allowed humans not only to increase their reproductive output but also to evolve even larger brains than other mammalian species with combined care resulting in the rapid and unparalleled brain expansion during hominin evolution (cf. Burkart et al. 2009; Burkart and van Schaik 2010; van Schaik and Burkart 2010).