Introduction

The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is located in the anterior portion of the cerebral cortex and integrates sensory information from association areas located in the parietal, temporal, and occipital lobes, and within the limbic system (Miller and Choen 2001; Corbetta and Shulman 2002; Kimoto et al. 2011). The PFC plays an important role in motivational and cognitive control processes, such as planning, decision making, and social behavior, and plays a central role in executive functions (Pinti et al. 2015). The relationship between modulations of PFC activity and motivational and cognitive control processes has been reported by several clinical researchers. Damasio et al. (1994) reported the famous case of Phineas Gage who, after an industrial accident, had PFC lesions and become irreverent and capricious. He also exemplified deficits in decision making, cognitive, social, and behavioral functions. Sabri et al. (1999) reported that dementia, or other neuropsychological deficits, are reflected exclusively by functional imaging parameters (regional CBF and regional glucose metabolism) in patients with proven cerebral microangiopathy. Hornberger et al. (2014) reported that insight loss, which is a major feature of frontotemporal dementia, correlated with ventromedial and frontopolar prefrontal atrophy across a large cohort of neurodegenerative patients, including frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease patients. Matsuo et al. (2003) investigated the relationship between alterations in the hemodynamic response of the PFC during a verbal fluency task, and memory function in a patient with post-traumatic stress disorder. The authors reported a significant correlation between the total hemoglobin in the PFC and the Wechsler Memory Scale score. Moreover, an aging population has led to an exponential increase in the number of patients with dementia. According to the Alzheimer’s Disease International report (2015), this number is estimated to reach 131.5 million by 2050. Thus, it is particularly important to better understand the functions of the PFC, which underlies normal cognitive functioning.

Previous work has also identified an association between cognitive function and physical activity. For example, Weuve et al. (2004) reported that higher levels of physical activity were associated with better cognitive performance and less cognitive decline in women over the age of 70. Similarly, a prospective cohort study from Abbott et al. (2004) reported that physically capable, elderly men, who walk on a regular basis, are less likely to experience an accelerated cognitive decline and have a reduced risk of dementia. Yaffe et al. (2001) also reported that elderly people (aged 65 and above), with greater baseline physical activity, such as increased walking distance per week, were less likely to experience cognitive declines over a 6- to 8-year follow-up. In addition, many epidemiological studies have reported a relationship between cognitive and oral functions. For example, Yamamoto et al. (2012) reported, over a 4-year follow-up, that incomplete mastication, because of poor dental health (missing teeth, etc.), was associated with a higher risk of dementia in populations over 65 years. Kaye et al. (2010) and Batty et al. (2013) reported that the risk of cognitive decline increased as more teeth, which are essential for mastication, were lost. However, it is not yet clear which aspect of mastication, or tooth loss, underlies the significant association with cognitive function, or whether these factors are also associated with PFC activity.

In our previous work, we focused on the relationship between PFC activity and sensory integration during a motor task; we found that periodontal afferent input significantly affected PFC activity during maintenance of occlusal force, with or without local anesthesia, which can eliminate sensory information from the periodontal ligament (Higaki et al. 2016). However, the relationship between PFC activity and sensory information from the periodontal ligament, or resulting performance of a motor task, has not yet been evaluated in detail. In the present study, we focused on the differences in distribution density of periodontal mechanoreceptors in the incisors and molars, and differences of significant sensory receptors according to the interocclusal distance. The hypothesis of this study was that PFC activity during a biting task would be different when biting with incisors and molars based on the interocclusal distance. The relationship between cognitive and oral functions has been suggested by many epidemiological studies. Our work here provides proof-of-concept of this relationship and serves to clarify this point raised in brain research.

The purpose of the present study was to investigate the effect of different interocclusal distances, during occlusion of incisors and molars, on PFC activity, and performance of a motor task requiring maintenance of occlusal force. We additionally sought to clarify the effect of PFC activity on this motor task.

Materials and methods

Participants

Eleven young, healthy Japanese participants (7 males and 4 females with a mean age of 28.0 ± 3.7 years) attending the Tokushima University of Japan, volunteered to participate in this study. All participants had normal dentitions without any stomatognathic, or central nervous system complications. This study was conducted with the approval of the Ethics Committee of the Tokushima University Hospital (No. 1780) and all experiments were carried out in accordance with the approved guidelines.

PFC activation

PFC activation was evaluated using a wearable functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) device that measured cerebral blood flow (CBF). The fNIRS device measures relative changes in the concentration of oxy-hemoglobin using light attenuation at two wavelengths of 705 nm and 830 nm, which easily pass through skin, tissue, and bone. A total of 16 measuring probes were used in the study, out of which 10 were spaced at intervals of 30 mm and covered three measurement areas: Brodmann’s area 10, and the right and left side of Brodmann’s area 46. Brodmann’s area 10 is located across the frontal pole within the frontal lobe, and Brodmann’s area 46 is divided into right and left sides, dorsolateral to the PFC.

Experimental task and protocol

The experimental task involved maintaining occlusal force within an instructed range under three different external feedback conditions and was based on a study by Higaki et al. (2016). Figure 1 shows the experimental apparatus used. We tested either the left side central incisors, or first molars. Participants were seated on a chair, in a relaxed position, and asked to bite down on an occlusal force transducer with both upper and lower target teeth engaged and to maintain an instructed occlusal strength value. The occlusal force transducer was modified using an auto-curing acrylic resin: a commercial load cell was fitted to the occlusal surface to accommodate the bite of the upper and lower target teeth. Signals from the occlusal force transducer were digitized using a digital data acquisition device with an indicator and transferred to a computer.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Experimental configuration

Measurements were performed in a sound-proof room. Any external disturbances were minimized, and room lighting and temperature were kept constant. Participants were instructed to continuously bite the occlusal force transducer with a force of 27.5 ± 2.5 N (instructed range) for 30 s. Before the experimental task, participants practiced maintaining occlusal force for 2 min, while watching the occlusal force value displayed on the indicator.

All participants completed this same task under three different external feedback conditions (participants were allowed to rest for 1 min between each task, and the order of conditions was randomized). External feedback conditions provided information to guide participants in maintaining occlusal force. Three feedback conditions were used: (1) visual feedback using green and red LED lights. A green light was illuminated to indicate if the measured occlusal force value was within the instructed range, and a red light when it was not; (2) auditory feedback that consisted of sounding a buzzer when occlusal force left the instructed range; and (3) no external feedback information was provided. A series of measurements related to CBF in Brodmann’s areas were taken as these tasks were performed.

In addition, we modified the occlusal force transducer to measure four biting conditions defined as follows: (1) occlusion with the central incisors with an interocclusal distance of 5 mm (BI-5 mm); or (2) 10 mm (BI-10 mm); (3) occlusion with the first molars with an interocclusal distance of 5 mm (BM-5 mm), or (4) 10 mm (BM-10 mm). Tasks under a single biting condition were repeated 4 times under each external feedback condition (12 tasks total). Additionally, each biting condition was conducted on a separate day.

Data analysis

Data from the fNIRS and occlusal force transducer were synchronized and analyzed. The time resolution of both signals was 200 ms. The mean CBFs for 10 s immediately after measurement were defined as the baseline and used to correct data obtained during each task. The changes in CBF at task completion were calculated by subtracting the CBF at task onset. CBF values were analyzed in accordance with our previous study method (Higaki et al. 2016). That is, regional CBF values were calculated using average probe data from each area, as follows: probes 7, 8, and 9 for the left side of Brodmann’s area 46; probes 11, 12, 13, and 14 for Brodmann’s area 10; and probes 15, 16, and 17 for the right side of Brodmann’s area 46. The mean occlusal force from 10 to 30 s after task onset was calculated and compared between biting conditions and feedback conditions to eliminate early adjustments, such as rapid force rising, that occurred before reaching a constant occlusal force. The Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used to compare CBF in the PFC that occurred under different biting conditions. All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS® version 24.0 software (IBM Corp. Armonk, NY, USA), and significance was defined as p < 0.05.

Results

Figure 2 shows typical topographical patterns of CBF in the PFC just after the completion of each task. In the BM-5 mm task, a strong increase in CBF was observed in almost all oral regions, regardless of the external feedback condition. In the BI-5 mm, BI-10 mm, and BM-10 mm tasks, an increase in CBF in almost all regions or partly decrease in CBF was observed.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Typical topographical patterns of cerebral blood flow in the prefrontal cortex. Red and blue colors indicate increases and decreases of regional cerebral blood flow, respectively

Table 1 shows regional CBF changes upon completion of each task. The highest regional CBF values were observed during the BM-5 mm task; these were significantly higher than those recorded for the BM-10 mm task, and this trend was maintained in all three areas of the PFC measured. No significant differences were found between the BI-5 mm, BI-10 mm, and BM-10 mm biting conditions, regardless of the measurement area and the presence, or absence, of feedback information.

Table 1 Changes in regional cerebral blood flow in each condition

Table 2 shows the mean occlusal force values during each task. The mean occlusal force in the BM-5 mm task was lower than the instructed range, regardless of feedback information, and resulted in significantly lower values than those recorded during the BM-10 mm task (Visual feedback: p = 0.021, Auditory feedback: p = 0.05, No feedback: p = 0.026). In the biting task with incisors, no significant difference in occlusal force was found between the BI-5 mm and BI-10 mm biting conditions, regardless of the external feedback method used. However, the mean occlusal force in the tasks performed without feedback information did not reach the instructed range and showed significantly lower values than those tasks performed with feedback information.

Table 2 Mean occlusal force executed in each condition

Discussion

Several studies concerning periodontal sensation and brain activity, including in the PFC, have been reported. Tamura et al. (2002) reported that brain activity in the motor and premotor cortexes increased during teeth clenching. Takada and Miyamoto (2004) reported that activity in the dorsolateral and ventral PFC increased during a gum-chewing task, but not during a sham chewing task (i.e., just opening-closing movements of the jaw). In addition, Iida et al. (2010) reported that PFC activity, during a teeth-clenching task, was greater than that during a fist-clenching task. These reports were based on functional magnetic resonance imaging data, with which it is difficult to assess the quantitative relationship between task performance and brain activity, or to clarify how PFC activity is related to the task. In the present study, we tried to clarify how the PFC is related to occlusal force control by comparing PFC activity between various motor tasks in a more physiological state using fNIRS.

In the present study, regional CBF was increased in all three PFC areas during the BM-5 mm tasks, both with and without feedback information. However, the regional CBFs in the PFC were not increased in continuous biting with BM-5 mm when periodontal sensation around the molars was inhibited by local anesthetic, as found in our previous report. This present study also found that regional CBF in the PFC did not increase in the BM-10 mm, BI-5 mm, or BI-10 mm biting conditions. This suggests that afferent sensory input affects PFC activity. Compared to periodontal sensations, somatic sensations in the temporomandibular joint and muscle spindles of jaw-closing muscles are primarily responsible for maintaining occlusal force. Manly et al. (1952) reported that periodontal sensation, however, dominates maintenance of the occlusal force when the interocclusal distance is less than 5 mm. Conversely, Christensen and Morimoto (1977) reported that somatic sensations in the temporomandibular joint and muscle spindles are primarily responsible for maintaining occlusal force when the interocclusal distance is greater than 10 mm. Regarding the significance of sensations from the temporomandibular joint and muscle spindles during mastication, Hidaka et al. (1999) and Inoue et al. (1981) reported that the timing of the facilitatory masseteric response is primarily controlled by muscle spindles, while the magnitude of the facilitatory masseteric response is primarily controlled by muscle spindles, and partly by periodontal receptors. At times when there is weak jaw-closing muscle activity, in the early stages of occlusion, periodontal sensation ensures that the occlusal force is executed to firmly catch hold of food with the teeth (Goldberg 1971; Trulsson and Johansson 1994). Once the occlusal force reaches a certain threshold, the periodontal sensation inhibits the excessive contraction of jaw-closing muscles to prevent tissue destruction due to overstressing (Brodin et al. 1993; Türker et al. 1994). Basically, the main role of molars is to grind food and, in contrast, incisors play a more prominent role in cutting food. Thus, form reflects function, and the molars can exert a much greater occlusal force than the incisors due to the anatomical structure of gnathological region (Gosen 1974). Moreover, it has been reported that incisors have a greater distribution of mechanoreceptors in the periodontal ligament than molars (Maeda 1994). Therefore, the periodontal sensation in the molar might ensure that excessive occlusal force is not executed through PFC activity.

During a sensory-motor task, such as eating, mastication and occlusal force control are mainly regulated by the chewing central pattern generator (CPG) located in the medulla oblongata, downstream of the PFC (Lund et al. 1998; Lund and Kolta 2006). It has recently been suggested that the CPG is influenced by higher brain centers (Morquette et al. 2012). Peripheral sensory information is transmitted to the primary somatosensory cortex and subsequently processed in multiple association areas (Johansson et al. 1988; Morissette and Bower 1996). The processed information can then be transmitted to the motor cortex directly, or via the PFC. Here, we present a hypothesis that the PFC contributes to inhibition of excessive occlusal force executed to molars. The motor cortex might receive the processed information directly, and be able to control occlusal force based on the CPG, without being actively assisted or supervised by the PFC. This could occur when the periodontal sensation in the molar region is not primarily responsible for maintaining occlusal force and there is sufficient afferent sensation from the temporomandibular joint and muscle spindles, as in the BI-5 mm, BI-10 mm, and BM-10 mm conditions. In the BM-5 mm condition, the periodontal sensation in the molar region was primarily responsible for maintaining occlusal force, and there were fewer inputs from the temporomandibular joint and muscle spindles. In this condition, the PFC might act as a supervisory attentional system to ensure that excessive occlusal force is not executed to the teeth. The supervisory attentional system, proposed by Shallice (1982), is an executive monitoring system that controls contention scheduling, i.e., the process by which routine actions are selected and others are inhibited. Our finding of an average occlusal force level in the BM-5 mm condition, that was lower than the instructed range, supports our hypothesis.

Visual and auditory feedback information significantly affected CBF in the PFC in the BM-5 mm condition, compared with CBF in the task without feedback information. In our previous study, CBF in the PFC was higher when sensory integration was required to complete the task, i.e., between the periodontal sensation and feedback information (Higaki et al. 2016). Both of the findings above indicate that the PFC plays a significant role in sensory integration. However, we found no difference in CBF in the PFC, regardless of whether feedback information was used, when the periodontal sensation in the molar region was not primarily responsible for maintaining occlusal force in the task.

We used a task which required the participant to maintain constant occlusal force consciously in this study. Regarding the influence of a jaw motor task on changes in the central nervous system, several reports have been published that illustrate a motor task can affect central nervous system changes irrespective of the level of consciousness. For example, studies utilizing conscious motor tasks, such as by Iida et al. (2014), reported in a study using transcranial magnetic stimulation that a consciously repeated tooth-clenching task could trigger neuroplastic changes in the central nervous system. Moreover, Zhang et al. (2016), who also using transcranial magnetic stimulation, reported that a “Hold-and-split” task (participants hold and spit out a test tablet with their incisors) induced signs of neuroplastic changes in the corticomotor pathways related to the masseter muscle. Unconscious tasks, such as bruxism (teeth clenching/grinding) during sleep, have also been reported to trigger central nervous system responses, specifically PFC activity. Ikuta et al. (2019) suggested that sleep bruxism might be associated with a significant reduction in the excitability of corticomotor pathways related to the masseter muscle. However, in the above-mentioned reports, the relationship between jaw motor tasks and the PFC were not discussed. Further research on the PFC activity that occurs during repetitive jaw motor tasks, in addition to continuous jaw motor tasks, will be needed.

To conclude, our results suggest that the PFC contributes to inhibition of excessive occlusal force executed by the teeth when the occlusal force is maintained at the molars with an interocclusal distance of 5 mm, in which the periodontal sensation is primarily responsible for maintaining occlusal force and there are few afferent inputs from the temporomandibular joint and muscle spindles.