Introduction

Substantial gaps remain in our understanding of the causes of cognitive impairment (CI) among treated HIV-infected patients in the era of combination antiretroviral therapies (cART). The frequency of HIV-associated neurocognitive impairment (HAND) remains unexpectedly high among patients receiving cART (Tozzi et al. 2007). A study conducted with over 1500 community dwelling HIV-infected subjects attending academic centers found that about one half performed below expectations on cognitive tests, although a substantial proportion of these individuals did not have suppression of virus in plasma (Heaton et al. 2010). HIV-specific factors remain important, given findings that the quantitative burden of HIV DNA in CD14+ cells and, separately, levels of plasma CD163 are associated with HAND among individuals with undetectable plasma HIV RNA (Burdo et al. 2013; Valcour et al. 2013). Increasingly, HIV-associated factors, such as medication toxicities, are under consideration, particularly in the setting of an aging population (Clifford and Ances 2013).

Despite availability of newer antiretroviral medications, metabolic dysfunction remains, perhaps owing to multiple factors including chronic inflammation and the long-term exposure to antiretroviral medications (Srinivasa and Grinspoon 2014). Previous analyses using data from the early cART era identified associations between metabolic derangements, principally insulin resistance (IR) and diabetes mellitus (DM), and cognition, but more recent work has brought this association into question (McCutchan et al. 2012; Valcour et al. 2012). Because the strongest evidence was based on cohorts enrolled during the early cART era, when antiretroviral medications were more closely linked to mitochondrial dysfunction, it is possible that previous associations represented epiphenomenon rather than links between metabolic disorders and cognition (Valcour et al. 2006). Inconsistencies from more recent work could be clarified with assessments completed in a larger cohort during the proximal cART era.

The Women’s Interagency HIV Study (WIHS) is a longitudinal cohort of HIV-infected and uninfected women and has a multiracial constitution, generally lower levels of education than other cohorts and more individuals with high body mass index (BMI) (Bacon et al. 2005; Barkan et al. 1998; Boodram et al. 2009). In a previous retrospective analysis using a brief neuropsychological testing battery limited to three tests, we identified worse performance associated with higher homeostasis model assessment (HOMA) on the Stroop color-naming task, a test of psychomotor speed (Valcour et al. 2012). In 2009, a more comprehensive 1-h battery was added to the core WIHS visits where concurrent fasting blood sampling occurred. This afforded an opportunity for a more comprehensive assessment of cognitive subdomains in relation to IR and an assessment of whether these associations remained in the proximal cART era.

Methods

Subject selection

The WIHS is a multicenter longitudinal observational cohort of HIV-infected and uninfected women who, during the time of this study, were enrolled from one of six US sites: New York (Bronx and Brooklyn), California (Los Angeles and San Francisco), Washington DC, and Chicago. Women initially enrolled in the WIHS had to be able to attend an outpatient study visit. All subjects signed institutional review board-approved consent forms.

An extended neuropsychological testing battery was added to the WIHS exam beginning in April 2009 and continuing through the first wave of testing which ended in April 2011. Of the active English-speaking WIHS participants (n = 1908), 1594 (84 %) completed the test battery of whom 1273 had concurrent fasting blood work within the prior 6 months. Subjects were excluded from this analysis for (a) presence of conditions that limit test validity (e.g., hearing loss, impaired vision, immediate influence of illicit substances, n = 5); (b) history of stroke (n = 11); and (c) self-reported use of antipsychotic medication in the past 6 months (n = 40). Individuals with diabetes, defined as having a self-reported diagnosis, a fasting glucose of over 125 mg/dL, or being on diabetic medications, were analyzed separately (n = 237). Thus, we evaluated 994 (659 HIV-infected; 62 % of the active cohort) for the insulin resistance analyses.

Cognitive characterization

The neuropsychological battery was designed by neuroAIDS experts to allow future diagnostic characterization using HAND criteria; HAND diagnoses have not been applied to the cohort to date and were therefore not available for these analyses. Our battery included the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (HVLT); Stroop Test; Trail Making Test Parts A and B; Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Letter Fluency (F, A, S); Semantic Fluency (animals); Grooved Pegboard; and Letter-Number Sequencing Test (LNS). The LNS measures verbal working memory from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale IV (WAIS IV). For the control condition (attention), the test requires the participants to listen to a list of letters and numbers and then repeat back the string of letters and numbers exactly in the order in which they were stated. For the experimental condition (working memory), the test requires the participant to listen to a list of letters and numbers and then repeat back the numbers in ascending order and the letters in alphabetical order. All testers were internally certified through structured training and quality assurance.

Metabolic and clinical variables

Fasting specimens for glucose determination were collected in tubes with glycolytic inhibitors. Serum for insulin was obtained at the same time and was stored (−70 °C) until the day of assay. Plasma glucose was measured using the hexokinase method, and insulin was measured using the IMMULITE 2000 assay at a central laboratory (Quest Diagnostics, Baltimore, MD). Insulin resistance was estimated using the HOMA, defined as (insulin X glucose)/405 with insulin measured in μU/mL and glucose measured in mg/dL (Matthews et al. 1985). We confirmed HIV status by FDA-approved enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay with Western blot when positive. Plasma HIV RNA levels and CD4 T-lymphocyte counts were completed using standard techniques. Hypertension was defined as having a systolic blood pressure >140 mmHg, diastolic blood pressure >90 mmHg, and/or self-reported use of antihypertensive medications. Heavy alcohol use was defined as reporting >7 drinks per week or more than 4 drinks in one sitting. Hepatitis C was defined using both HCV Ab and HCV RNA levels. Negative for HCV was operationalized as being HCV Ab negative or having absent HCV RNA in those with HCV AB+. Subjects who were HCV AB+ with detectable or untested HCV RNA were coded as HCV positive.

Statistical analyses

We examined demographic characteristics by quartiles of HOMA using ANOVAs for continuous variables and chi-squared tests for categorical variables. We used a regression-based approach to create demographically corrected normative standards (t-scores) for individual neuropsychological tests, as previously published (Maki et al. 2015; Rubin et al. 2014). The regression equations were based on the larger sample of WIHS women (N = 1521). The Trail Making tests, Stroop, Grooved Pegboard scores, and HOMA were skewed and thus log-transformed before inclusion in our models. We took the average of dominant and non-dominant hands for our composite Grooved pegboard measure. In the overall sample, we used multivariable regression analyses to examine the separate and interactive associations of HOMA and HIV status on cognitive performance. The models adjusted for confounders including body mass index; waist-to-hip ratio; fasting cholesterol; marijuana use; crack, cocaine, and/or heroin use; alcohol use; antidepressant medication use; depressive symptoms; HCV infection; income; and study site. We also adjusted for number of prior exposures to the Stroop (range 1–4), HVLT (range 1–2), SDMT (range 1–5), and the Trail Making Test (range 1–5). Interactions between HOMA and HIV status were retained in the final model if p < 0.10; stratum-specific estimates within HIV status are reported for interactions. For significant interactions (p < 0.05), the final model was re-run for the HIV-infected women only including the additional covariates of recent CD4 count and viral load, nadir CD4 count, and cART use and adherence. Planned exploratory analyses in HIV-infected women were also conducted to examine interactions between HOMA and HIV clinical characteristics (HIV viral load, recent and nadir CD4 count, cART use and adherence. Due to multiple comparisons, we defined significance at the p < 0.01 level and trends for p < 0.05. Analyses were performed using SAS PROC GENMOD (version 9.4, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC).

Results

About two thirds of the women were HIV-infected; mean age ranged from 43 to 46 years accross groups, and women reported completing about 12 years of education (Table 1). Women in the higher quartiles of HOMA were more often HIV-uninfected women (p = 0.003). Metabolic parameters also differed by group with the highest HOMA quartile having a higher BMI (33.5 kg/m2), more hypertension (44 %), and a greater waist-hip ratio (0.93). Depression and hepatitis C co-infection were also more common among those in the highest HOMA quartile. Substantial differences in former or current crack, cocaine, heroin, or marijuana use were not identified (data not shown), but those in the highest quartile of HOMA were more likely to report abstaining from alcohol (61 %, p = 0.01). The women were largely on cART, and most were adherent with only 10–20 % having plasma HIV RNA levels >10,000 copies/mL.

Table 1 Baseline demographic and clinical characteristics of the women by quartile of HOMA-IR

In unadjusted models, increasing HOMA was significantly associated with poorer cognitive test performance on Stroop trials 1 and 2, phonemic fluency, Grooved Pegboard, and LNS attention (p’s < 0.01; Table 2). Trend level associations were noted on HVLT recognition, Stroop trial 3, and SDMT (p’s < 0.05). After adjustment, increasing HOMA remained significantly inversely associated with performance on LNS attention (β = −0.10, p < 0.01) and HVLT recognition (β = −0.10, p < 0.01) with weaker but statistically significant level associations remaining on phonemic fluency (β = −0.09, p = 0.02).

Table 2 Univariate and multivariate models for cognitive outcomes associated with HOMA (log)

We investigated the possibility that HIV status modified the association of HOMA on cognitive test performance. In these analyses, we found a HOMA and HIV status interaction effect on LNS attention (p = 0.01) and on Stroop trials 1 and 2 (p = 0.04) (Fig. 1). Specifically, among women with HOMA of 2.81 or greater, HIV-infected women performed significantly worse than uninfected women on LNS attention and Stroop trials 1 and 2. In multivariable analyses of HIV-infected women only, increasing HOMA was significantly associated with poorer performance on LNS attention (β = −0.15, p = 0.001) and Stroop trials 1 and 2 (β = −0.10, p = 0.02) even after controlling for disease characteristics such as CD4 count, viral load, cART medication use, and duration of ART.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Interaction effects by serostatus for Stroop 1 and 2 tasks combined and for Letter-Number Sequencing Attention Task. There is a decreasing t-score with higher quartile of HOMA-IR in HIV-infected women (declining bar height) that is not seen in HIV-uninfected women. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01

Exploratory analyses examined whether HOMA interacted with any HIV clinical characteristics on LNS attention and Stroop trials 1 and 2. There was a significant interaction between HOMA and nadir CD4 count on Stroop trials 1 and 2 (p = 0.04) after adjusting for other clinical characteristics. In follow-up analyses, HOMA was negatively associated with Stroop trials 1 and 2 performance in HIV-infected women with nadir CD4 counts >200 cells/μL (p < 0.01). No other significant interactions were noted on either Stroop trials 1 and 2 or LNS attention

We completed a separate analysis to determine if having diabetes was associated with worse performance on individual tests (Table 3). Here, isolated effects were identified on one test of manual dexterity/psychomotor speed (grooved pegboard test), noting that diabetes was associated with worse performance on this timed measure.

Table 3 Univariate and multivariate models for cognitive outcomes associated with diabetes

Discussion

HIV-infection in the current treatment era has been linked to conditions that are HIV-associated but not necessarily directly due to infection itself (Justice and Braithwaite 2012). These conditions increase in frequency with age and have emerged to have a greater influence on mortality than HIV-associated events (Palella et al. 2006). Similarly, cognitive impairment among individuals with access to treatment is influenced by both HIV-related and unrelated factors. (Clifford and Ances 2013) In a recent multicenter study, the probability of cognitive impairment increased from less than 30 % among cases without confounding or contributing factors to nearly 80 % among those with such conditions, including head trauma or cerebrovascular events (Heaton et al. 2010).

In the present work, we confirm contributions to cognitive impairment from metabolic factors by identifying an influence of both IR and DM on neuropsychological test performance. Our work adds further support for HIV-unrelated factors contributing to cognitive abilities in the current treatment era. We extend our past work by identifying an HIV-related modulation in the effect of insulin resistance where effects are noted only among HIV-infected women. It is possible that this modulation is not specific to HIV but instead represents an enhanced vulnerability when a second cognitive syndrome exists, in this case, that related to HIV infection; but, exploratory analyses suggest a stronger effect among patients without low CD4 nadir counts, somewhat contrary to this hypothesis. Past work in HIV-uninfected populations has predominantly isolated associations in those with mild cognitive impairment or early Alzheimer’s disease (Craft and Watson 2004). This is supported by our findings of strongest effects in attention measures, known to be associated with HIV.

A number of factors were noted to differ in the group with the highest levels of HOMA, including metabolic factors (BMI, hypertension) and hepatitis C. This group was also more likely to abstain from alcohol use, a possible reflection of current or past hepatitis C, and thus counseling to reduce further injury to the liver or past alcohol use, although we noted no difference in rates for self-reported past treatment for alcoholism in those currently abstaining compared to non-abstainers among individuals where that information was available (p = 0.20, n = 474). Although our models adjusted for these variables, this finding remains of interest. It may not be possible to completely adjust for these factors, raising suspicion that IR reflects a more global picture of metabolic and hepatic dysfunction being detrimental to cognition in the current HIV treatment era.

Our work utilized internal controls to develop t-scores, an approach that strengthens our ability to isolate HIV-specific effects, as WIHS HIV-uninfected women were enrolled because of similar risk behaviors. However, it limits our ability to interpret the magnitude of HOMA effects external to the WIHS, since our t-scores would not be expected to match those from published norms or be similar for the general population. This issue is highlighted in Fig. 1, where average performance appears to be greater than 50, due to women contributing to this analysis having slightly higher performance than the full WIHS HIV-uninfected women (not that of the general population represented in published normative data tables).

In summary, we identify an association between insulin resistance and, separately, diabetes with neuropsychological testing performance in cohort of women who have access to cART and who, in large part, are adherent to treatment. These data confirm an influence of metabolic derangements in the current treatment era and provide further evidence that factors not specific to HIV continue to influence cognition.