Introduction

Investment in adaptive behaviours is a key element of animal success across a wide range of circumstances. One example of adaptive behaviour is that which provides resistance to parasite or pathogen infection (hereafter parasites). Parasite avoidance and other anti-parasite behaviours form part of the “behavioural immune system” (BIS) that has become increasingly recognised as a vital first line of defence against infection (Schaller and Park 2011; Ackerman et al. 2018; Amoroso 2021). Anti-parasite behaviours have been reported in a variety of animals, including crustaceans, amphibians, fish, birds, and mammals (Kiesecker et al. 1999; Karvonen et al. 2004; Behringer et al. 2006; Bush and Clayton 2018; Hart and Hart 2018). Such behaviours include avoidance of infectious stages or infected individuals, self-medication, and behavioural thermoregulation (Hart 1990, 1997; Parker et al. 2011). The benefits of a BIS are substantial given the costs associated with physiological resistance and infection itself (Bonneaud et al. 2003; Curtis 2014), making it critical to identify circumstances under which anti-parasite behaviours may be enhanced or compromised.

Notably, various abiotic and biotic stimuli may represent a challenge for homeostasis (i.e. act as a stressor) and can cause a physiological response that increases levels of various hormones, including glucocorticoids (GCs) in vertebrates (Sapolsky et al. 2000; Bonier et al. 2009). The hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis controls such reactions to stressors in mammals, with the hypothalamic–pituitary–interrenal (HPI) axis doing so for fish, amphibians and reptiles (Romero 2004; Sapolsky et al. 2000; Romero et al. 2009; Sheriff et al. 2011). Specifically, exposure to stressors can activate the HPA/HPI axis, resulting in the secretion of GCs—this is primarily cortisol for many mammals and ray-finned fish, but corticosterone (CORT) for birds, amphibians, and reptiles (Sheriff et al. 2011). In turn, GCs mediate various behavioural and physiological changes to help cope with the stressor(s), including their actions as potent anti-inflammatory agents, but they are also involved in day‐to‐day functions (see reviews by Wingfield et al. 1998; Sapolsky et al. 2000; MacDougall-Shackleton et al. 2019). GCs have thus often been considered as stressor biomarkers (Narayan et al. 2019) even though acute elevations are now typically considered adaptive because they aid in maintaining homeostasis, and can even be immune-stimulating (e.g. MacDougall-Shackleton et al. 2019). However, chronic increases can cause reductions in immunity and other critical activities (Wingfield et al. 1998; Sapolsky et al. 2000; Romero 2004; Martin 2009). For instance, GCs can inhibit leukocyte (white blood-cell—WBC) access to sites of inflammation and induce eosinophil apoptosis (Cain and Cidlowski 2017; Strehl et al. 2019). This said, animals may escape the effects of chronically-elevated GCs by reducing their reaction to stressors through habituation and dampening the GC response (Bennett et al. 2016; Fraker et al. 2009; Bokony et al. 2021).

The concept of ‘eco-physiology’ or ‘conservation physiology’ is thus important when considering that environmental stressors may affect wildlife through a range of physiological effects with various repercussions (Busch and Hayward 2009; Dantzer et al. 2014; Cooke et al. 2021), including their susceptibility to infectious diseases (Blaustein et al. 2012; Kernbach et al. 2018). It has been suggested that individuals in suboptimal environments, or in poor condition, may still be able to maintain behavioural defences against infection in the absence of a robust immune response (Adelman and Martin 2009); however, stress-associated hormone changes may not only affect physiological resistance via effects on innate or acquired immunity, but also the BIS given that elevated GCs are known to influence vertebrate behaviours. Short-term increases in GCs can increase locomotor activity, exploratory behaviour, foraging activity and risk-taking behaviours (Sapolsky et al. 2000; Martin et al. 2005), but chronically high GC levels have been associated with depression-like behaviours (e.g. lethargy and lack of reaction to stimuli in experimental animals—see Kim and Han 2006; Myers et al. 2014; Chen et al. 2016).

The effects of GCs on vertebrate behavioural responses to threats posed by natural enemies, such as predators, have reported varied findings such as reduced foraging and courtship that depend on the prey and predator species involved, as well as cue type (see review by Harris and Carr 2016). For instance, administering exogenous GCs was associated with stronger responses to predator cues in tree lizards (Thaker et al. 2009) and in rats (Rosen et al. 2008), but other authors have reported inhibition of anti-predator behaviour, such as for newts (Neuman-Lee et al. 2015) and tadpoles (Middlemis Maher et al. 2013; Gabor et al. 2019). In addition, fish with higher baseline cortisol levels were better able to maintain anti-predator displays when faced with repeated stressors (Cull et al. 2015). Sapolsky et al. (2000) suggested that higher baseline circulating GCs may better prepare animals to face certain challenges, such as predator attacks, but no clear trend has emerged regarding behavioural defences and activation of the HPA/HPI axis (Harris and Carr 2016).

Given the many similarities between predators and parasites as natural enemies with respect to victim defence strategies (Raffel et al. 2008; Buck et al. 2018), GC alterations could also affect anti-parasite behaviours in manners similar to those reported for predator avoidance. One such pathway would be if GCs directly suppress behaviours involved in parasite avoidance or removal, thereby reducing host resistance to infection. Such effects are critical to consider because behavioural resistance precedes physiological resistance during a host’s interaction with a single parasite (Amoroso 2021), and resistance precedes tolerance, be it behavioural or physiological (Råberg et al. 2007, 2009). Notably, behavioural changes in response to parasites are often mechanistically driven by physiology (Thompson and Kavaliers 1994), so could potentially be influenced by GCs. Changes in physiological resistance (innate and acquired immunity) could also indirectly affect behavioural resistance because these two types of defence are not independent, and are expected to interact with one another (Amoroso and Antonovics 2020; Amoroso 2021), thus representing a second pathway by which GC alterations could affect host anti-parasite behaviours. Both positive and negative covariation between behavioural and immunological traits may be seen, with either type of resistance driving the relationship, or both may be simultaneously affected by a third factor that links them together (see review by Lopes 2017).

Importantly, trade-offs between behavioural and physiological resistance to parasites may be expected because both have associated costs (Hall et al. 2017; Gassen et al. 2018; Gibson and Amoroso 2022). Individuals investing in a robust physiological (immune) response to infection risk should thus exhibit reduced anti-parasite behaviours to compensate for this cost. Conversely, individuals unable to mount an effective physiological response may instead invest more in putatively less-costly behavioural resistance (Curtis 2014). Such negative relationships between behavioural and physiological resistance have been demonstrated at the individual (e.g. Zylberberg et al. 2013, 2014; Stephenson 2019) and population levels (Klemme et al. 2020), but other studies have found a positive relationship (Klemme and Karvonen 2016), or even both positive and negative, depending on the circumstances (Schreier and Grindstaff 2020). Importantly, although physiological mechanisms underlying any behavioural–physiological resistance trade-offs have not been definitively demonstrated to date, GCs represent one potential link (Zylberberg et al. 2014; Schreier and Grindstaff 2020). Altered GCs could thus lead to concomitant changes in host resistance through behaviour or physiology, or both (Lopes 2017).

Here, we investigated whether larval amphibians (tadpoles) experimentally exposed to exogenous CORT were able to maintain effective anti-parasite behaviours, and if these were related to immune measures. Exogenous CORT is a well-established technique to raise endogenous CORT levels in larval amphibians (reviewed by Belden et al. 2005), and here this served as a proxy for a stressor that would result in the latter. In nature, exogenous exposure can also occur via ambient/environmental CORT that can affect aquatic animals (Tornabene et al. 2021) and should be considered in examining effects on host parasitism. Acute and chronic increases in tadpole CORT levels in response to various abiotic and biotic stressors are well-documented, including predation risk (e.g. Middlemis Maher et al. 2013; Bennett et al. 2016) and other challenges such as pond-drying, high conspecific density, pollution, and pathogen infection (e.g. Hopkins et al. 1997; Belden et al. 2007; Denver 2009; Gabor et al. 2013; Bókony et al. 2021). However, CORT production can be downregulated during chronic exposure to stressors (Fraker et al. 2009; Bennett et al. 2016; Bókony et al. 2021), so will not necessarily remain elevated.

As CORT is known to affect larval amphibian behaviour, including anti-predator defences, elevations could also influence anti-parasite behaviours. CORT generally stimulates tadpole locomotion and foraging (e.g. Crespi and Denver 2004, 2005), but elevated CORT enhances or reduces anti-predator behaviours such as freezing depending on the context (e.g. Fraker et al. 2009; Kulkarni and Gramapurohit 2017), and may not even play a role in some circumstances (Hossie et al. 2010). In contrast, bursts of high activity consisting of rapid swimming and twisting are key to the highly effective anti-parasite behaviours displayed by tadpoles from many taxa in the presence of free-swimming infectious stages (cercariae) from several species of trematode (parasitic flatworms). These movements are necessary for cercariae evasion and removal, and their experimental suppression leads to greater infection (e.g., Koprivnikar et al. 2006; 2014; Daly and Johnson 2011; Sears et al. 2013). Such behaviours are important as many trematodes can be pathogenic to larval amphibians (see review by Koprivnikar et al. 2012a). However, tadpole anti-parasite behaviour also shows significant intra- and interspecific variation (Koprivnikar et al. 2012b, 2014), and could be driven by infection risk or other factors, including stressor exposure.

Exogenous CORT could also affect anti-parasite behaviours in tadpoles by influencing their physiological immunity given that these components of resistance are unlikely to be independent of one another (Amoroso and Antonovics 2020; Amoroso 2021). Links between the HPI axis and immunity in larval amphibians are well-established, as well as with metamorphosis, with elevated endogenous GCs having various effects on WBC profiles (Rollins-Smith 2001, 2017; Rollins-Smith and Blair 1993). Critically, exogenous-CORT exposure of tadpoles has been shown to increase trematode infections while also altering WBC profiles (Belden and Kiesecker 2005; LaFonte and Johnson 2013), which are a key component of the larval amphibian immune response to these parasites (Martin and Conn 1990; Holland et al. 2007).

These attributes of our model system allowed us to experimentally expose hosts to a stressor-induced hormone (CORT) known to compromise the immune response against a parasite infection for which there is also effective behavioural resistance. We hypothesised that CORT-exposed tadpoles would exhibit a reduced immune response (via altered WBC profiles) to trematode infection but would correspondingly engage in greater anti-parasite behaviours to compensate if there is an inherent negative trade-off between behavioural and physiological resistance. Conversely, reductions in the BIS owing to elevated GCs would be problematic given the multiple stressors increasingly faced by wildlife that are capable of doing so (Munns 2006), with potentially important consequences in the context of infectious disease dynamics.

Materials and methods

Tadpole rearing and maintenance

Northern leopard frog (Rana (Lithobates) pipiens) egg masses were obtained from a commercial supplier (Boreal Science) and kept at room temperature (~ 22 °C) with a 14:10 h light:dark cycle until tadpoles had hatched. Tadpoles were then housed in ten 5 L tanks with ~ 100 tadpoles in each. Water was changed every second day using dechlorinated water, i.e. DW (tap water containing Nutrafin Aquaplus, A7928 Hagen®, and Nutrafin Cycle, A7906 Hagen®), and tadpoles were fed ad libitum with store-bought organic spinach softened with boiled DW as well as rabbit food pellets (Hagen®). Tadpoles were housed for approximately 3 weeks until a sufficient number reached at least Gosner developmental stage 25 (Gosner 1960); tadpoles at this stage are known to survive exposure to the trematode and exogenous-CORT doses used here (Belden and Kiesecker 2005; Daly and Johnson 2011; LaFonte and Johnson 2013; Koprivnikar et al. 2014). Animals were used in accordance with the Canadian Council on Animal Care guidelines, and with institutional approval from the animal care committee (AUP 633).

Parasite collection

Echinostomatid trematodes have a complex life cycle, whereby the first intermediate host is an aquatic snail within which asexual reproduction by the parasites produces free-swimming cercariae. Many echinostomatids infect tadpoles as their second intermediate host, with cercariae forming cysts in the nephric system that can substantially compromise renal function and decrease survival (Schotthoefer et al. 2003; Szuroczki and Richardson 2009; Koprivnikar et al. 2012b). Previous studies have demonstrated tadpole behavioural evasion and removal of echinostome cercariae (e.g., Koprivnikar et al. 2006, 2012a, 2014). This is a response to initial contact by the cercariae, which often creep down the tadpole’s exterior to reach the cloaca where they must enter to reach the nephric system. For this study, aquatic snails (Helisoma sp.) were collected locally in Ontario from two ponds and screened in the lab for trematode infection using established methods (see Szuroczki and Richardson 2009) to stimulate cercariae emergence by exposing snails to heat and light from a lamp. Emerged cercariae were identified as a species of Echinostoma using a standard key (Schell 1985); snails infected with this trematode were housed separately in DW and fed ad libitum with raw spinach. Whilst the exact identification of Echinostoma sp. would require molecular tools, cercariae of this genus readily infect many amphibians, and tadpoles behaviourally respond in the same manner to different echinostomatid cercariae (e.g. Koprivnikar et al. 2006, 2014). Prior to the tadpole behaviour experiments, Echinostoma sp. cercariae were collected as described above, pooled, and then divided into 1.5 mL microcentrifuge tubes so that each contained 15 cercariae in DW. Cercariae were no older than 4 h before use to avoid reductions in infectivity (McCarthy 1999).

Exogenous corticosterone exposure

We haphazardly removed 9 tadpoles that had reached at least Gosner stage 25 (based on visual inspection) from each of the 10 group housing containers, for a total of 90 tadpoles that were exposed to one of three solution treatments (30/treatment): 1) DW; 2) DW containing the solvent ethanol (0.2 mL of 80% ethanol per 1 L); and, 3) DW containing CORT (Sigma-Aldrich® ≥ 98.5%) at 0.1 µM, made by dissolving 0.0003464 g of CORT in 0.2 mL of 80% ethanol before adding it to 1 L of DW. Treatments 2 and 3 followed similar protocols for exogenous CORT-induced immunosuppression of tadpoles from previous studies (Belden and Kiesecker 2005; LaFonte and Johnson 2013). Exogenous-CORT exposure was used here to simulate a general stressor that elevates GCs because this is a well-established approach which results in higher levels of endogenous CORT for several amphibian species (e.g. Belden et al. 2005, 2010; Fraker et al. 2021). Tadpoles were individually housed in 1 L plastic containers (diameter ~ 10 cm) holding 500 mL of their respective treatment for 12 days. Treatments were renewed every second day by replacing the appropriate treatment solution, and tadpoles were fed boiled organic spinach ad libitum.

Behavioural trials

At the end of the 12-day period, behavioural trials were conducted by exposing each tadpole to 15 Echinostoma sp. cercariae which were collected as described above for snail screening and counted using a dissecting microscope. To assess tadpole anti-parasite behaviour, we followed a methodology employed by similar studies (e.g. Koprivnikar et al. 2012b, 2014). Briefly, tadpoles were first acclimatised in individual experimental arenas (500 mL containers of DW) for 15 min during which their behaviour in the absence of cercariae was recorded. Recordings took place using three digital camera and tripod stations that were separated from each other and the experimenters by cardboard blinds. Individual tadpoles were assigned to each camera using a random number generator. After the initial 15 min of recording, cercariae were then added by emptying the contents of each microcentrifuge tube into the container, followed by recording for another 15 min. Following the behavioural recordings, tadpoles were left in their individual experimental containers and euthanised 24 h later using a buffered solution of 1% tricaine methanesulfonate (MS-222). When viewing the recordings, we considered tadpole activity at 45 time points that were separated by 20-s intervals (i.e. 15 min total). Tadpole behaviour was recorded as active/inactive at each time point.

Immune profiles

We chose to focus on the relative abundance of white blood cells (WBCs, i.e. leukocytes) as our measure of tadpole physiological (immunological) resistance because these play a key role in vertebrate defences against macroparasites and are specifically involved in tadpole responses to infection by echinostomatids (e.g. Martin and Conn 1990; Holland et al. 2007). Eosinophils play a particularly important role in this context (Kiesecker 2002; Belden and Kiesecker 2005; LaFonte and Johnson 2013). Importantly, WBC profiles in amphibians are not only affected by various stressors, including parasitic infection and contaminants (Davis et al. 2004, 2010; Marcogliese et al. 2009), but also by elevated GCs (e.g. Burraco et al. 2017), including that resulting from exogenous-CORT exposure (Belden and Kiesecker 2005; LaFonte and Johnson 2013).

Following euthanasia, tadpoles were weighed, and blood smears were prepared for each tadpole before freezing them in individually labelled plastic bags for follow-up parasite examinations. To collect blood smears from individual tadpoles, a slit was made on the tail, with blood collected using a 1 mL plastic pipette. A drop of blood was placed on a glass slide, and a smear was prepared using a second glass slide (Hadji-Azimi et al. 1987). The blood smear slide was left to air dry for approximately 30 min before fixation by dipping it for 1 min into a methanol solution. Slides were then immersed in Giemsa stain for 30 s, rinsed twice by dipping for 10 s into each of two consecutive jars containing distilled water, and left to air dry. Blood cells were identified and counted under a dissecting microscope following standard methods (Davis 2009). Due to their small size, it was not possible to obtain a standardised amount of blood from each tadpole. We therefore quantified the ratio of WBCs to red blood cells for each individual tadpole rather than total number, as measured on each slide up to the point when either 100 total WBCs were counted, or 150 fields of view had been examined. Given their importance for tadpole immune responses to trematodes, we also calculated the proportion of eosinophils making up the WBCs present.

Parasite enumeration

After the frozen tadpoles were thawed, their Gosner developmental stage was determined using a stereomicroscope. Tadpoles were then dissected, with their nephric system examined to assess the number of Echinostoma sp. cysts present. Given the need for necropsies to assess parasite infection, this excluded the possibility of assessing whole-body CORT levels in the tissues of individual tadpoles, with previous studies involving exogenous CORT and trematode exposure facing the same constraint (Belden and Kiesecker 2005; LaFonte and Johnson 2013). Whilst assays using water-borne CORT as a proxy for endogenous CORT levels in larval amphibians are a valuable tool for non-invasive measurement (Narayan et al. 2019), the relationship between the two is not consistent among species and development stages (McClelland and Woodley 2021). Notably, water-borne CORT may not be a reliable way to assess endogenous CORT for premetamorphic (Gosner stage 25–35) northern leopard frog tadpoles (McClelland and Woodley 2021).

Data analysis

All data analyses were carried out using R version 3.3.1 (R Core Team 2021) in RStudio (RStudio Team 2021) with the package lme4 (Bates et al. 2015) for GLMMs. Due to mortalities during the treatment and experimental periods, the final numbers of tadpoles in each of the 3 treatments were 14, 17, and 12 in the DW, ethanol solvent, and CORT treatment groups, respectively. All tadpoles used in this study were Gosner developmental stages 27–30 at the time the experiment ended, with stage positively correlated with tadpole mass. A generalised linear model using the Poisson family was used to test for any effect of treatment on Gosner stage and no differences were found between any of the three treatments (CORT/DW: z = −0.239, p = 0.811; ethanol/DW: z = −0.046, p = 0.963; CORT/ethanol z = −0.206, p = 0.837) (Electronic Supplementary Material: Table ESM1). However, given that previous studies have reported changes in immunocompetence with Gosner stage (e.g. Davis 2009), all of our models included this as a random effect variable rather than mass. To analyse the effect of treatment on parasite load, we first used a generalised linear mixed-effects model (GLMM) that included parasite load as the response variable and treatment as the predictor variable using the Poisson family.

A separate GLMM using the binomial family was used to investigate the effect of parasite exposure on tadpole activity across the three treatments. The number of times spent active out of a total of 45 timepoints both in the presence and absence of trematode cercariae was the binomial response variable with treatment and parasite exposure as predictor variables. To further examine the relationship between activity and parasite load, a GLMM using the Poisson family was carried out which contained the number of trematode cysts as the response variable, with two predictor variables—the centred variable of activity in the presence of parasites (number of time points during which a tadpole was active), and the categorical variable of treatment. In both models, an interaction between the two response variables, parasite exposure and treatment, and tadpole activity and treatment, respectively, was included. After examining the corrected akaike information criterion (AICc) value for these models, using the package MuMin (Bartoń 2018), the simpler model was chosen in both cases based on a lower AICc (AICc 606.87, 233.99 respectively), compared to the model which included the interaction (AICc 609.66, 236.08 respectively).

Finally, two linear regression mixed models were used to examine tadpole immune measures. First, tadpole immune measures were analysed in response to parasite load and the three treatment conditions. The response variable was the ratio of white to red blood cells (WBC: RBC) after a square-root transformation, with the predictor variables represented by the number of trematode cysts and treatment. The second analysis examined WBC: RBC (square-root transformed) in response to the centred predictor variable of tadpole activity in the presence of parasites. We did not analyse the eosinophil data specifically as the number of eosinophils as a proportion of the total WBCs was the exact same for all three treatments at 0.01.

Results

Mean parasite loads (number of Echinostoma sp. cysts) were 6.17, 6.06, and 3.93 for the CORT, ethanol, and DW-treated tadpoles, respectively; of the tadpoles surviving until the end of the experiment, 95% were successfully infected with cercariae. Parasite loads from tadpoles treated only with DW were significantly lower than those treated with either CORT (z = 2.163, p = 0.031) or the vehicle control ethanol (z = 2.644, p = 0.008) and there was no significant difference in parasite load between CORT and ethanol treated tadpoles (z = −0.305, p = 0.760) (Fig. 1; Electronic Supplementary Material (ESM), Table ESM2).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Mean number (±95% confidence intervals) of Echinostoma sp. cysts in tadpoles subjected to one of three treatments for 12 days before parasite exposure: exogenous corticosterone (pink, N = 12), ethanol (black, N = 17), and dechlorinated water (blue, N = 14)

When considering tadpole activity as a measure of their behavioural resistance, tadpoles showed increased activity in the presence of trematode cercariae (z = 5.551, p =  < 0.001; Fig. 2; Table ESM3), and there was an overall negative relationship between individual tadpole activity during exposure to the live trematode cercariae and the resultant parasite loads, i.e. less active tadpoles had higher loads of Echinostoma sp. cysts (z = −2.289, p = 0.022; Table ESM4). When comparing overall activity between treatments, that of tadpoles exposed to CORT was significantly lower than for tadpoles exposed to DW (z = −2.888, p = 0.004) or to ethanol (z = −2.054, p = 0.040) (Fig. 2). There was no significant difference in activity between tadpoles exposed to ethanol or DW (z = −0.853, p = 0.394) (Table ESM3). On average, CORT-treated tadpoles were active during 6.42 of the 45 time points at which activity (yes/no) was assessed, and ethanol- and DW-treated tadpoles were active for an average of 11.71 and 14.07 time points out of 45, respectively.

With respect to physiological immune measures, the WBC: RBC of tadpoles showed no significant relationship with their parasite load (t = −1.614, p = 0.107), and was not significantly affected by prior CORT exposure (t = 1.152, p = 0.249) (Table ESM5). In addition, tadpole WBC: RBC showed no significant relationship with their activity level in the presence of parasites (t = −0.026, p = 0.979) (Table ESM6). Gosner stage accounted for close to zero variance in models examining tadpole WBC: RBC (Table ESM26).

Discussion

Here we used exogenous glucocorticoid (GC) exposure of larval amphibians to simulate an aversive environmental stimulus that is capable of elevating endogenous GCs as part of a general physiological response to stressors which is seen in many vertebrates, finding that this negatively impacted the anti-parasite behaviours exhibited by tadpoles. Tadpoles exposed to exogenous CORT were the least active overall, including in the presence of free-swimming parasite infectious stages (cercariae of the trematode Echinostoma sp.), and concomitantly had high parasite loads. However, CORT exposure had no effect on immunity as measured by WBC profiles, and there was no relationship between the latter and host anti-parasite behaviour. Contrary to our expectations, this suggests that exposure to a stressor which elevates GCs over a similar period can increase host susceptibility to parasite or pathogen infection by compromising the BIS alone. Our results highlight the importance of adaptive host behavioural responses to the risk of parasite infection, and thus the broad potential of environmental stressors to influence infectious disease dynamics if they negatively affect anti-parasite behaviours by elevating endogenous GCs.

Anti-parasite behaviours have been reported for many host taxa and are a key component in avoiding infection by various parasites and pathogens (Behringer et al. 2018; Bush and Clayton 2018; Hart and Hart 2018). Our findings confirm the negative relationship between tadpole activity in the presence of cercariae and infection loads as reported by previous studies (e.g. Daly and Johnson 2011; Koprivnikar et al. 2014), with tadpoles in the water control having the fewest cysts. However, the number of cysts in CORT-exposed individuals was unexpectedly similar to the solvent control, and we posit that altered anti-parasite behaviours may still have played a role even though the overall activity of tadpoles in ethanol was similar to those only exposed to water. Notably, while tadpoles in the CORT treatment were consistently less active than those in the ethanol or water treatments, extreme activity levels were most commonly seen in ethanol-exposed tadpoles, especially when cercariae were present (see Fig. 2). This matches what has been reported for activity by ethanol-exposed R. pipiens tadpoles in response to a novel stimulus (Durbin et al. 2021), as well as overall activity in larval zebrafish after ethanol exposure (Chen et al. 2011). Critically, ethanol-exposed fish were not only prone to hyperactivity, but they also exhibited simplified swimming paths, i.e. fewer sharp twists and turns. Whilst we were not able to evaluate this, we believe it also occurred in our experiment, with important implications because the sharp flicks and twists of tadpoles are key to preventing cercariae establishment (Taylor et al. 2004; Koprivnikar et al. 2006; Szuroczki and Richardson 2012; Sears et al. 2013). Future studies should thus consider the effects of stressors and elevated GCs on specific components of anti-parasite behaviours, not just overall activity.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Median tadpole activity (with upper and lower quartiles shown) as measured by the number of times an individual was active (yes/no) during 45 timepoints in 15 min either in the absence (grey) or presence (pink) of trematode cercariae after being subjected to one of three treatments for 12 days before parasite exposure

It is important to identify environmental circumstances in which behavioural defences against infection may be reduced. For instance, both synchronous and asynchronous exposure to predation cues can interfere with tadpole anti-parasite behaviours (Koprivnikar and Penalva 2015; Koprivnikar and Urichuk 2017). In addition, elevated trematode infection loads as a result of tadpole exposure to aquatic pollutants, such as road salts, have been attributed to reduced behavioural rather than physiological immunity (Milotic et al. 2017). Whilst GC levels were not considered in these studies that demonstrated how various aversive stimuli can negatively affect the BIS, our results suggest that elevation of endogenous GCs in response to environmental stressors could be a common underlying factor which warrants further study given the capacity for these hormones to influence host behaviour.

Whilst our study is the first to consider how a general stressor could affect anti-parasite behaviours in aquatic vertebrates through GCs, others have examined anti-predator responses. The majority of these investigations reported that exogenous-CORT exposure reduced tadpole anti-predator behaviours by increasing their activity levels (e.g. Fraker et al. 2009; Middlemis Maher et al. 2013). This is the opposite response of most larval amphibians to predation risk (e.g. Hossie et al. 2017), thus elevated CORT could increase activity in a way that makes tadpoles more likely to be seen by visual predators (Gabor et al. 2019). In contrast, Kulkarni and Gramapurohit (2017) found that larval frog exposure to 5, 10 or 20 μg/L CORT enhanced their anti-predator response in a concentration-dependent manner by reducing their activity. In addition, Sullivan et al. (2021) reported that chronic, but not short-term, CORT elevations reduced exploratory behaviours in adult salamanders. However, the nature of the predator stimulus (e.g. conspecific alarm cues or larval dragonfly kairomones) used varies amongst studies and may contribute to the inconsistent effects of CORT on tadpole behavioural responses. It is thus unclear whether elevated GCs in amphibians generally decrease or increase their activity levels, as well as their responses to natural enemies. In mammals, chronically-elevated GCs may induce depression-like behaviours such as lethargy and helplessness (reviews by Erickson et al. 2003; Chen et al. 2016).

It is important to note that GCs have a wide range of physiological, behavioural and cognitive functions; elevated GCs in several contexts are thus not necessarily “stressful” in a negative sense and are important for maintaining homeostasis (Sapolsky et al. 2000; Erickson et al. 2003; MacDougall-Shackleton et al. 2019; Romero and Beattie 2022). Short-term increases in GC levels can aid in early responses to aversive stimuli (Erickson et al. 2003; Wingfield and Sapolsky 2003; Romero and Beattie 2022); however, chronic elevations are known to suppress various behaviours (e.g. reproduction as in Woodley and Lacy 2010). In the face of chronically-elevated GCs, diminished-behavioural output as a strategy to redirect critical resources may be advantageous in many situations (Myers et al. 2014), especially as sustained-GC elevations can result in homeostatic overload (Romero et al. 2009). Such a mechanism may help to explain why CORT-exposed tadpoles here showed the least activity overall.

Whilst infection loads were higher in our CORT- and ethanol-exposed tadpoles compared to the water control, there was no difference in their physiological immune response as measured by WBC: RBC, nor was there a difference in their eosinophil ratios. This is despite the fact that previous studies clearly demonstrated immunosuppressive effects of exogenous-CORT exposure on larval amphibians, including decreased eosinophils, that were associated with greater trematode infection (Belden and Kiesecker 2005; LaFonte and Johnson 2013). It thus appears that chronic exposure to exogenous CORT affected host susceptibility to parasite infection in our study through changes in the behavioural, and not physiological, immune system. Although exogenous CORT and trematode exposure should have opposing effects by being behaviour and immune-suppressing vs. behaviour and immune-stimulating, respectively, tadpoles in the water treatment would have had the capacity to mount a response via WBCs. As such, exogenous-CORT exposure either did not affect the post-infection immune response or plasma-WBC profiles are not appreciably altered as a result of echinostome infection, as seen for another trematode, Ribeiroia ondatrae (Koprivnikar et al. 2019). It is also possible that the 24 h period between parasite exposure and tadpole euthanasia here was insufficient for WBC changes to have occurred. In addition, plasma-WBC profiles provide information on only one aspect of the potential immune response to trematode cysts. For instance, Holland et al. (2007) found that eosinophils and macrophages developed directly around echinostome cysts in infected tissue. Future investigations should thus consider additional measures of immunity when assessing responses to elevated GCs. It would also be helpful to evaluate physiological immunity in a subset of tadpoles before parasite exposure, as well as measure whole-body CORT to quantify individual levels although we had no reason to presume that exogenous-CORT exposure failed to raise endogenous levels here given that this is a well-documented approach to achieve this outcome in larval amphibians (review by Belden et al. 2005). For similar studies, further refinement and validation of water-borne CORT assays may also be useful. Finally, measuring CORT in tadpoles while in the presence of parasite infectious stages such as cercariae would also be informative to understand CORT regulation in response to an acute stressor.

We had predicted that individual tadpoles exhibiting the greatest anti-parasite activity in the presence of trematode infectious stages (cercariae) would show the least robust-induced immune response post-infection, i.e. display a trade-off between behavioural and physiological immunity. However, this was not seen despite various reports of a negative relationship between different types of defences against natural enemies. For example, crucian carp with a morphological phenotype providing defence against predators have reduced immune function (Vinterstare et al. 2019). More specifically, trade-offs have also been reported with respect to host defences against parasites (review by Lopes 2017). For instance, Klemme et al. (2020) demonstrated significant genetic variation in parasite defence traits across populations of salmonid fish, as well as negative associations between traits, with the most resistant populations showing the weakest parasite avoidance behaviours, and also the lowest infection tolerance. At the individual level, male house finches that avoided sick conspecifics invested less in their immune response to a pathogen—in contrast, males which spent time associating with, or near, sick conspecifics had stronger immune responses (Zylberberg et al. 2013). This being said, trade-offs may be dependent on the immune measures employed, especially if these have different costs (Zylberberg et al. 2014), with antibody responses most commonly considered (e.g. Zylberberg et al. 2013; Schreier and Grindstaff 2020). It is possible that investment into WBC-mediated immune responses, such as those considered here, are not affected by that in the BIS. This trade-off might also only occur in specific circumstances or may not be induced by changes in GCs. Future investigations should consider the specific circumstances under which trade-offs between behavioural and physiological immunity may be observed (e.g. dual stressors such as predators and parasites), and also include a suite of immune measures to examine both standing and induced immunity.

It is critical to understand the extent to which potential hosts can engage in anti-parasite behaviours given their central roles in defence (Hart 1990, 1997), especially because the threat posed by parasites and pathogens is predicted to increase in response to various environmental stressors (Oppliger et al. 1998; Byers 2020). Our study not only demonstrates the key role of behavioural immunity but also highlights the potential for interruption of this front line of defence by any environmental stressor that could chronically elevate endogenous GCs, including ambient/environmental CORT (e.g. Gabor et al. 2018; Tornabene et al. 2021), as we simulated here with exogenous GC exposure. The field of eco-physiology, which considers how anthropogenic disturbances may affect wildlife through physiology, including their susceptibility to infectious diseases (Busch and Hayward 2009; Hing et al. 2016), should therefore consider behavioural effects as well. Behavioural responses to multiple stressors are also important to understand (Hale et al. 2016). We recommend that future studies continue to explore various aspects of parasite avoidance, resistance, and tolerance, and how these are affected by physiological responses to aversive stimuli. In this way, we stand to gain a greater understanding of how both physiological and behavioural defences will play roles in mitigating the risks and costs of parasitism in an ever-changing environment.