Introduction

Hereditary retinal dystrophies, particularly retinitis pigmentosa, have no established therapy and are among the leading causes of blindness worldwide, especially in developed countries [1, 2]. In recent years, artificial vision technology has begun to attract attention as a mode of therapy. Artificial vision technology recovers vision by electrically stimulating neurons at specific locations along the visual pathway. Among these technologies, the most widely developed approaches involve retinal prostheses, which insert stimulating electrodes at the retina. There are three types of retinal prostheses, classified according to the position in which the electrodes are inserted: epiretinal prostheses [3, 4], subretinal prostheses [5,6,7], and suprachoroidal prostheses [8,9,10]. Our aim was to develop a suprachoroidal type of retinal prosthesis (STS, suprachoroidal–transretinal stimulation) [10].

The simplest method to further improve vision through retinal prostheses in the future is to increase the resolution by reducing the size and increasing the number of stimulating electrodes. Previously, Zrenner et al. stimulated the retina with 1500 stimulating electrodes, obtaining good results; however, the number of electrodes and visual acuity are not necessarily directly proportional [11]. Our group conducted a clinical trial of an STS retinal prosthesis, which contained 49 stimulating electrodes. In the present clinical trial, we observed that participants were unable to discriminate between two phosphenes induced simultaneously by adjacent stimulating electrodes because the phosphenes overlapped. This finding indicates that in STS retinal prostheses, to discriminate between two phosphenes, it is necessary to increase the distance between the stimulating electrodes with the current value at which phosphenes are obtained. Therefore, we aim to improve next-generation STS by enlarging the area of the electrode array to expand the visual field rather than by increasing the number of stimulating electrodes and increasing resolution.

One of the merits of expanding the visual field is that the visual information obtained at any one time increases. Furthermore, the position of the visual field induced by the retinal prosthesis can be selected depending on the insertion position of the electrode array. It is still to be determined which visual field position is most advantageous. Generally, it has been concluded that an inferior visual field has a more profound influence on quality of life (QOL) than a superior visual field [12,13,14]. However, it is not yet known whether these findings hold true for an artificial visual field induced by retinal prosthesis; therefore, further research is required.

In the present study, we investigated the effect of an expanded visual field and the effect of visual field position, on participant mobility—an important visual behavior—using a retinal prosthesis simulator.

Subjects and Methods

Subjects

Twelve healthy volunteers (4 males, 8 females) with an average age of 26.8 ± 5.5 years (± SD) were included in the study. All participants had best-corrected visual acuity (BCVA) ≥ 20/20. The procedures used in this study adhered to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and were approved by the Ethics Committee of Osaka University Hospital. A full explanation of the purpose of the study and the procedures to be used was presented to the participants, who all signed an informed consent form. It was made clear to all participants that they were free to withdraw from the study at any time.

Methods

Each participant, wearing full correction lenses and a retinal prosthesis simulator, underwent a mobility test. For the mobility test, white circular cloths with a diameter of 20 cm were placed on a black carpet measuring 6 m × 2.2 m. Obstacles were placed on the carpet at an interval of 40 cm (Fig. 1). Participants were instructed to pass the length of the carpet without stepping on obstacles. In addition, participants were equipped with a head-mounted iPod touch® (Apple, Cupertino, USA) with gyroscope function; visual scanning through head movement (hereafter referred to as head-scanning) during the mobility test was measured simultaneously. During the mobility test, the retinal prosthesis simulator presented images received through three different processing methods (described in the following sections). The time taken to pass through the obstacle course, the number of steps taken on obstacles, and the extent of head-scanning for each processing method were compared. The time taken to pass through the obstacle course was measured by a stopwatch and the number of steps taken on an obstacle was measured visually.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Mobility test. a. Pattern diagram of the mobility test course. On a black carpet with dimensions of 6 m and 2.2 m, white cloth circles with a radius of 10 cm are placed as obstacles at 40 cm intervals. b Actual picture of the mobility test course

Retinal prosthesis simulator

The retinal prosthesis simulator receives images from a web camera (BSW20KM11BK, Buffalo, Nagoya, Japan) placed on a head-mounted display (HMD) (HMZ-T1®, SONY, Tokyo, Japan). The input images are processed with a custom-built program (Nidek, Gamagohri, Japan) on a laptop computer (PC-GL19614GU, NEC, Tokyo, Japan) then displayed on the HMD. The retinal prosthesis simulator displayed a retinal prosthesis visual field of 15° (visual angle) per side, imitating an electrode array consisting of 5 × 5 stimulating electrodes in three ways: ① dual-electrode array inferior visual field (visual field (VF)1), ② single-electrode array superior visual field (VF2), and ③ dual-electrode array superior visual field (VF3), in addition to a central circular natural visual field with a diameter of 5° (visual angle) (Fig. 2a). The central natural visual field imitates the vision of patients with advanced retinitis pigmentosa (Fig. 2b). The visual acuity of the central natural visual field was measured as having a value of approximately 1.0 logMAR (logarithm of the minimum angle of resolution) units. In the retinal prosthesis simulator, if the average luminance range of one electrode exceeds a certain value, it is displayed as one light spot (Fig. 2b). The laptop for processing images was contained in a rucksack, which was carried by the participant (Fig. 3).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Pattern diagrams of three types of visual fields displayed by the retinal prosthesis simulator. a The retinal prosthesis simulator displayed a central circular natural visual field with a diameter of 5° (visual angle) and the retinal prosthesis visual field 15° (visual angle) on one side in three different ways: VF1, dual-electrode array inferior visual field; VF2, single-electrode array superior visual field; VF3, dual-electrode array superior visual field. b A schematic view of what the participants see (VF3). When the participant sees an obstacle with the retinal prosthesis simulator, the artificial visual fields are displayed as light spots for the corresponding part, and the central natural visual field shows the actual image in low resolution

Fig. 3
figure 3

Actual picture of the retinal prosthesis simulator. The participant is wearing a head-mounted display with a web camera. A laptop computer for processing the images taken by the web camera is kept in the rucksack on the participant’s back. An iPod touch® is attached to the back of the participant’s head

Head-scanning

Subjects wore a 6th generation iPod touch® on their head, and the extent of head-scanning during the mobility test was measured through a built-in gyro sensor. Using the custom-built program, the horizontal, vertical, and rotational angular velocities of the head were measured every 0.02 s, and the absolute values of these values were integrated to obtain the total extent of head-scanning during the test. Among the three directions of head movement, horizontal and vertical directions were analyzed.

Statistical analyses

One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and Fisher’s least significant difference (Fisher’s LSD) test were used for comparisons between the three groups. A p value of 0.05 or less was considered significant. SigmaPlot version 13.0® (SystatSoftware Inc., San Jose, USA) analytical software was used for analysis.

Results (Table 1)

Number of steps taken on obstacles

The average number of steps taken on obstacles were 6.5 ± 4.1 (VF1), 7.2 ± 5.0 (VF2), and 3.8 ± 2.6 (VF3), respectively. One-way ANOVA revealed these results to be statistically significantly different (P = 0.028). Furthermore, statistically significant differences were observed between VF1 and VF3 (P = 0.039, Fisher’s LSD) and between VF2 and VF3 (P = 0.012, Fisher’s LSD).

Table 1 Information of 12 subjects and the results of the mobility test

Time taken to pass through the obstacle course

The average times taken to pass through the obstacle course were 47.6 ± 22.4 s (VF1), 50.7 ± 25.2 s (VF2), and 44.0 ± 26.1 s (VF3), respectively. This was included as reference data because there was no significant difference in one-way ANOVA (P = 0.12); however, time taken to pass through the obstacle course tended to be shorter for VF3 than for VF2 (P = 0.040, paired t test).

The extent of head-scanning

The average extent of head-scanning in the vertical direction was 274.9 ± 167.6°(VF1), 287.3 ± 167.6°(VF2), and 244.7 ± 174.5°(VF3), respectively. There was no significant difference in one-way ANOVA (P = 0.55). The average extent of head-scanning in the horizontal direction was 533.2 ± 253.6°(VF1), 568.5 ± 371.0°(VF2), and 423.2 ± 289.4°(VF3), respectively. This was included as reference data as there was no significant difference in one-way ANOVA (P = 0.072); however, the extent of head-scanning in the horizontal direction tended to be smaller for VF3 than for VF2 (P = 0.029, paired t test).

Discussion

In the present study, we simulated the use of retinal prosthesis for patients with advanced retinitis pigmentosa using healthy participants and evaluated walking ability. The rationale for selecting walking ability as an outcome is that this parameter is greatly influenced by the visual field, which is important for locating a wide range of targets and obstacles. Furthermore, in patients with visual field disturbances, it has been reported that mobility performance can be improved by training visual scanning using saccades, unlike other activities, such as reading [15, 16]. As described later, we considered that saccades are equivalent to head-scanning in retinal prosthesis patients, and that evaluating head-scanning would be an indicator of the quality of mobility of retinal prosthesis patients.

As indicators of walking ability, we evaluated three parameters: the number of steps taken on obstacles on the obstacle course, the time taken to pass through the obstacle course, and the extent of head-scanning. In general, patients with a retinal prosthesis featuring external cameras rely on head movements to find targets because the camera does not follow eye movements [17]. Currently, retinal prostheses have started to be developed that incorporate gaze tracking systems [18, 19]. Despite these developments, we hypothesize that visual scanning based on head movements will continue to be a necessary skill for the time being due to current limitations with the gaze tracking system, such as calibration accuracy and delay. Patients with a retinal prosthesis are often elderly people; therefore, lots of head movement may prove a heavy burden for patients. For this reason, we selected head-scanning as one of the parameters for evaluation because reducing head-scanning by enlarging the visual field may be helpful for patients.

In the present study, we evaluated mobility with three visual field patterns and found that the number of steps on obstacles was significantly smaller in VF3 than in VF1 or VF2. Although only included as reference data, in VF3, the time taken to pass through the obstacle course tended to be shorter, and the extent of head-scanning tended to be smaller than in VF2. Generally, an inferior visual field is thought to have a larger influence on QOL than a superior visual field [12,13,14]. However, in this experiment, better performance was observed with a superior visual field than with an inferior visual field. The reason for this may be that in ultra-low vision conditions, participants cannot avoid seeing their feet when walking. Seeing their feet, participants can then obtain visual information in the direction of travel from the superior visual field, improving performance with the superior visual field. It has previously been reported that superior visual field disorders have a larger impact on the ability to observe obstacles when driving as compared to inferior visual field disorders [20]. The most important visual field position in everyday life may change depending on tasks; therefore, it is necessary to consider this on an activity-by-activity basis.

Considering that the extent of head-scanning tended to be smaller, and the time taken to pass through the obstacle course tended to be shorter with a wide visual field, dual-electrode array, we conclude that participants can obtain a wide range of information about obstacles simultaneously when using this mode, resulting in a reduction of the extent of head movement and time taken to pass through the obstacle course. It has been reported previously that glaucoma patients with narrowed visual fields complete more saccades than healthy controls when performing tasks to which they are unaccustomed and that these patients require more time to perform the task [21]. If we consider saccades and head-scanning of retinal prosthesis patients to be similar, we would conclude that the width of the visual field affected the results in our study, in keeping with previous findings. As results regarding head-scanning and time required to complete obstacle course are only tendencies and did not reach statistical significance, it is necessary to increase the number of participants and test these again in the future.

Finally, the conditions of the mobility test in our study consisted of high-contrast obstacles allocated at equal intervals; in the future, it is necessary to evaluate walking ability under more natural circumstances.

In the present study, we examined the size and position of the visual field in the simulated retinal prosthesis and found that walking performance improved depending on the conditions, particularly when using the VF3 setup. We should also consider other activities to fully evaluate the effects of size and position of visual field on walking ability, particularly in realistic conditions.