Introduction

Spinocerebellar ataxia type 2 (SCA2) is one of the most common forms of SCAs worldwide and the most frequent subtype in southern Italy [1]. This dominantly inherited degenerative disorder is due to an expanded CAG trinucleotide repeat in the ATXN2 gene coding for the protein ataxin [2]. Ataxic gait, impaired balance and speech, dysmetria, and slow eye movements [3,4,5,6] are typical motor manifestations of SCA2; the clinical spectrum of SCA2 may also include an altered cognitive profile characterized by disturbances in fronto-executive functions, memory, and visuoconstructive skills [7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14]. Although the patho-anatomical basis of neuropsychological impairment in SCA2 remains to be fully elucidated, recent anatomical and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies have revealed abnormalities in the cerebellar network as well as in non-cerebellar regions, such as orbitofrontal cortex, the middle frontal region, the primary sensorimotor cortex, and temporomesial and insular cortices that are known to be functionally connected to the cerebellum [15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22]. Cognitive profile of SCA2 patients meets the features of the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome (CCAS) or Schmahmann’s syndrome that characterize the disturbances of higher function in patients with cerebellar injury across different disease types and ages [23].

Over the last two decades, an accumulating body of evidence showed that motor processes should no longer be considered as mere automated activities but as complex tasks also requiring cognitive abilities [24]. It is therefore likely that certain cognitive functions contribute to motor control [24, 25]. Nevertheless, studies addressing the relationships between severity of motor disturbances and cognition in SCA2 provided inconsistent results [11, 13, 14, 26,27,28,29,30].

The present study was designed to explore the relationships between the severity of motor ataxia (as assessed by a validated and widely used clinical severity scale and by an objective automated computerized system of gait analysis) and the burden of cognitive impairment in global intellectual abilities, general executive functions, and short- and long-term verbal and visuospatial memory in SCA2 patients.

Materials and Methods

Study Subjects

Case patients were recruited among outpatients with genetically confirmed SCA2 who attended the outpatient movement disorders clinic of the Neurology Unit “Amaducci,” “Aldo Moro” University of Bari, Italy, from September 2016 to July 2017. Patients who were able to walk without support were included in the study whereas patients who could not walk without support because of ataxia or patients who complained of neuro-otological, orthopaedic, or visual disorders, or were under drugs possibly affecting ataxic manifestations were excluded. Sex- and age- (± 5 years) matched healthy control subjects were recruited among relatives of non-ataxic outpatients. All participants provided a written informed consent and study procedures were approved by the local ethics committee (“Aldo Moro” University of Bari, Italy).

Study Procedures

Case and control individuals were asked to participate in a study on cognition and movement approved by the ethics committee. A trained interviewer, not blinded to the case/control status but unaware of the study hypotheses, collected relevant demographic and clinical information. Severity of ataxia was rated by the validated clinical “scale for the assessment and rating of ataxia” (SARA) that measures eight domains (stance, gait, sitting, speech, finger chase, nose–finger, fast alternating hand movements, heel-shin slide) and provides a total score (SARA-TOT) ranging from 0 to 40, with 0 indicating absence of ataxia and 40 the most severe degree of ataxia [31]. We also tested the SARA-gait subscore for possible correlations.

A neuropsychological test battery was administered by an experienced neuropsychologist (who was blinded to the results of motor assessments) to both case and control subjects one to four days after motor assessment. The test battery included the Mini-mental State Examination (MMSE), the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), and the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS). MMSE provided a global assessment of cognitive functions [32]. The WCST [33] is a well-established measure of general executive function evaluating several “frontal” lobe properties, including strategic planning, organized searching, utilizing environmental feedback to shift cognitive sets, directing behaviour toward achieving a goal, and modulating impulsive responding. The test uses stimulus and response cards (64-card scoring version) characterized by various forms, colors, and numbers [33]. The participant is told to match the cards, but not how to match them; however, the subject is told whether a particular match is right or wrong. As the test progresses, there are unannounced shifts in the sorting principle which require the subject to alter her/his approach. Performance is recorded as number of correct categories (W-CA), total number of corrected responses (W-CORRECT), and number of perseverative errors (W-PERRORS). The WMS measures different memory functions by several subtests, including information on general knowledge, orientation in time and place, mental control (testing the ability to repeat sequences such as the alphabet), logical memory (immediate repetition of short stories presented orally), digits forwards and digits reversed (the conventional digit span tests), visual reproduction (drawing reproduction of three simple designs, each presented individually for 10 s), and associate learning (the patient is given three trials to learn 10 pairs of words, with six pairs having a logical match and four pairs having no logical connection). The Beck depression Inventory (BDI) was used to assess depressive symptoms [34].

Spatio-temporal parameters of gait were assessed in SCA2 patients by a motion capture system (SMART-E, BTS Bioengineering, Milan, Italy) equipped with a set of 8 infrared cameras [35]. After acquisition of data on height, weight, anterior superior iliac spine (ASIS) distance, pelvis thickness, knee and ankle width, and leg length (distance between ASIS and medial malleolus), 22 retro-reflective spherical markers for the lower-body segments were placed on anatomical landmarks in accordance with Davis model [36]. Kinetic data were collected using 2 piezoelectric force platforms (Kistler Instruments, Winterthur, Switzerland; 400 × 64 cm) embedded in a walkway. Patients were asked to walk over the walkway at their comfortable, preferred speed; each trial started 2 m in front of the mat and continued for 2 m beyond it in order to consider only stationary conditions (i.e., constant speed) discarding acceleration and deceleration phases. After a familiarization phase, each participant performed at least six walking trials. The raw kinematic data were then processed to calculate the following spatio-temporal parameters: gait speed, step length, step width, cadence, stance, and double support phase duration (expressed as percentage of the gait cycle). As intra-subject variability of gait parameters has been reported as a relevant feature of ataxic gait, we also calculated for each parameter the coefficient of variation (CV) using the formula: CV = standard deviation × 100/mean.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed by Stata 11.0 package (Stata Corporation, College Station, TX, USA). Data were expressed as mean and standard deviation (SD) unless otherwise indicated. Differences between study groups were assessed by the chi-square test and the Student’s two-tailed t tests, as appropriate. p values from multiple case-control comparisons were adjusted by the Bonferroni correction. Correlations between study variables were checked by Spearman’s rank correlation analysis. Sex was categorized as 0 for men and 1 for women. Statistical significance was set at the 0.05 level.

Results

Demographics and Clinical Features of SCA2 Patients and Controls

Twenty-two right-handed SCA2 patients were seen during the study period. Two subjects were however excluded from the study, one because of severe visual impairment due to diabetes mellitus, the other one because of severe spinal disc herniation-related pain. The remaining 20 patients who met eligibility criteria were 11 women and 9 men from 11 families. The SCA2 group and the healthy control group were matched for sex and did not significantly differ for age (53.4 ± 13.3 vs. 49.6 ± 2.7 years, p = 0.11) and years of schooling (9.5 ± 3.4 vs. 10.7 ± 4.4, p = 0.2).

SCA2 patients carried a heterozygous CAG triplet expansion with 37.6 ± 2.9 repeats (range, 35 to 47) in the coding ATXN2 region. The normal alleles contained 22 repeats in all case patients. Mean age at disease onset was 39.7 years (SD, 13.6; range, 10–62) and mean disease duration was 13.8 years (SD, 10.6; range, 1–37). There was a negative correlation between number of repeats in the expanded allele and age at disease onset (Spearman rho = − 0.56, p = 0.01). No patient was under riluzole therapy.

SARA-TOT score was 10.4 ± 4.5 (range, 2.5 to 17.5). There was no significant correlation between SARA-TOT score and age (Spearman rho = − 0.15, p = 0.5) or gender (Spearman rho = 0.08, p = 0.7), whereas non-significant trends were observed for a correlation between SARA-TOT score and age at disease onset (Spearman rho, − 0.43, p = 0.06), disease duration (Spearman rho = 0.31, p = 0.07), and number of triplets (Spearman rho, 0.43, p = 0.06). No significant correlation was found between SARA-TOT score and years of schooling (Spearman rho, − 0.12, p = 0.62).

Neuropsychological Testing

Neuropsychological assessment showed that SCA2 patients significantly differed from healthy controls for MMSE, W-PERRORS, and total WMS score (Table 1). BDI was comparable in case and control subjects (Table 1). All subitems from WMS except WMS-orientation and WMS-digits forwards were significantly lower in case patients (p values ranging between < 0.04 and < 0.0001 after Bonferroni correction orientation). No SCA2 patient could be diagnosed with dementia according to DSM-V criteria, even though 35% of them scored ≤ 23 on MMSE. Of note, WCST and WMS were impaired in both patients who scored < 23 on MMSE and patients yielding MMSE score in the non-demented range (data not shown).

Table 1 Results of neuropsychological testing in SCA2 patients and healthy controls

In the SCA2 group, there was no significant correlation between demographic variables (age, sex, years of schooling, age at disease onset, disease duration, and number of triplets) and most cognitive tests (data not shown): the only significant correlation was observed between number of triplets and W-CORRECT score (rho = − 0.55, p = 0.01); moreover, male gender was associated with higher BDI score (rho = − 0.46, p = 0.04).

Spearman analysis revealed significant correlations between the overall severity of ataxia as assessed by the SARA scale and performance on MMSE and WCST (Table 2). These findings were confirmed even when analysis was restricted to the gait parameter from SARA (SARA-gait vs. MMSE: rho = − 0.61, p = 0.004; SARA-gait vs. W-CORRECT score: rho = − 0.68, p = 0.004). By contrast, no significant correlation emerged between SARA-TOT score and performance on BDI, WMS total score (Table 2) or any subtest of the WMS (data not shown).

Table 2 Spearman correlation analysis between SARA severity score and cognitive tests in 20 SCA2 patients. Significant correlations are in bold

Gait Analysis

The results of gait analysis in the 20 SCA2 patients are in Table 3. Higher SARA-TOT and SARA-gait scores significantly correlated to worse performances on gait analysis (Table 4). Analyzing the relationships between demographic variables (age, number of repeats, age of disease onset, disease duration, and schooling) and gait analysis parameters on Spearman correlation, the only significant correlations were found between disease duration and stance phase duration (rho = 0.61, p = 0.01), double support (rho = 0.51, p = 0.04), step length (rho = − 0.57, p = 0.02), and gait speed (rho = − 0.69, p = 0.003).

Table 3 Results of computer-assisted gait analysis performed in 20 SCA2 patients
Table 4 Spearman correlation between SARA-TOT and SARA-gait scores and the parameters of the gait analysis in the 20 SCA2 patients. Significant correlations are in italics

Significant correlations also emerged between cognitive performance on the WCST and stance phase, double support subphase, step width, gait speed, or step length (Table 5). No significant correlation was observed between gait parameters and MMSE, BDI, WMS (Table 5), or any subtest of the WMS (data not shown). Finally, there was no significant correlation between CV of the examined gait parameters and any neuropsychological test (data not shown).

Table 5 Spearman correlation analysis between computer-assisted gait analysis parameters and cognitive tests in the 20 SCA2 patients. Significant correlations are in italics

Discussion

Our psychometric evaluation showed that SCA2 patients performed worse than age- and sex-matched healthy controls of comparable level of schooling on a neuropsychological test battery assessing global intellectual abilities, general executive functions, and short- and long-term verbal and visuospatial memory. Overall, these findings confirmed previous observations in SCA2 patients [7, 10,11,12,13,14, 17, 19, 27, 29]. The 35% frequency of SCA2 patients scoring ≤ 23 on MMSE was comparable to the frequency of altered general cognitive status reported in SCA2 patients [7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14]. None of our patients could be diagnosed with dementia according with DSM-V criteria, which was consistent with the low rate of dementia characterizing most SCA2 pedigrees [7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14, 22, 27, 29].

Although SCA2 patients showed a poorer performance in almost all administered tests, only global intellectual abilities and executive functions significantly correlated with the overall severity of ataxia as assessed by the SARA scale; and impaired executive functions alone correlated with performance on several spatio-temporal gait analysis parameters. These findings would probably suggest a prominent influence of executive functions on motor abilities in SCA2 patients. Supporting the specificity of the findings, there was no correlation between cognitive performance and age, gender, disease duration, years of schooling, or repeat length.

Several studies dealt with the relationships between motor abilities and cognition in patients with heredodegenerative ataxia [11, 13, 14, 26,27,28,29,30] but results were inconsistent. In 18 SCA2 patients, La Pira et al. reported a significant correlation between ataxia severity and a few items of the California Verbal Learning Test, a neuropsychological tool that measures verbal memory and learning strategies [13]. By contrast, Burk et al. [11] did not find any significant relationships between motor performance (evaluated using a severity scale for limb ataxia and dysarthria only) and cognitive impairment in 17 SCA2 patients by MMSE, WCST, and WMS. Using different clinical and electrophysiological approaches, significant inverse relationships emerged between severity of motor disturbances and performance in cognitive domains in large series of SCA2 patients [27,28,29,30]. In particular, higher SARA score was associated with greater impairment in phonemic verbal fluency [27], error-based visuomotor learning [28], attention [29, 30], visual memory, spatial planning and motor control [29], and working memory [30]. Two further studies assessing genetically heterogeneous samples of SCA patients found significant correlations between severity of ataxia, measured by the SARA scale [26] and the ataxia clinical rating scale [14], and cognitive performances assessed by MMSE, WCST, and WMS [26] in one study, by MMSE, Raven PM, phonemic fluency, semantic fluency, and attentional matrices in the other study [14]. However, both studies did not provide separate data on SCA2 subgroup, probably because of the small sample size and the low statistical power. Finally, in the study for the development of the cerebellar cognitive affective/Schmahmann syndrome scale [23], Hoche et al. did not find any strong correlation between the Brief Ataxia Rating Scale and cognitive performance in various domains in both exploratory and validation cohorts, each consisting of various forms of acquired or inherited conditions. The heterogeneity of cerebellar diseases and the very small number of SCA2 patients included in the study (there were 5 SCA2 patients in the exploratory cohort, only one SCA2 patient in the validation cohort) may explain the discrepancy between our and Hoche study.

At variance with previous studies that used a variety of tools to assess motor activity and cognition [11, 13, 14, 26], in the present work, we used widely validated and suitable clinical tools to ascertain both motor and cognitive abilities in a homogeneous group of SCA2 patients [22, 31]. This makes the results of our study probably valid and accurate. The use of an objective system to measure ataxic gait also distinguished the present study from previous works [11, 13, 14, 23, 26,27,28,29,30] and corroborated the validity of our findings. Further supporting the validity of our methods, we observed an obvious correlation between SARA scores and gait analysis parameters.

The pathophysiological basis underlying the relationships between ataxia and executive dysfunction in SCA2 patients remains to be fully elucidated. As revealed by neuropathological and voxel-based morphometry studies, patients affected by SCA2 show a pattern of both olivo-ponto-cerebellar and supratentorial damage [15,16,17,18,19,20,21]; moreover, functional magnetic resonance imaging studies in SCA2 have shown a connection between posterior regions in the cerebellum and regions in the cerebral cortex related to cognition [16, 21]. Although it is now well accepted that cerebellum plays a role in both motor and cognitive tasks, including executive functions [15, 22, 37,38,39,40], it remains unclear whether the association between ataxia and executive dysfunction reflects a prominent cerebellar involvement or a concomitant neural degeneration in extra-cerebellar structures leading to a disconnection in the fronto-ponto-cerebello-thalamo-cortical circuits [2, 9, 10, 37, 41]. The cerebellar cognitive affective/Schmahmann syndrome has also been described in patients with isolated cerebellar pathology [23, 37]. However, it could not rule out the possibility that the pathological alterations in non-cerebellar structures may play a determinant role in the positive association between ataxia and cognitive disturbances emerged in patients with complex cerebrocerebellar pathologies [13, 22, 23, 27,28,29,30]. In keeping with this view, in patients with SCA2, cognitive disfunctions has been found to correlate with both cerebellar [19, 20] and cerebral [17] structural damage using the voxel-based morphometry and diffusion tensor imaging. Various degrees of cognitive impairment have been described in different dominantly inherited ataxias with isolated cerebellar or complex cerebrocerebellar pathology [22, 42,43,44,45,46], but only few studies evaluated the relationship between motor abilities and cognitive state, showing either the presence [42, 43] or the absence [44,45,46] of a correlation. The inconsistency in neuropsychological tests selection and other potential confounding factors including the use of tests unsuitable to explore specific cognitive domains in patients with ataxias due to their visual, speech, and motor disturbances may explain discrepancies across studies [22].

Regardless of the diffusion of neural damage to extra-cerebellar structures, the loss of the cerebellar contribution to the cerebrocerebellar circuitry has been argued to serve as the basis for the cognitive impairments in patients with cerebellar pathology, a concept embedded within the dysmetria of thought theory [37]. Studies based on pathological examination as well as longitudinal and functional neuroimaging studies could better investigate whether the correlation between motor functions and cognition emerged in different ataxic disorders relies on the cerebellar damage alone or on the diffusion of the neurodegeneration process to extra-cerebellar structures.

It has been postulated that executive functions may play an important role in the assessment, planning, response to a given situation, and choice of the gait pattern [47, 48] and there is emerging evidence indicating that cognitive interventions may improve motor activity in several conditions [49]. Different studies were designed to investigate the effect on motor abilities of either cognitive-enhancing medications or cognitive training interventions [49]. Though providing preliminary findings with different methodologic limitations, most of these studies showed that improvement in executive functions could result in good outcomes in gait, balance, risk of falls, and dual-task performances in series of patients with Parkinson’s disease [50,51,52], mild-to-moderate dementia [53, 54], and in healthy older adults [55,56,57,58]. Although studies on this issue are lacking in heredodegenerative ataxia, the relationship between ataxia and executive functions we found may support the idea that cognitive pharmaceutical and rehabilitative interventions can be of benefit in the management of motor problems in SCA2 patients.

Our study has strengths and limitations. First, this was not a population-based study, but recruiting criteria yielded a case series resembling the general population of cases in both demographic and clinical features. The significant correlations we found between the number of CAG repeats and age at disease onset and between SARA scores and disease duration were in agreement with data from previous SCA2 series, [11, 13, 30, 59] thus supporting the representativeness of our sample. Second, technical difficulties linked to motor impairment due to ataxia might have contributed to the poorer cognitive performance showed by SCA2 patients, at least in some cases. However, we took care to exclude tests that could have been influenced by manual and speech abilities. In particular, the WCST avoids the pitfalls of performance speed pressure and do not place a premium on motor or visual speed and accuracy. Influence of depression mood on the differences in cognition between groups was also unlikely because of the comparable BDI scores showed by patients and healthy subjects. Using a clinical tool to assess motor severity might have provided inconsistent results. However, the results obtained using a validated and widely used clinical scale (the SARA scale) were corroborated by the findings from an objective computer-assisted system measuring gait performance. Third, in our gait analysis, we did not use a control group, but we referred to the normative data provided by the motion capture system manufacturer (BTS Bioengineering, Milan, Italy); supporting the validity of our observation, our findings are comparable with pathological values of ataxic patients from a study that used the same gait analysis system and also provided a control group. [60]. Finally, our case and control groups are relatively small and we cannot exclude the possibility that non-significant results reflect, at least in part, lack of statistical power. Further studies investigating the relationship between motor control and cognition in larger series of cases and healthy subjects are therefore necessary.

Despite the foregoing limitations, our analysis confirmed the poor performance of SCA2 patients in several cognitive domains and provided new information suggesting a specific correlation between ataxia and executive dysfunction. Information from this study may help to clarify the relationships between motor and cognitive performance in SCA2 patients and also to identify new targets for rehabilitative and pharmacological intervention.