Introduction

Verbal encouragement has been shown to improve self-efficacy and physical performance specifically in sports performance, exercise, and rehabilitation [13]. Physiological studies, such as incremental tests in competitive runners [4], in non-athletes [5] and in university endurance athletes’ performances [6] found better performance following verbal encouragement. Positive verbal encouragement also induced peak force of the elbow flexors during an isometric muscle action [1], VO2 max, blood lactate concentration [7], and Wingate test parameters [8]. Moreover, verbal encouragement facilitates attentional focusing on the motor task and consequently performance improvement [9]. However, to our knowledge, no fMRI studies have yet been carried out to highlight the effect of verbal encouragement on brain activation, especially on networks subserving sensorimotor training/performance, motivation and, likely, attention. Therefore, it can be postulated that at least three cerebral circuits should be recruited: a sensorimotor network including the cerebellum, the ventral striatum for motivation, and the attentional/executive control network. Moreover, the cerebellum could constitute a hub coordinating these neural circuits.

Firstly, the sensorimotor circuit encompasses: the sensorimotor, medial, and lateral motor cortices, the ventral thalamus, the lenticular nucleus, and contralateral cerebellum (lobules IV-V, VIIIA-B) [10, 11]. This network is involved in programming, executing, and controlling motor performance by modulating the primary motor cortex through cerebello-thalamocortical connection [1214]. During training, performance improvement is associated with shifting activity within the cerebellum and the striatum. One general hypothesis would be that the cerebellum computes error-based signals based on “forward” models and predicts sensory consequence of movement. The cerebellum role in the temporal organization, coordination, and finessing of actions can be conceptualized in terms of adaptive filtering and internal modeling of the body, learned through adaptive processes dependent on sensory prediction errors [14, 15]. The prominent role of the cerebellum linked to sensorimotor networks and anticipatory control predicts that the cerebellum could play an important part in this force production task, mainly in lobule VI.

Secondly, the cerebellum also participates in the executive control (lobule VII) and the limbic (lobules VI and VIIA-B) networks. Manto et al. [16] postulated that the cerebellar network recruits cerebellar regions (lobule VI/VII) to execute attentional or motivational functions. Clinical and experimental data indicate that direct and indirect links connect the cerebellum to the cortical limbic regions [17, 18]. Animal and human studies show that attentional and motivational impairments occur when posterior lobe lesions affect lobules VI and VII and the limbic system [19, 20]. Therefore, the cerebellum could also take part in reallocating attention to the motor task being accomplished.

Additionally, mobilizing attention in response to encouragement could also be mediated by the ventral striatum implicated in the reward and motivational system. For instance, basal ganglia (mostly the ventral striatum and the pallidum) have been identified as responsible for motivating force production [21, 22]. Work ranging from electrophysiological and lesion experiments in animals to clinical population-based and imaging studies in humans has undoubtedly demonstrated that the basal ganglia, and the striatum in particular, plays a critical role in the planning, learning, and execution of a new motor skill.

Lastly, the cerebellum and basal ganglia are anatomically and functionally interconnected. Results from early imaging studies have revealed gross dynamic functional interactions occurring between the striatum and the cerebellum as subjects are practicing the motor skill until they reach asymptotic performance in the first training session [23].

More recent findings [24, 25] have shown the extent of dynamic cerebral interactions occurring between the striatum, the cerebellum, and the motor cortical areas motor skill learning. The cerebellum projects to the caudate and putamen via the thalamus, while the subthalamic nucleus is connected back to the cerebellum via pontine nuclei. Tractography studies have also found cerebello-striatal interconnections in humans. Therefore, cerebellar and basal ganglia may interact during reward-based or motivational learning.

This study aims to explore the role of the cerebro-cerebellar and striatal network during a motor performance enhanced by verbal encouragement. We used fMRI to detect the results of a specific activation of cognitive, motivational, and motor regions in the presence and absence of verbal encouragement associated with a handgrip task. Based on resting-state connectivity and functional imaging studies, we predicted that: (1) Lobule VI and ventral striatum might be specifically recruited and that (2) Lobule VI might constitute a hub for integrating motor, motivational, and cognitive aspects of motor performance during verbal encouragement.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Twenty-two right-handed volunteers (eleven men and eleven women, aged 30.3 ± 4.3 years) participated in the study. The participants were students from sport sciences. They were instructed to close their eyes and to motivate themselves. They had to strongly concentrate on their movements and to press the handgrip as hard as possible without making any sudden movements. All subjects were healthy and had no known neuromuscular disorders at the time of the study. They also answered a questionnaire, which ensures the security of the examination. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. The study was approved by the local ethics committee.

Paradigm

The protocol began with a familiarization and warm-up phase with the handgrip device. Lying under the MRI scanner, participants were asked to squeeze the handgrip device as hard and as fast as possible with headphones placed on their ears following two tasks: (i) a verbal encouragement task: participants were verbally encouraged throughout the contraction. A recorded human voice repeated firmly “GO” 20 times during 4.4 s to encourage the subjects. The vocal encouragement was recorded by WavePad Audio Editing Software. Verbal encouragements accompanied the movement to enhance the motivation so that participants could perform better force contraction (ii) a non encouragement task: exactly the same task as the first one but without verbal encouragement (Fig. 1). The order of the tasks was counterbalanced over subjects.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Scheme of paradigm. Participants executed five maximal voluntary contractions in each condition using handgrip device. In verbal encouragement condition, execution period was associated to pre-registered verbal encouragements

Every condition of the task included five trials. Each one began with a preparation period (6.6 s) where subjects were asked to motivate themselves and to concentrate on the movement that they had to execute. Then, the contraction period (4.4 s) started when participants heard the starting signal “GO”. They had to perform the task of squeezing the handgrip as hard as possible, developing a maximal voluntary contraction and then stop after a period of 4.4 s of muscular contraction. Finally, a long rest period (44 s) allowed participants to recover effort (resting state).

Force Data Acquisition

Force data was displayed on the laptop computer monitor in the control room. The MRI machine (General Electric) and Biopac system (Biopac MP150, System Inc., Santa Barbara, CA) simultaneously recorded the functional brain images and force data. Handgrip force was measured by a pressure transducer (TSD121B-MRI Hand Dynamometer). The handgrip device was an 8-m long cable, weighed 323 g and was connected to Biopac MP150. The metal-free handgrip device was held in the subject’s right hand in the MRI room and was connected to the Biopac located in the MRI control room. The output of the transducer was connected to an amplifier, DA100C, whose output was directed to AcqKnowledge software (Version 4.2, System Inc., Santa Barbara, CA). This amplifier simultaneously recorded on the hard disk of a laptop computer. The force signal was digitized at 200 samples/s. Moreover, the force output was amplified through a pressure gauge amplifier, and the data were sampled at 200 Hz.

fMRI Data Acquisition

Neuroimaging data were acquired with a 3-T, whole-body MRI system, equipped with a head volume coil (Signa; General Electric Medical System, Milwaukee, WI). Functional images with blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) contrast enhancement were acquired for each participant. Single-shot echo-planar images (EPIs) were also acquired using a typical T2* weighted gradient-echo sequence. Two runs of 124 EPI volumes with no gap were acquired for each subject (TR/TE = 2215/30 ms; flip angle = 80; matrix = 64 × 64; FOV = 240 × 240 mm2; isotropic voxel volume = 52.7 mm3). At the end of the first functional run, a series of T1-weighted 3D anatomical images was collected in order to provide detailed anatomical information. Conventional 3D imaging used an SPGR sequence (matrix: 256 × 256; flip angle: 15; TR/TE: 6.608/2.828 ms; 124 slices, 1.2 mm thickness). Head movements were restrained by cushions in order to reduce motion-related artifacts. All MR data were acquired in a single session for each subject.

fMRI Image Pre-processing

Image processing was performed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM12 software from the Methodology Group, Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, Institute of Neurology, London, UK: http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). SPM run with software Matlab R2012a.

For each participant, four scans were discarded; all EPI volumes were corrected to adjust for within-volume time differences and then realigned with the last volume to correct for head movements. Spatial smoothing was performed with an 8 mm t Gaussian kernel. Hemodynamic responses were modeled as a box-car function, convolved with a synthetic hemodynamic response function. To minimize the contamination of the data with movement artifacts, the motion vectors derived from the realignment were used as regressors to model the data.

Group Statistical Parametric Mapping

A single-subject fixed-effect model was created for each individual participant, in order to perform the based-conditions random-effect analysis. In each single-subject analysis, a significance level of P = 0.005 uncorrected was used to detect activated voxels. While data were acquired for the brain as a whole, SPM was constrained to specific hypotheses concerning cerebral, cerebellar, and brainstem regions. As we had a prior hypothesis about the localization of change, significance levels for t statistics were set at P < 0.005 uncorrected. In addition, we performed a small volume correction (SVC) at the voxel level using an 8 mm of radius around the voxel with the higher level of significance into the clusters that passed uncorrected thresholding, corrected for multiple comparisons, using p family-wise error (FEW) < 0.05. The small volume correction avoided false positive results by reducing the number of voxels entering the statistical computation.

Local maxima were located within anatomical regions and then assigned to a MNI coordinate and a Brodmann area.

Regions of Interest

Considering that our task solicits four functions: Language comprehension, motivation, concentration, and motor involvement, we looked for the main cortical areas responsible of these functions in the literature. (i) Left Primary Motor Cortex (M1) [BA 4], Kuypers [26] defined the motor cortex as the region giving origin to the corticospinal (pyramidal) tract. It corresponds to the agranular cortex of Brodmann’s areas 4 located in the precentral gyrus and corresponds to the primary motor cortex, characterized by the presence of the giant pyramidal cells of Betz. (ii) Right “Orbitofrontal Cortex” (OFC) [BA 47] is a brain area implicated in the motivation and the experienced value of rewards [2733]. This brain region is known to provide a common currency function, ensuring the value of different types of rewards to be represented and analyzed on a common value scale [3437]. (iii) Right “Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex” (DLPFC) [BA 46] is known for its involvement in the executive functions. It is an umbrella term for the management of cognitive processes, including working memory, cognitive flexibility, planning, and attentive concentration [38, 39]. DLPFC is also involved in information monitoring and abstract rule processing [4044]. (iv) Left “Superior Temporal Gyrus” (STG) [BA 22] encompasses Wernicke’s area. It supports the recognition of patterns in spoken language, language comprehension, semantic processing, and language interpretation [45]. For the ROI construction, we used WFU Pickatlas toolbox which automatically generates segmented ROI templates in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space [46]. The ROIs defined in this atlas were originally based on manual delineation of the brain region borders according to the MNI coordinates.

Psychophysiological Interactions

In order to highlight connectivity between the involved regions, a psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis was performed at three levels; cerebral, cerebellar, and brainstem. A psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis was conducted to investigate interactions among brain regions [47]. A PPI analysis tests the hypothesis that activity in one brain area can be explained by an interaction between the presence of a cognitive process and activity in another region of the brain. We predicted that activity of the source region will covary with activity of the target region. In our study, the PPI analysis was performed to examine the enhancement of effective connectivity between a source region and other brain regions during verbal encouragement [47, 48]. The connectivity is considered to be proportional to synaptic strength and to the number of synapses a target neuron establishes within the source population. The latter additionally depends on the activity of the target neurons.

PPI from Cerebrum Regions to the Cerebellum

Firstly, we tested 4 different PPI from cortical brain regions to the cerebellum. At the cortical level, we independently selected right OFC, right DLPFC, left STG, and left M1 as sources areas.

These regions are known to exert motivational, cognitive, and motor functions at the cortical level and to project to the cerebellum. Several target areas in the cerebellum have been identified, as it is known that the cerebellum is involved both in cognitive and motor processes. Thereby, we used a maximum probability map (MPM) in order to locate precisely which cerebellum areas were the most activated in the verbal vs. non-verbal condition. A MPM is a summary map of different histological maps. It is based on the idea of attributing each voxel of the reference space to the most likely cytoarchitectonic area, or the most likely myeloarchitectonically defined fiber-tract at this position. MPMs allow the definition of non-overlapping representations of all areas from a set of inevitably overlapping probabilistic maps [46, 49]. We used MPM Anatomy Toolbox v1.5 [46, 49, 50] to predict the cluster size and the percentage of activation in right anterior and posterior parts and in neighboring regions of lobule VI.

PPI from the Cerebellum to the Red Nucleus and Ventral Striatum

Based on the results of MPM, we independently selected right posterior and anterior parts of lobule VI as our source. The target area was first the left red nucleus. We then tested PPI connectivity from the right anterior and posterior lobule VI to the ventral stiratum. Moreover, we selected the red nucleus and the ventral striatum as target areas to explore the possibility that these structures may be implicated in cognitive and motivational functions.

PPI from the Red Nucleus to Cortical Regions

Finally, we tested four red nucleus PPI (source area) to right OFC, right DLPFC, left STG, and left M1 (target areas). On the cerebellar and brainstem levels, the assumption was that the red nucleus could reinforce and modulate cognitive, motivational, and motor processes.

Results

Force Results

A paired t test was conducted on mean force over time (N), which represents the total force divided by the force duration (4.4 s). While subject to verbal encouragement, participants squeezed the handgrip with greater force than in a non-verbal setting. Mean force for five trials measured in the whole group in the verbal condition (mean: 29.26, SD: 11.43) was significantly higher relative to the non-verbal condition (mean: 26.97, SD: 12.12) (t = 3.29, P = 0.004).

fMRI Results

SPM Analysis

Table 1 presents the statistical parametric results for the whole brain concerning the contrast “verbal encouragement versus no verbal encouragement.” Table 1 is partitioned in the cerebellum, brainstem, and cerebrum areas, and shows t values with a threshold at an uncorrected p < 0.005 value. The resulting SPM maps revealed significant activations in right and left lobule VI, in right DLPFC, in right, and left OFC, in right, and left M1, in left STG, and in the left red nucleus.

Table 1 Results of activated regions in verbal versus no verbal encouragement contrast

PPI and MPM Analyses

Table 2a shows the connectivity from cerebral source regions to cerebellar target areas. Consistent with the literature and our hypothesis, the left M1 is significantly (FWE-corrected P = 0.001) connected to the right anterior lobule VI area (Fig. 2a). The right OFC, right DLPFC, and left STG are significantly connected (FWE-corrected P < 0.003) with the right posterior lobule VI area (Fig. 2b). The extent of the activity in Fig. 2b takes into account the activity which overlaps for the three regions.

Table 2 Connectivity from source to target regions
Fig. 2
figure 2

Activity patterns of anterior and posterior lobule VI displayed on the MNI single-subject template. a Cluster revealed by the PPI from left motor cortex source region. b Cluster revealed by the PPI from right DLPFC. The extent of the activity in figure a takes into account the activity which overlaps for the three source regions (OFC, DLPFC, and STG)

Table 3, reporting detailed activity in target areas, displays cytoarchitectonic area/target area ratio (percentage), cluster size (voxels number), and MNI coordinates (X, Y, Z) that correspond to SUIT probabilistic atlas of the cerebellar lobules in the anatomical space defined by the MNI152 template [51] as well as MRI atlas of the human cerebellum [52]. MPM analysis reveals that the source region M1 (Fig. 2a, Table 3) co-activates with a cluster located on the right anterior lobule VI (132 voxels) and a cluster located on the right lobule V (51 voxels).

Table 3 Functional activation clusters in target areas

Source areas OFC, DLPFC, and STG (Fig. 2b, Table 3) present two common co-activation clusters: one in posterior lobule VI of the right cerebellum hemisphere, the second one in the right lobule VII (Crus I). The MPM analysis reveals that these cognitive and motivational areas were more associated with the right posterior lobule VI than with the right posterior lobule VII (Crus I). For example, considering the source region OFC, the cluster in the posterior lobule VI (67 voxels) target area was considerably larger than that of the right lobule VII (Crus I) (6 voxels).

Table 2b shows the connectivity from anterior and posterior right lobule VI source areas to brainstem target area. In line with the literature and our hypothesis, the PPI analysis reveals that anterior and posterior lobule VI source areas (Fig. 2) are significantly connected (FWE-corrected P < 0.05) to the red nucleus (MNI coordinates: −9 −22 −4 and −3 −22 −5 respectively) (Fig. 3a).

Figure 3
figure 3

Activity patterns of target areas displayed on the MNI single-subject template. a Clusters revealed by the PPI from the right posterior lobule VI source region to the left red nucleus. b Clusters revealed by the PPI from the right posterior lobule VI source region to left ventral striatum. c Clusters revealed by the PPI from the left red nucleus to left STG, left M1, right DLPFC, and right OFC. L left; R right

The activity appears to be mostly localized in the caudal ventrolateral level of the red nucleus. This region corresponds to the magnocellular red nucleus, which gives rise to the crossed rubrospinal tract to spinal motor neurons and interneurons of distal muscles.

Table 2c shows the connectivity from anterior and posterior right lobule VI source areas to the ventral striatum target area. The PPI analysis reveals that anterior and posterior lobule VI source areas (Fig. 3b) are significantly connected (FWE-corrected P < 0.05) to ventral striatum.

Table 2d shows the connectivity between the left red nucleus to left M1, right OFC, right DLPFC, left STG. We selected the left red nucleus because of its decussation with the lobule VI; we did not find any activity in the right red nucleus. The left red nucleus source area is functionally connected (FWE-corrected P < 0.05) to left M1 (110 voxels), right OFC (177 voxels), right DLPFC (98 voxels), and left SFG (95 voxels) (Fig. 3c).

Figure 4 illustrates the changes of regression slope (PPI) between the left red nucleus activity (−3, −22, −11) and right OFC activity (48, 17, −5), depending on verbal encouragements.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Graphical display illustrating the psychophysiological interaction between the left red nucleus and right OFC for all subjects. The BOLD values for the voxels (−3,−22, −11) in the left red nucleus and (48, 17, −5) in the right OFC are plotted during verbal (green) non-verbal (blue) conditions. Slopes are significantly different between the two conditions (P < 0.05). There is no significant difference between the correlation coefficients

This graphic highlights the most important interaction found in Table 2d (t = 4.45; P = 0.01), which took place between the red nucleus (brainstem) and the OFC (cerebrum). This result supports our hypothesis that the motivational component of the task is possibly maintained through this interaction.

Taken together, the results indicate significant connectivity from left M1 to the right anterior lobule VI of the cerebellum (P = 0.001) as well as significant connectivity from right OFC (P = 0.003), right DLPFC (P = 0.02), and left SFG (P = 0.001) to the right posterior lobule VI of the cerebellum. There is also significant connectivity (P = 0.03) between both the right anterior and posterior lobule VI of the cerebellum to the left red nucleus (magnocellular part). Connectivity from left red nucleus also shows significant values to left M1 (0.03), to right OFC (0.03), to right DLPFC (0.02), and to left STG (0.03) (Fig. 5).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Functional connectivity from all interactions between the cerebrum, the cerebellum, and the red nucleus. The arrows reflect connectivity from the source to the target area, each of these with a varying starting path, all of them ending in the left red nucleus. The blue arrow shows PPI from L-M1 to the cerebellum (R anterior Lobule VI), while the yellow ones illustrate PPI from R DLPFC to the cerebellum (R posterior Lobule VI). The orange arrows show PPI from R OFC to the cerebellum (R posterior Lobule VI), and the purple arrows show PPI from L STG to the cerebellum (R posterior Lobule VI). The numbers reflect FWE-corrected p values (all P < 0.05). CB6 ant Anterior Lobule VI, CB6 post Posterior Lobule VI, L left, R right

These observations suggest that connectivity could form closed-loop connections. Cortical motor area (left M1), cortical motivational, and cognitive areas (right OFC, right DLPFC, and left SFG) project respectively to right anterior and posterior lobule VI areas. In turn, these areas project to the left red nucleus, from which the same cortical areas (left M1, right OFC, right DLPFC, and left SFG) receive input.

Figure 5 shows the significant connectivity that we examined between cerebral, cerebellar, and brainstem regions reinforced by the verbal encouragements. The numbers reflect FWE-corrected p values for each of the connected regions.

Discussion

The present study investigated whether verbal encouragement during a unilateral handgrip task might induce changes in force production, brain activity, and connectivity between the cerebrum, the cerebellar, and the brainstem in healthy individuals. Overall, the current results support the hypothesis that the right lobule VI of the cerebellum could constitute a hub that integrates motor, cognitive, and motivational functions. Lobule VI integrates these functions and sends them back polysynaptically to said cortex through the left red nucleus.

Force

All subjects had a significantly higher mean force in verbal conditions than in non-verbal conditions. These muscular results are in line with other studies performed in physiology: (1) verbal encouragement improved performance in different fields such as: (2) Wingate test aerobic parameters [8], (3) VO2 max and blood lactate concentration during a treadmill test [7], (4) muscular endurance [53], (5) elbow flexors during an isometric muscle action [1], (6) knee extension [3], and (7) triceps surae flexion [54] during maximal voluntary isometric contraction. Belanger and McComas [55] claimed that the activation of motor units may be inhibited when a maximal voluntary contraction is required due to a supraspinal drive acting on the motor units. McNair et al. [1] assessed that verbal encouragement may lead to a disengagement of this supraspinal inhibition, thus resulting in an enhancement of muscle strength.

Lobule VI Forms a Hub

PPI analysis revealed that the left M1 is significantly connected to the right anterior lobule VI area (Fig. 2a). MPM analysis revealed that the source region M1 (Table 3) co-activates with a cluster located on the right anterior lobule VI and with a cluster located in the right lobule V. The majority of the activated voxels (38 %) were assigned to lobule VI, whereas a smaller part of the cluster (14.9 %) was allocated to the lobule V adjacent to lobule VI.

The present findings corroborate with human and non-human studies showing that motor-related cortices project primarily to the contralateral anterior cerebellar lobe (lobules I–V) with an extension into medial lobule VI, and lobule VIII via the caudal basis pontis. Our results support also the anatomical data of Kelly and Strick [56], sensorimotor representations of the body that have been found in the superior/anterior-most part of the cerebellum (lobules III-V-VI). Smith and Bourbonnais [57] found that, in cerebellar lobules V and VI of monkeys, spike frequency augments with increasing grip force amplitude. fMRI studies found that cerebellar activation was positively correlated with isometric wrist force amplitude [58] and flexor carpi radialis EMG activity (i.e., the agonist muscle), with grip force amplitude [5961], and with force amplitude during isometric contractions [62]. The motor cerebellum involvement in this task could be associated with motor learning, especially lobules V/VI/VIII, which are involved in motor learning specifically during the early phases [16]. Numerous studies over the last 40 years document that the cerebellum plays a key role in many types of motor learning. Some studies hypothesized that the role of the cerebellum in motor learning is to form internal models of action [63], whereas others attributed cerebellar activity to the generation of prediction errors [64] and motor timing.

Cerebellum activity optimizes cerebral cortical functioning by regulating the timing of movement and sensation [6567]. Several studies showed an important contribution of both cerebellum and basal ganglia in the neural representation of time [6870]. The cerebellum is known to proceed as an internal timing system that maintains the temporal representation of prominent actions across hundreds of milliseconds [7174]. The strong implication of cerebellum in motor and perceptual timing includes numerous time-based behaviors as rhythmic tapping [75], anticipatory adjustments of the limb segments [76], discrimination of temporal intervals [77, 78] and timing during conditioning tasks [79, 80]. Specifically in lobule VI, the magnitude of change in brain activation covaries with the magnitude of temporal change in the rhythmic stimulus interval [81].

Otherwise, source areas OFC, DLPFC, and STG (Table 3) present two common co-activation clusters located in posterior lobule VI of the right cerebellum hemisphere, and in the right posterior lobule VII (Crus I). The MPM analyses reveals that these regions were more associated with the right posterior lobule VI than with the right posterior lobule VII. The majority of the activated voxels (>60 %) were within lobule VI, whereas a smaller part of the cluster (<10 %) was situated in the adjacent area of lobule VI (lobule VII, Crus I) (Fig. 2b).

This anatomical segregation is in agreement with O’Reilly and colleagues [82] who divided the cerebellum into at least two functional zones: the primary sensorimotor (Lobules V, VI, and VIII) and supramodal (lobules VIIA, Crus I, and II). Likewise, Stoodley and colleagues [83] found that cognitive tasks tend to engage lateral regions of lobule VI and lobule VII (Crus I, Crus II, and VIIB). These observations confirmed our hypothesis, which stipulated that lobule VI might constitute a hub for integrating cognitive aspects of motor performance during verbal encouragement.

To facilitate the generation of future hypotheses related to motor timing or supplementary closed-connectivity loops, data for activation within SMA are also reported in Table 1. The SMA has been shown to play a special role for the coordination and initiation of temporal organization of sequencing movements, in particular during self-initiated and multi-segmental voluntary movements [8486]. Also, SMA has been shown to be the recipient of posterior parietal axons both directly and via the basal ganglia. SMA and lateral premotor cortex widely projects to basal ganglia system which is thought to play a key role in selecting and adapting motor programs based on initial conditions using proprioceptive input, or in formulating an internal model of body kinesthesia. Moreover, recent findings propose an SMA involvement in both classical and “extended” mirror neuron regions during the observation and execution of movements [87, 88].

Red Nucleus

PPI analyses reveal that the red nucleus is concurrently connected to both the right lobule VI (anterior and posterior parts) and cortical areas (left STG, left M1, right OFC, and right DLPFC).

Firstly, PPI analysis reveals that right anterior and posterior parts of the lobule VI source area are significantly connected to the left red nucleus (Table 2b, Fig. 3a). This result indicates that the red nucleus integrates motor and motivational functions established from anatomical connection with the right lobule VI. This finding is supported by a functional connectivity MRI study [89] using independent component analysis, which identifies that the anterior motor cerebellum (lobules V–VI) and the posterior cerebellum (lobules VI–VII) are intrinsically connected networks encompassing RN. Secondly, PPI analysis reveals also that RN is connected with left STG, left M1, right OFC, and right DLPFC (Table 2d, Fig. 3c) reporting a key motor, cognitive, and motivational involvement of the red nucleus. More specifically, the positive correlation between OFC and RN exposed an important interconnection between both structures (Fig. 4). This result suggests that OFC activity is modulated by RN and that the motivational component of the task is possibly maintained through this interaction. Nioche and colleagues resting-state study [89] have shown that the human RN participates in a widespread cortical functional network including associative prefrontal, parietal, occipital, and temporal areas. The present interaction between OFC and RN has already been confirmed by anatomical connections between RN and cortical cortex demonstrated in Nioche study [89]. They highlighted that RN displayed bilateral temporal coherences with the prefrontal cortex (BA 11/45/47), (BA7), the superior temporal cortex (BA 20), DLPFC (BA 9), and the thalamus.

Taken together, PPI results from the cortical cortex (left STG, Left M1, Right DLPFC, and right OFC) to the right anterior and posterior lobule VI, to the left red nucleus and back to the cortical cortex (Table 2) suggest the existence of a closed-connectivity-loop modulated by the red nucleus. This submission is broadly consistent with the neuroanatomical studies that have shown bidirectional pathways between the cerebellum areas involved in motor and cognitive domains and their counterparts in the cerebral cortex. The motor and non-motor cerebellum regions form bidirectional connections with the motor, prefrontal, posterior parietal, and superior temporal cortices, limbic system, brainstem nuclei via corticopontocerebellar pathways [18, 90, 91]. The cerebellar motor domains have been shown to both receive inputs from and send outputs to M1, forming a closed-loop circuit.

Regions within the non-motor domains of the cerebellar cortex also participate in distinct closed-loop circuits with regions of the prefrontal cortex [56, 92]. Using DTI-based tractography in human, Palesi and colleagues [93] showed contralateral massive efferences from neocerebellum to prefrontal cortex (BA 9–12, 25, 46–47), parietal (BA 1–3), and temporal cortices (BA 41–42), and, to a lesser extent, to basal ganglia.

Thus, multiple closed-loop circuits represent a fundamental feature of interactions between the cerebellum and the cerebral cortex. The cerebellum displays a multi-scale modular organization including for instance biochemical longitudinal zonation with zebrin [9498], olivocerebellar microzones grouped into functional microcomplexes consisting of multiple cerebello-olivo-cerebellar modules [98103] and that, lastly, are segregated into parallel, large-scale closed loops subserving specific sensorimotor, cognitive, and emotional functions [100, 104106]. These anatomical bases could explain that cerebellum modular organization may stand an important role for treating cognitive, motivational, and motor output induced by the task through the cerebro-cerebellar anatomical connections.

The polysynaptic interactions associated with the cognitive role of the RN related to salience detection and executive control [89] lead to the modulation of motivation through a closed-connectivity loop. Essentially, the cerebellar links with the OFC, DLPFC, and STG are in line with the cerebellar theory of Ito [107]. The present connectivity patterns also provide support for a distinct motivational/default network in the cerebellum [108, 109].

Otherwise, cognitive and motor functions could also be modulated via another loop, the neocerebello-rubro-olivo-neocerebellar loop. The main efferent from the human red nucleus is that to the bulbar principal olivary nucleus (PO) and DTI studies [110, 111] have shown that RN is strongly connected to PO via the central tegmental tract. The PO neurons’ axons, the climbing fibers, terminate as excitatory inputs onto Purkinje cells in the cerebellar cortex. The Purkinje cells, consecutively, exert a powerful synaptic inhibition onto these cerebellar nuclear neurons [112]. Neuronal output from dentate nucleus reaches the thalamus through the (parvicellular part of the) red nucleus but without forming synapse with rubral neurons [113].

The neocerebello-rubro-olivo-neocerebellar loop is implicated in motor and mainly cognitive networks and functions [114]. The cortical projections are directed to the red nucleus, which sends efferents in the central tegmental tract to olivary subnuclei. These subnuclei receive spinal cord input and which are reciprocally interconnected with the cerebellar motor and non-motor lobules.

Considering the major role of the olivocerebellar system in motor learning, this loop might have played an important role in this study. However, we could not confirm this connectivity loop, probably because of anatomical and statistical reasons: (1) the small size of the PO. The PO is a small nucleus compared to the spatial resolution of smoothed functional images. (2) Restriction may be due to the low spatial resolution of the fMRI images and statistical thresholding may have been too stringent.

OFC and Ventral Striatum

Activation of right OFC, right and left ventral striatum (Table 1) is correlated with verbal encouragement. In humans, motivational deficits can arise from damage to the basal ganglia, diencephalon, limbic system, nucleus accumbens, nucleus of the diagonal band of Broca, internal capsule, globus pallidus, and/or precommissural gray and OFC.

PPI analysis from OFC to the right posterior lobule VI (Table 2a, Fig. 2b) and from the left red nucleus to right OFC (Table 2d, Fig. 3c) denote that motivation induced by verbal encouragement could be maintained by OFC neurons. OFC activity is shown to be involved in motivational processes. It appears to subserve evaluation and learning of stimulus-incentive associations, and thus plays a key role in the motivational control of goal-directed behavior. OFC is also involved in context-dependent stimulus coding, by responding differently to the same instruction cue as a function of its motivational significance [115].

In conjunction with OFC activity, significant ventral striatum activity (Table 1) suggests that this structure may also play an important role to support motivation during this motor task. This observation is in line with previous studies reporting that the basal ganglia can act as an interface between non-motor processes and motor processes [116]. The limbic part of the basal ganglia, which includes the ventral striatum and ventral pallidum, may convert motivation signals into motor signals [117]. Other studies have described striatal nuclei as critical nodes of topographically organized connections between cortical and subcortical areas. These areas are involved in processing motor, affective, and cognitive information [118].

The dense interconnection between the cerebellum and the ventral striatum [119, 120] may explain the significant PPI connectivity from lobule VI to ventral striatum (Table 2c, Fig. 3b). Both structures receive input from and send output to the cerebral cortex. Recent findings reveal the existence of an anatomical substrate for the bidirectional communication between the cerebellum and the basal ganglia. In particular, studies on rats [121] and monkeys have demonstrated that the cerebellum sends a strong disynaptic projection to the striatum through the thalamus. Similar evidence has been recently reported in the human brain [122]. Hoshi and colleagues [120] showed that the dentate nucleus has a disynaptic projection to an input stage of basal ganglia processing, the striatum. Besides its motivational involvement in this task, the striatum may also be involved in motor learning [23, 123] in conjunction with the cerebellum and the motor cortex [124, 125]. Penhune and Steele [13] propose that the role of the striatum is to learn predictive associations between the individual movements in the sequence. Furthermore, the role of the cerebellum is suggested to create an optimal internal model, while the function of M1 is the retention of the motor memory.

PPI connectivity from the posterior lobule VI to the ventral striatum (Table 2c, Fig. 3b) correlated to listed anatomical connections between these two structures, which can modulate motivation through multisynaptic loops with the cerebral cortex. The ventral striatum, mainly the nucleus accumbens, receives inputs from the “limbic” prefrontal cortex, [126, 127] and its outputs are directed back to the cerebral cortex via the striato-pallidothalamic and striato-nigro-thalamic pathways. Large numbers of cortical neurons project to the input stages of the basal ganglia and the cerebellum. Similarly, the output stages of the basal ganglia and the cerebellum project to subdivisions of the ventroanterior and ventrolateral thalamus. These regions of the thalamus then project back upon the cerebral cortex. Inputs from the cerebral cortices terminate in the pontine nuclei then project to the cerebellum. Subsequently, the feedback projections travel from the cerebellum via the deep cerebellar nuclei and terminate in the thalamus, which then sends projection back to both motor and non-motor cerebral cortices [128130]. Thus, the cerebello-thalamocortical loop, formed by the interaction between the input and output stages of processing, were believed to provide a route by which information from cognitive, motivational, and motor domains influenced the generation and control of movement, all while participants were actively motivated.

Notwithstanding, this study has some limitations. The main one is the PPI interpretation way: we used a psychophysiological analysis demonstrating how the contribution of one region to another is altered by the experimental context. However, we did not analyze the other way of interpretation regarding how an area’s response to an experimental context is modulated by input from another region. We definitely cannot exclude a possible different effect from this other way of interpretation. Further investigations are required for assessing effective (causal) connectivity using causal modeling (DCM) [131]. Other limitations are the lack of primary olivary nucleus activity and the failure to detect the neocerebello-rubro-olivo-neocerebellar loop. Resting-state and tractography methods should partly overcome these limitations.

Conclusion

Several conclusions can be drawn: (1) verbal encouragement strongly and positively influences muscular force, (2) the right lobule VI (anterior and posterior parts) is dually recruited by the motoric and motivational aspect of the sensorimotor performance in conjunction with the contralateral red nucleus, (3) RN plays a main role in maintaining motivation through interactions with OFC and ventral striatum, and (4) these circuits correspond to a closed-connectivity-loop modulated by the red nucleus, a cerebro-cerebello-striatal loop and probably a cerebello-rubro-olivo-cerebellar loop.