Introduction

Epidemiological studies conducted in the United States estimate that the average prevalence rate for childhood (age 9–13) and adolescent (age 13–18) psychopathology is between 13 % and 22 % (Costello et al. 2003; Merikangas et al. 2010). Further, of those who have any disorder, 25 %–40 % have two or more disorders (Costello et al. 2003). Research examining the latent structure of common mental disorders has supported the conceptualization of two broad, higher-order Internalizing and Externalizing factors. This structure has been found in both child and adolescent samples (Cosgrove et al. 2011; Hewitt et al. 1997; Lahey et al. 2004, 2008b). The Internalizing factor reflects anxiety, fear, and misery and typically includes major depressive disorder (MDD), generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), separation anxiety disorder (SAD), panic disorder (PD), and phobias (Cosgrove et al. 2011; Krueger 1999). The Externalizing factor reflects acting-out, aggression, and antisocial behavior and usually includes attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), and conduct disorder (CD) in children and adolescents (Lahey et al. 2004, 2008b).

Although internalizing and externalizing problems are conceptualized as distinct factors, they are often correlated (Angold et al. 1999; Cosgrove et al. 2011; Kessler et al. 2005; Krueger 1999; Krueger et al. 1998; Krueger and Markon 2006; O’Connor et al. 1998; Pesenti-Gritti et al. 2008; Rhee et al. 2007). The correlation between Internalizing and Externalizing factors is estimated to be between 0.66 and 0.72 in children (Cosgrove et al. 2011; Lahey et al. 2004), suggesting substantial co-occurrence of disorders across factors. Children and adolescents with co-occurring psychopathology are at increased risk for later alcohol and substance use problems (Miller-Johnson et al. 1998; Pardini et al. 2007), increased antisocial behavior (Polier et al. 2012), arrests, sexual promiscuity (Dishion 2000), suicidal ideation (Capaldi 1992), repeat admission to inpatient services (Fite et al. 2008), and adult psychopathology (Dalsgaard et al. 2002; Fischer et al. 2002; Fombonne et al. 2001).

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Co-occurring Internalizing and Externalizing Behaviors

Despite the high prevalence and negative outcomes, relatively little is known about the development of co-occurring disorders. One possible route to co-occurrence is through common genetic and environmental influences. The twin study method allows for an estimation of the contribution of genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental influences on a behavior or set of behaviors. Genetic influences, and more specifically additive genetic influences, refer to the sum of effects across multiple gene alleles which are directly inherited from one’s parents. Shared environmental influences are aspects of the environment that affect both twins and make them more similar, and nonshared environmental influences are those that are unique to each twin (as well as measurement error) and contribute to differences between twins. Twin studies have been used to examine the overlap of genetic and environmental influences between internalizing and externalizing behaviors and results have been mixed. For instance, in a community sample of 10- to 18-year-old twins, O’Connor et al. (1998) examined checklists of depression and antisocial symptoms and found that 45 % of their co-occurrence was attributed to genetics, 30 % to shared environment, and 25 % to nonshared environment. However, Gjone and Stevenson (1997a) found that shared environment primarily accounted for the co-occurrence between Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach 1991) internalizing and externalizing scores for children between the ages of 5 and 15, although genetic influences contributed as well, especially in older children. Differences between these studies are expected, though, due to different conceptualizations of internalizing and externalizing behaviors (i.e., symptoms of a single disorder vs. a broad measure). However, others examining the CBCL in a slightly older sample found that both shared environment and genetic factors contributed about equally to the co-occurrence of internalizing and externalizing behaviors, with negligible influence of non-shared environmental factors (Pesenti-Gritti et al. 2008). Although the CBCL provides a broad measure of externalizing, it does not tap into all symptoms of disorders on the externalizing spectrum. For example, the externalizing scale of the CBCL does not include symptoms of ADHD. Perhaps a more comprehensive way to examine the covariation between internalizing and externalizing problems is to conceptualize each as the latent factor underlying internalizing and externalizing disorders.

A recent study by Cosgrove et al. (2011) examined the genetic and environmental effects of the covariance between an Internalizing factor (including MDD, GAD, and SAD) and an Externalizing factor (including ADHD, ODD, and CD) with manifest variables assessed using a clinical interview in a large community sample of 12- to 18-year-old adolescent twins. Using a bivariate common factor model, they concluded that the co-occurrence between these Internalizing and Externalizing factors was due to common genetic influences (62 %) as well as nonshared environmental influences (38 %), with no shared environmental influences. Another recent study (Lahey et al. 2011) examined a slightly younger sample of twins (i.e., ages 9–17 years) and assessed a wider range of psychopathology, including agoraphobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), social phobia (SP), and specific phobia on the Internalizing factor. They tested several models of the structure of genetic and environmental effects of these disorders and similarly concluded that co-occurrence among the disorders can be explained primarily by genetics, with very little influences of shared and nonshared environment. Overall, the majority of the research suggests that genetic influences contribute, at least to some degree, to the co-occurrence between internalizing and externalizing disorders. Conclusions about environmental influences are more difficult to establish. Studies measuring the dimensions of internalizing and externalizing with the CBCL tend to find more shared environmental effects, whereas studies assessing the individual disorders that make up the Internalizing and Externalizing factors tend to find smaller or nonsignificant shared environmental effects contributing to the co-occurrence.

The Role of Negative Affect

Another route to co-occurrence of internalizing and externalizing disorders may be through common underlying dispositions, such as temperament or personality factors (Clark 2005; Bijttebier and Roeyers 2009; Lilienfeld 2003; Muris and Ollendick 2005; Nigg 2006; Saudino 2005; Tackett 2006). Although several dispositions may interact to influence the development of specific psychopathology, it has been proposed that negative affect may play a causal role in the development of both internalizing and externalizing disorders (Lilienfeld 2003). Negative affect (sometimes referred to as negative emotionality) is defined as the stable and pervasive propensity to experience aversive emotions, such as nervousness, anger, guilt, rejection, and sadness (Watson and Clark 1984). Research has shown that negative affect is moderately positively correlated with both internalizing and externalizing disorders in children (Anthony et al. 2002; Gjone and Stevenson 1997b; Guerin et al. 1997; Lengua et al. 1998; Muris et al. 2007; Rhee et al. 2007; Singh and Waldman 2010) and has been conceptualized as a predisposition for both (Clark et al. 1994; Lonigan et al. 2011; Muris and Ollendick 2005; Schmitz et al. 1999; Tackett 2006), whereas other dispositions are often conceptualized as specifically related to either internalizing (i.e., positive affect; Watson and Naragon-Gainey 2010) or externalizing (i.e. constraint; Krueger et al. 2002) disorders. Given the idea that negative affect may underlie both internalizing and externalizing disorders, examining their common genetic and environmental influences may provide a greater understanding of vulnerability to childhood psychopathology, and may inform the development of effective interventions.

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Covariation of Negative Affect and Internalizing Behaviors

Only one study to date examined the common genetic and environmental influences of negative affect and internalizing disorders in children or adolescents. Goldsmith and Lemery (2000) examined the longitudinal bivariate relationships between fear (negative affect as a result of anticipated threat) and overanxious and separation anxiety symptoms in twins ages 3 to 9 years. They found that fear and overanxious symptoms had common genetic influences, and fear and separation anxiety had common shared environmental influences. Although there is limited research examining young samples, several studies have examined the common genetic and environmental influences of neuroticism (a personality construct similar to negative affect) and internalizing disorders in adults (Hettema et al. 2004, 2006; Jardine et al. 1984). In these studies, the relationship between neuroticism and internalizing disorders was largely due to common genetic influences.

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Covariation of Negative Affect and Externalizing Behaviors

Others have examined the common genetic and environmental influences on negative affect and externalizing disorders (Singh and Waldman 2010; Waldman et al. 2011) and found that the relationship between them is due largely to common genetic influences and, to a lesser degree, nonshared environmental influences. In the most comprehensive study thus far, Singh and Waldman (2010) examined the genetic and environmental influences on the covariation between the manifest variables of negative affect, ODD, CD, inattention, and hyperactivity/impulsivity in a sample of 838 four- to 17-year-old twins. They first fit bivariate models to negative affect and each externalizing disorder separately. They then fit a multivariate model to negative affect and all the externalizing disorders together. Their results suggested that negative affect and externalizing disorders have common genetic variance (25–67 %), nonshared environment variance (5–23 %), but not shared environment variance. Further, CD, inattention, and hyperactivity/impulsivity shared all of their additive genetic influences with negative affect.

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Covariation of Negative Affect, Internalizing, and Externalizing Behaviors

The first reported twin study of the covariation of negative affect, internalizing, and externalizing behavior was conducted by Gjone and Stevenson (1997b). This study used the EAS Temperament Survey (Buss and Plomin 1984) to assess emotionality (the tendency to cry and get upset easily, and react intensely when upset) which is similar to negative affect. The authors conducted a series of bivariate analyses examining the longitudinal relation between emotionality and behavioral problems, including anxious/depressed behavior, attention problems, delinquent behavior, and aggressive behavior (assessed with the CBCL), in 7- to 17-year-old twins. Their results suggested that there were no common genetic or environmental factors between emotionality and anxious/depressed behavior or between emotionality and delinquent behavior. Further, only common genetic factors (not environmental factors) accounted for the relations between emotionality and aggressive behavior and between emotionality and attention problems.

Using a younger twin sample, Schmitz et al. (1999) reported findings on bivariate relationships between infant emotionality (as measured by the Colorado Childhood Temperament Inventory; CCTI; Rowe and Plomin 1977) and internalizing and externalizing scores from the CBCL at age 4 years. Consistent with Gjone and Stevenson (1997b), they concluded that common genetic factors accounted for the relation between emotionality and externalizing behaviors, although they also found a similar relation with internalizing behaviors. Rhee et al. (2007) intended to extend the findings from the Schmitz et al. (1999) study by examining the relation between emotionality in infancy (assessed with the CCTI) and internalizing and externalizing behaviors assessed (with the CBCL) between ages 4 and 12 years. This was the first study to explore emotionality, internalizing behaviors, and externalizing behaviors together in a single multivariate model. Because the researchers were interested in disentangling the effects of gender, they explored these relations in males and females separately. They reported that, for males, emotionality did not share genetic or shared environmental influences with internalizing or externalizing, though nonshared environmental influences had a small but significant inverse influence on externalizing. For females, emotionality, internalizing, and externalizing only had shared environmental influences in common. However, Rhee et al. were concerned that their mulitivariate analyses were underpowered and therefore reinterpreted the data by dividing phenotypic correlations between emotionality and internalizing behaviors and emotionality and externalizing behaviors into phenotypic correlations attributable to common genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental effects. Results from those analyses suggested that, in males, the covariation between emotionality and internalizing behaviors and between emotionality and externalizing behaviors was explained by common genetic influences and shared environmental influences. In females, the covariation between emotionality and internalizing behaviors and emotionality and externalizing behaviors was explained mostly by common shared environmental influences.

Finally, a recent study by Tackett et al. (2011) examined whether the genetic influences on negative emotionality (as assessed by the Child and Adolescent Dispositions Scale; CADS; Lahey et al. 2008a) were shared with the genetic influences contributing to the co-occurrence between MDD and CD. Using data from over 2,000 twin pairs between the ages of 6 and 18 years, Tackett et al. examined multivariate models on manifest variables separately for males and females. They found common genetic (for males only), shared environmental, and nonshared environmental influences on negative emotionality, internalizing (MDD), and externalizing (CD).

Overall, research examining the common genetic and environmental influences on negative affect, internalizing behavior, and externalizing behavior suggests that primarily genetic influences contribute to their covariation; however, the evidence for the contribution of shared and nonshared environmental influences is mixed, suggesting the need for additional research to clarify the influences of these factors. Further, only two previous studies have examined negative affect, internalizing problems, and externalizing problems in the same model (Rhee et al. 2007; Tackett et al. 2011). To date, no studies have examined the genetic and environmental influences on the covariation between negative affect, internalizing, and externalizing, while conceptualizing internalizing and externalizing as factors that capture the shared variance among the disorders that make up those dimensions. The current study addresses this gap in the literature with a sample of child and adolescent twins. Examining the common genetic and environmental influences may aid in understanding vulnerability to the development of co-occurring internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. For instance, if the data show that common genetic influences are associated with negative affect, internalizing, and externalizing, then this would suggest that genes are conferring risk for a child to experience negative affect as well as conferring risk for developing a range of psychological disorders. If shared environmental influences are associated with negative affect, internalizing, and externalizing, then this would indicate that perhaps aspects of the rearing environment (e.g., low parental warmth, chaotic home environment, low economic resources, etc.) promote the development of negative affect and a range of psychological disorders in children. Further, using a factor model will allow for including the full spectrum of internalizing and externalizing problems while also decreasing the influence of error in the model. Based on previous findings, it was hypothesized that the covariation between the three factors (Negative Affect, Internalizing, and Externalizing) would be mostly explained by common underlying genetic influences. It was expected that shared and nonshared environmental influences would also contribute to the covariation, though to a lesser degree than genetic influences.

Method

Participants

Twins for this study were originally recruited through the Florida State Twin Registry (Taylor et al. 2006) via the Florida Twin Project on Reading (e.g.,. Taylor and Schatschneider 2010) Twins were identified through a statewide database of student academic test scores and parent-provided school registration information. Recruitment packets for inclusion in the Florida State Twin Registry were given to parents of children in this database who had the same last name, birth date, and school and were in grades K through 5. These packets contained a letter about the study, a consent form for the parents, and a five-question form assessing zygosity that has been used in other twin studies and has demonstrated a high accuracy rate (Lykken et al. 1990). As part of a larger study concerning the behavior and environment of twins, parents whose families were part of the Florida State Twin Registry were mailed consent and assent forms, questionnaires, and a cover letter explaining the project. One parent (or guardian) was asked to complete the questionnaire. Given the young age of the twins, it was expected that a parent would provide the most valid and accurate report of the twins’ symptomatology. Parents received a $30 gift card to the retailer of their choice for their participation.

Through the Florida State Twin Registry, 1,624 same-sex twin families were sent packets requesting their participation. Of the packets sent out, 150 (9 %) were returned by the post office as undeliverable. Of the packets delivered, 718 families (49 %) replied, and 693 families (47 %) agreed to participate and returned a completed packet. Two families were not included because the zygosity of the twins could not be determined. The final sample for this study included 691 same-sex 7- to 13-year-old (M = 10.08, SD = 1.73) twin pairs and their parents (350 families with female twins; 341 families with male twins). Racial composition of the present sample as reported by parents was 70 % White, 12 % African American, 9 % mixed race, 2 % Asian, and 5 % other with 2 % failing to provide information about race. Parent reported ethnic composition was 24 % Hispanic and 70 % non-Hispanic with 6 % not reporting ethnicity. Parents reported household income on a scale ranging from 1 “less than $10,000” to 6 “$90,000 or more”. The mean response was 3.98 (SD = 1.67), indicating an average household income of $50,000 to $69,000. The modal reported income was $90,000 or more. Income was not reported by 3.5 % of the sample. There were no significant differences between MZ and DZ twins on parent reported race (χ 2 (4, N = 675) = 4.60, p = 0.33), ethnicity (χ 2 (1, N = 652) = 1.46, p = 0.23) or income (χ 2 (6, N = 667) = 8.14, p = 0.23). Potential sampling biases were examined by comparing participants and non-participants on demographic variables available from 2000 census data by zip code in which the twins’ family lived (detailed data on income were not available from the 2010 census). Participants did not differ from non-participants in terms of housing value (t = 1.28, p = 0.20) and number of households (t = 1.46, p = 0.14). Participants lived in areas with significantly higher median family income than non-participants (t = 2.18, p = 0.03), but the effect size was small (d = 0.12). There was also a significantly higher number of households receiving public assistance in the areas where non-participants lived (t = 4.44, p < 0.001) and again the effect was small (d = 0.24). It should be noted that no multiple-comparison correction was used, allowing for a less conservative view of differences between groups.

Measures

Externalizing Behaviors

The Disruptive Behavior Disorder scale (DBD; Pelham et al. 1992) was used to measure parent reported externalizing disorder symptoms. Previous studies provide support for the reliability and validity of the DBD (Pelham et al. 2005). Further, research suggests that parent report of child disruptive behavior disorders may be more valid than child report (Loeber et al. 1989; Smith 2007). The DBD contains 45 items closely aligned with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association 2000) criteria for ODD, CD, and the inattention and hyperactive/impulsive dimensions of ADHD. Parents rate their children on a 4-point Likert-like scale, with response options of 0 “Not at all” to 3 “Very much.” Scores for inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, ODD, and CD scales were obtained by calculating the mean of the summed responses to each scale. Scales were created allowing for up to 10 % missing data by respondent. Oppositional defiant disorder was measured by eight items and provided adequate reliability in this sample (α = 0.86). Conduct disorder was measured by 15 items and provided adequate reliability (α = 0.71). The inattention and hyperactivity dimensions of ADHD were measured by nine items each and provided adequate reliability (α = 0.90 and α = 0.85 respectively).

Internalizing Behaviors

The Revised Child Anxiety and Depression Scale (RCADS; Chorpita et al. 2000) is a 47-item measure originally designed to assess child self-report symptoms of the DSM-IV internalizing disorders of MDD, SP, SAD, PD, GAD, and OCD. The parent version used in this study is identical to the child version except that the items have been altered so as to apply to the parent’s perspective (Ebesutani et al. 2011a; Muris et al. 2003). The parent version of the RCADS has demonstrated good convergent and discriminant validity and reliability (Ebesutani et al. 2011a). Items are assessed using a 4-point Likert-like scale, with response options of 1 “Never” to 4 “Always.” Scores for each scale were obtained by calculating the mean of the summed responses to each scale. Scales were created allowing for up to 10 % missing data by respondent. Major depressive disorder was measured by 10 items and provided adequate reliability in this sample (α = 0.75). Social phobia was measured by nine items and provided adequate reliability (α = 0.82). Panic disorder was measured by nine items also and provided marginal reliability (α = 0.69). Separation anxiety disorder was measured by seven items and provided marginal reliability (α = 0.68). Generalized anxiety disorder was measured by six items and provided adequate reliability (α = 0.81). Obsessive-compulsive disorder was measured by six items and provided marginal reliability (α = 0.63).

Negative Affect

The Child and Adolescent Dispositions Scale (CADS; Lahey et al. 2008a) is a 57-item measure designed to assess the dispositions of prosociality, negative emotionality, and daring. Previous studies provide support for the reliability, internal validity, and external validity of caretaker ratings of the CADS scales (Lahey et al. 2008a). Parents rate how often children’s behavior occurred over the last 12 months using a 4-point Likert-like scale with response options 1 “Not at all” to 4 “Very much.” Scores for each scale were obtained by calculating the mean of the summed responses to each scale. Only the negative emotionality scale was used in this study. Negative emotionality was measured by seven items and provided adequate reliability in this sample (α = 0.78).

The PANAS (Watson et al. 1988) was used to measure positive and negative affect. The PANAS contains 20 one-word adjective items reflecting relatively pure positive and negative emotions. Research supports the use of parent rated measures of positive and negative affect (Ebesutani et al. 2011b). Parents rate the degree children feel each emotion using a 5-point Likert-like scale with response options 1 “Very slightly or not at all” to 5 “Extremely.” Scores for each scale were obtained by calculating the mean of the summed responses to each scale. Scales were created allowing for up to 10 % missing data by respondent. Only the negative affect scale was used in this study. Negative affect was measured by 10 items and provided adequate reliability (α = 0.86).

Data Analysis

Twin analyses take into account that identical or monozygotic (MZ) twins share 100 % of their genes, and fraternal or dizygotic (DZ) twins share approximately 50 % of their segregating alleles. This allows for an estimation of additive genetic (A), shared environmental (C), and nonshared environmental (E) influences on traits and behaviors. First, twin intraclass correlations were calculated and compared across zygosity for all measured variables. Intraclass correlations suggest genetic influences when MZ correlations are higher than DZ correlations, shared environmental influences when MZ correlations are less than twice the magnitude of DZ correlations, and nonshared environmental influences (and measurement error) when MZ correlations are less than 1.00. Next, univariate biometrical models were used to estimate A, C, and E for each measured variable.

Finally, a multifactorial independent pathway model (Neale and Cardon 1992) was used to examine the genetic and environmental influences underlying the covariation of negative affect, internalizing disorder symptoms, and externalizing disorder symptoms (see Fig. 1). As a first step within the larger model, three factors representing the three constructs were created. The use of factors allows for a test of construct validity, as well as provides random-error-free estimates for further analyses (see Gayan and Olson 2003). Therefore, the Negative Affect factor was comprised of the negative emotionality scale from the CADS and the negative affect scale from the PANAS. The Internalizing factor was comprised of the SP, SAD, PD, GAD, OCD, and MDD scales from the RCADS. The Externalizing factor was comprised of the CD, ODD, inattention, and hyperactivity/impulsivity scales of the DBD.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Independent pathway model representing Negative Affect, Internalizing, and Externalizing as separate factors. Standardized path coefficients and residuals (and 95 % confidence intervals) are provided. A = additive genetic influences; C = shared environmental influences; E = nonshared environmental influences; NA = Negative Affect scale from the PANAS; NEG = Negative Emotionality scale from the CADS; SP = social phobia; SAD = separation anxiety disorder; PD = panic disorder; GAD = generalized anxiety disorder; OCD = obsessive-compulsive disorder; MDD = major depressive disorder; CD = conduct disorder; ODD = oppositional defiant disorder; ATT = attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder inattention; HYP = attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder hyperactivity/impulsivity

An independent pathway model was simultaneously applied to the factors, allowing for four sets of biometric factors to be estimated. The first set (A1, C1, and E1) is particularly relevant for the current hypotheses, as it represents the genetic and environmental influences that contribute to the covariance among the three factors (Negative Affect, Internalizing and Externalizing). The other three sets of biometric factors represent the genetic and environmental influences that are unique to each factor after accounting for the first set of biometric factors.

Results

Means, standard deviations, and ranges for all raw variables are presented in Table 1. The distributions of measured variables were examined for normality and found to be positively skewed. Therefore, data were transformed using an inverse transformation as recommended by Tabachnick and Fidell (2007). Because age and gender effects can bias twin analyses, and age and gender differences were not a focus of the current hypotheses, data were corrected by residualizing on age, age-squared, gender, and an age-by-gender interaction prior to analyses (McGue and Bouchard 1984). However, it should be noted that non-transformed and non-residualized data produced similar results. Pearson correlations between variables (post transforming and residualizing) are presented in Table 2, and intraclass correlations by zygosity are presented in Table 3. Intraclass correlations were consistently higher among MZ twins, suggesting genetic influences. Given that the MZ correlations were not twice the magnitude of DZ correlations and less than 1.00, there was also evidence for shared and nonshared environmental influences.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for negative affect, internalizing and externalizing scales
Table 2 Pearson correlations among negative affect, internalizing and externalizing scales
Table 3 Intraclass correlations by zygosity and univariate estimates for measured variables [with 95 % confidence intervals]

Biometric models were fit to transformed scores using Mx; 95 % confidence intervals were used to test for significance (Neale et al. 2003). Data were missing for some scale scores for between six and 56 participants due to items left unanswered by participants, and this was handled by using the Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) approach. Univarite genetic (or heritability, h2), shared environmental (c2), and nonshared environmental (e2) estimates, presented in Table 3, were consistent with intraclass correlations. Each variable was found to have significant genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental influences. The only exception was CD, which had a nonsignificant estimate of shared environmental effects. Univariate estimates of each factor are presented in Table 4. Results for Negative Affect, Internalizing, and Externalizing factors suggested moderate genetic and shared environmental effects. For all factors, nonshared environmental effects were low.

Table 4 Univariate estimates for negative affect, internalizing, and externalizing factors [with 95 % confidence intervals]

The main purpose of the current study was to examine the genetic and environmental influences underlying the covariation of negative affect, internalizing disorder symptoms, and externalizing disorder symptoms. This was done using a multifactorial independent pathway model, shown in Fig. 1. The model fit statistics were as follows: −2 times log-likelihood = 37,931.08, df = 16159; Akaike’s Information Criterion = 5,613.08; Bayesian Information Criterion = −33,800.69. Factor loadings of the indicators on the Negative Affect, Internalizing and Externalizing factors (presented in Table 2) were moderate to high, ranging from 0.52 to 0.99 for the Negative Affect factor, 0.62 to 0.78 for the Internalizing factor, and 0.66 to 0.85 for the Externalizing factor, suggesting that the measured variables formed cohesive factors. The first set of biometric factors estimated (A1, C1, and E1; see Fig. 1) in the independent pathway model represents the general genetic and environmental influences that contribute to the covariance among the Negative Affect, Internalizing and Externalizing factors. The general genetic biometric factor (A1) suggested significant (i.e., confidence interval did not include zero) shared genetic effects underlying the Negative Affect, Internalizing, and Externalizing factors (pathways 0.69, 0.34, and 0.30). The general shared environmental biometric factor (C1) suggested significant shared environmental effects underlying the three factors as well (pathways 0.42, 0.63, and 0.51). Finally, the general nonshared environmental biometric factor (E1) also suggested common nonshared environmental factors among three factors (pathways 0.37, 0.24, and 0.09). Results support the hypothesis that the Negative Affect, Internalizing, and Externalizing factors have common genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental effects. However, the results suggest higher than expected shared environmental influences on the covariation among factors.

The second set of biometric factors (A2, C2, and E2) represent the genetic and environmental influences unique to the Negative Affect factor beyond the variance accounted for by the general biometric factors (A1, C1, and E1). As can be seen in Fig. 1, the only significant pathway in the second set of biometric factors was C2 suggesting independent shared environmental effects accounting for 21 % of variance (0.462) in Negative Affect. The third set of biometric factors (A3, C3, and E3) represent the genetic and environmental influences unique to the Internalizing factor. Pathway A3 suggests significant independent genetic influences on the Internalizing factor, accounting for 28 % of variance (0.532). The fourth set of biometric factors (A4, C4, and E4) represent the genetic and environmental influences unique to the Externalizing factor. Pathways A4 and C4 were both significant, suggesting independent genetic effects accounting for 36 % of the variance (0.602) and independent shared environmental effects accounting for 28 % of the variance (0.532) in the Externalizing factor.

Discussion

Understanding the genetic and environmental influences underlying the co-occurrence of disorders may help us to understand vulnerability to psychopathology and may also shed light on developmental trajectories of psychopathology. Previous research clearly shows that internalizing and externalizing disorders covary (Angold et al. 1999; Cosgrove et al. 2011; Kessler et al. 2005; Krueger 1999; Krueger and Markon 2006; Krueger et al. 1998; O’Connor et al. 1998; Pesenti-Gritti et al. 2008; Rhee et al. 2007), and many have suggested that negative affect may partially account for the covariance between internalizing and externalizing disorders (Lilienfeld 2003; Rhee et al. 2007; Tackett et al. 2011). The present study is the first to provide an examination of negative affect in relation to a wide spectrum of childhood internalizing and externalizing problems using a biometric model. This study extends prior findings of more narrowly focused associations by using a factor approach including multiple disorders and provides a comprehensive perspective of these relations. Results of the current study suggest that negative affect shares genetic and environmental influences with both internalizing and externalizing disorders in childhood. These common influences may partially explain their co-occurrence.

Several previous studies have examined the bivariate relationships between negative affect and either internalizing or externalizing behaviors (Gjone and Stevenson 1997b; Rhee et al. 2007; Schmitz et al. 1999). Though results of these studies are not entirely consistent, jointly they demonstrate that genetic and/or shared environmental influences contribute to the covariation between negative affect and psychopathology. Results of the current study are consistent with the literature to the extent that both genetic and shared environmental influences were found to be significant contributors to the covariation between the Negative Affect, Internalizing, and Externalizing factors. The results of a study by Tackett et al. (2011) are perhaps more comparable to this study, as they include negative emotionality, an internalizing disorder, and an externalizing disorder in the same model. The results of the Tackett et al. (2011) study suggest significant genetic (for males), shared environmental, and nonshared environmental influences contributing to the covariation among negative emotionality and disorder dimensions, which is similar to the pattern of results of the current study (although the current study likely lacked power to decisively examine gender differences). Results of the current study are somewhat inconsistent with the literature in that the estimates of non-shared environmental influences are lower than those found in prior work (Rhee et al. 2007; Tackett et al. 2011). This is likely due to the use of a factor approach in the current study. An advantage of using a factor independent pathway model is that the measurement error is not included in the non-shared environmental estimates, but rather is contained in the residual variance of each manifest variable. Thus, the effects of measurement error in the biometric results are decreased, providing a better estimate of true non-shared environmental effects, which may have been overestimated in previous work.

Results of the present study were generally in line with the hypotheses, although the results suggest higher than expected shared environmental influences on the covariation between the Negative Affect, Internalizing, and Externalizing factors. As previously mentioned, the results of prior studies are mixed in terms of the significance and magnitude of shared environmental effects on childhood psychopathology. A meta-analysis by Burt (2009) offers some potential explanations. Burt’s findings suggested that different assessment methods and informants may lead to differences in estimates of genetic and environmental influences. Studies that rely on parent report rather than child report and studies that use questionnaires instead of diagnostic interviews tended to have higher shared environment estimates (Burt 2009). Considering that the present study relied on questionnaires completed by parents it is possible that estimates of shared environmental influence in this study are inflated. Another possibility is that the magnitude of shared environmental influence is, in part, determined by the type of environment. Studies have shown higher shared environment estimates in high-risk, disadvantaged environments (Cleveland 2003; Tuvblad et al. 2005). Although the present study did not specifically target families living in disadvantaged environments, the sample did include families with wide ranging incomes.

Prior studies have provided support for a ‘failure model’ in which the occurrence of an externalizing disorder (namely conduct disorder) leads to the development of an internalizing disorder, such as depression (Capaldi 1991, 1992). Results from the current study are consistent with this model such that common genetic and environmental influences, as well as negative affect, may contribute to the development of both disorders. The data from the current study were collected at one time point and, thus, could not provide information about the temporal relationship between externalizing and internalizing disorders. However, it would be useful for future studies with multiple waves of data on twins to examine the failure model more explicitly and test for genetic and environmental influences on the associations between negative affect, internalizing, and externalizing over time.

Weiss et al. (1998) proposed a model for characterizing common and specific features of childhood psychopathology. In this model, “common features” are those that distinguish psychopathology from normality, “broadband-specific features” are those that differentiate internalizing disorders from externalizing disorders, and “narrowband-specific features” are those that differentiate specific disorders from one another. From this framework, the present study suggests that negative affect is a “common feature,” partially accounting for the covariation between childhood internalizing and externalizing disorders (see also Lilienfeld 2003). Future research should seek to discover other “common features” contributing to this covariation, which may include other dispositions, temperament, or personality factors. Also, in the present study the Internalizing factor had unique genetic influences and the Externalizing factor had unique genetic and shared environmental influences (after accounting for the genetic and environmental influences contributing to the covariance among the Negative Affect, Internalizing and Externalizing factors). These unique influences can be conceptualized as “broadband-specific features,” which serve to differentiate internalizing and externalizing disorders. Future research should aim to identify specifically what these unique influences are, especially for the shared environmental influences on the Externalizing factor, as identifying these influences may lead to the development of interventions.

It should be noted that although dispositions are often thought to underlie or predispose to psychopathology, the relationship is not necessarily unidirectional. Multiple models have been proposed to explain the relationship between dispositions and psychopathology (Tackett 2006). The “vulnerability” model suggests that dispositions increase the risk for developing a particular disorder. The “complication” model suggests that the development of a disorder changes an individual’s disposition. The “exacerbation” model suggests that dispositional traits may influence the course, severity, or presentation of a disorder. Finally, the “spectrum” model suggests that dispositions and psychopathology are on a dimensional continuum. These models are not necessarily mutually exclusive (Tackett 2006; Lonigan et al. 2011), and the results of the current study fit with both the “vulnerability” model and the “spectrum” model. However, the present study was not designed or intended to evaluate these models. It is important for future research to utilize etiologically informative, longitudinal designs to gain a better understanding of the developmental course of psychopathology.

The present study included ODD as an indicator of the Externalizing factor and did not examine the unique relationship between ODD and negative affect. However, recent research suggests that the symptoms of ODD can be divided into three dimensions, including a dimension that may have construct overlap with negative affect (Burke et al. 2010; Stringaris and Goodman 2009a, b). Specifically, Stringaris and Goodman found that ODD was comprised of Irritable, Headstrong, and Hurtful dimensions. The Irritable dimension, thought to overlap with negative affect, was predictive of internalizing disorders (Stringaris and Goodman 2009b). Given that ODD loaded the highest on the Externalizing factor in this study, it is possible that the relationship between the Externalizing factor and the Negative Affect factor was driven by ODD (perhaps via the Irritable dimension). Although the current work focused on common variance among externalizing disorders, future work should continue to examine personality traits like Negative Affect in relation to individual externalizing disorders and their dimensional components.

As with all studies, there are important limitations that should be considered when interpreting these results. First, only parent reported data were available for the current study. Given the young age of many of the twins in this study, it is likely that parents would provide a more accurate and valid rating of negative affect and psychopathological symptoms than the twins could provide. However, as previously mentioned, the use of parent report tends to increase estimates of shared environmental influences (Burt 2009; Hewitt et al. 1992). It is possible that examining teacher report or child report could result in differences in estimates of genetic and environmental influences, and thus should be investigated in future research. Second, although diagnostic interviews are typically considered the “gold standard” for the assessment of psychopathology, the present study relied on questionnaires to assess symptoms. Given that the factor structures of internalizing and externalizing disorders in the present study were similar to studies that have used interviews (Krueger et al. 1998), it suggests that the method of assessment had little effect on the results. Finally, some previous studies that have examined the effects of gender have found differences between girls and boys in terms of the genetic and environmental influences contributing to the relationship between negative affect, internalizing disorders, and externalizing disorders (Rhee et al. 2007; Tackett et al. 2011). However, analyses examining these differences in a decisive way would likely have been underpowered in the present sample, given that the investigation that was able to investigate gender differences adequately (Tackett et al. 2011) had over twice the sample size of the current study. Future research should aim to further investigate gender differences, as well as differences across other characteristics such as socioeconomic status and age. It is possible that there may be developmental changes in the relative contribution of etiological influences. For example, shared environmental influences may contribute to the covariation of negative affect, internalizing, and externalizing behaviors more at an early age, while genetic influences on the covariation may increase throughout development.

Results of the present study suggest that a child’s level of negative affect is intertwined with their risk for various disorders, including internalizing disorders like separation anxiety and depression, as well as externalizing disorders like conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder. Understanding that negative affect is at least one contributor to the covariation among these disorders may highlight avenues for early risk assessment, intervention, and perhaps prevention. Future research should determine the extent to which negative affect in children is malleable, and if reduced negative affect decreases the risk for developing comorbid disorders. Also, it would be beneficial to identify protective factors that might prevent children high in negative affect from developing psychopathology. It is likely that negative affect interacts with other dispositions or temperament traits, and those different combinations of traits serve as different vulnerabilities. The use of prospective research designs examining temperament traits as well as environmental factors would aid in understanding the developmental course of comorbid psychopathology in children. Finally, results of the present study suggest common genetic and environmental influences underlying negative affect, internalizing and externalizing disorders. Future research should seek to identify these underlying specific genes as well as specific environmental factors, and attempt to discover how they interact.