Introduction

Most parasites occur on a restricted number of hosts and show some evidence of specificity. Host specific parasites generally have a major host and a few less frequently used hosts (Poulin 1992; Tripet et al. 2002a); even generalists commonly show a preference for some species above others (Tripet and Richner 1997; Soler et al. 1999; Johnson et al. 2002). Such a drift towards greater host specificity is a common feature of many parasitic groups. Selection apparently tends to favour specialization of parasites to their local environment, i.e. their hosts (Combes 1991; Thompson 1994; Kawecki 1998; de Meeûs et al. 1998). In particular, host specialization appears to be promoted by host-dependent fitness trade-offs dependent on the relative availability and predictability of hosts (Jaenike 1990; Thompson 1994; Combes 1995, 1997; Tripet and Richner 1997; Norton and De Lange 1999; McCoy et al. 2001). A parasite should specialize if the advantages of using one single host species in a profitable manner outweigh the benefits from interacting less profitably with several infrequent host species (Jaenike 1990; Norton and Carpenter 1998; McCoy et al. 2001). In other words, ephemeral or scarce hosts should promote parasite generalization (Tripet et al. 2002a), while parasites should specialize to their specific environmental conditions when hosts are abundant and predictable (Soler et al. 1999; Tripet et al. 2002b).

Host-parasite interactions, and thus host specificity, may be envisioned as taking place simultaneously at several different “host” levels. This is probably why such interactions are especially difficult to disentangle. In Spinturnicidae (ectoparasitic mites infesting colonial bats, Acari, Mesosotigmata), we have already investigated these interactions at two different host levels: individual (cost of parasitism: Giorgi et al. 2001) and population (Christe et al. 2000, 2003). In this study, we address host-parasite interactions at the community level and investigate the relative importance of different causal mechanisms underlying host specificity (Combes 1991; Poulin 1992; Ward et al. 1998; Timms and Read 1999) within this host-parasite system.

Parasite specificity may be mediated by three main mechanisms. First, the dispersal capacity of parasite (first barrier to specificity) depends on the number of host species it can physically encounter during its life. This is linked to the parasite’s intrinsic dispersal capacity (mobility and survival off the host) and upon the demographic and spatial properties of host populations (e.g. their mobility and geographic range; Gandon et al. 1996). In this respect, parasites may face difficulties invading several host taxa. Second, if parasites can disperse to another species, host preference (second barrier) will evolve if parasite fitness varies among hosts. If both conditions are fulfilled, the benefits to reduce host error outweigh the costs of developing host preference (Jaenike 1990; McCoy et al. 2001). Finally, when parasites disperse, they may have lost the ability to successfully transmit and establish a population on a novel host (last barrier). In this respect, highly specific parasites are expected to exhibit a higher reproductive success or survival on traditional, native host species than on less closely related ones (Norton and Carpenter 1998; Timms and Read 1999; Tompkins and Clayton 1999).

Numerous studies of host specificity have been performed on a variety of taxa from bacteria through parasitoids and nematodes to phytophagous and hematophagous insects. However, experiments that disentangle the relative importance of adaptive specialization versus dispersal ability have rarely been performed (Timms and Read 1999 but see de Meeûs et al. 1990, 1995; Becnel and Andreadis 1998; Kosoy et al. 2000; Thresher et al. 2000). In this study, we used a system with two parasite species belonging to the same genus (Spinturnix myoti, S. andegavinus) subjected to two related bat host species (Myotis myotis, M. daubentoni). With these two host-parasite pairs (M. myotis/S. myoti and M. daubentoni/S. andegavinus), we conducted two kinds of experiments: host preference experiments (dual host choice) and fitness experiments (infection and cross-infection). If adaptive specialization is responsible for host specificity, parasites should either prefer, or have a higher fitness on, traditional, native host species than on substitute ones. Thus, short-term and cross-infection experiments were performed to determine, respectively, the existence of host preference and the fitness of mites on traditional versus novel host species. If neither host preference nor parasite fitness differ between traditional and novel hosts, their host specificity should be governed by limited dispersal capacity and fixed long-term cospeciation events rather than by adaptive specialization. In this case, parasites should be capable of colonizing multiple hosts once the dispersal barrier is removed.

Materials and methods

Study species

This study was carried out during the breeding seasons of 1999 and 2000. Greater mouse-eared bats [Myotis myotis (Borkhausen, 1797)] were mist-netted at the entrance of two nursery colonies (church attics) in the upper Rhone Valley (Valais, Switzerland). Daubenton’s bats [Myotis daubentoni (Leisler in Kuhl, 1819)] were mist-netted during their foraging flights above the Chamberonne River (Vaud, Switzerland). At capture, sex, age (juvenile, subadult, adult) and reproductive status (non-reproductive, pregnant, lactating) of bats were noted. Pregnancy and lactation were identified through abdominal palpation or gentle milk extraction, respectively. Weight and forearm length were measured to the nearest 0.1 g and 0.1 mm, respectively. For ethical reasons, only subadult, non-reproductive females were taken and kept in captivity for laboratory experiments, under official authorization (Nature Conservation and Veterinary Offices of the cantons of Valais and Vaud).

Spinturnicidae mites [Spinturnix myoti Kolenati, 1856 and Spinturnix andegavinus (Kolenati, 1857); Acari, Mesostigmata, Spinturnicidae] are mobile ectoparasites, completing their entire lifecycle on bat wings and tail membranes (Rudnick 1960). The egg, pre-larval and first nymphal stages take place in the genital tract of the female mite. Pregnant females later give birth to male or female deuteronymphs, which already have an adult appearance (Evans 1968). All developmental stages are hematophagous, feeding on blood and lymph. As demonstrated earlier with S. myoti infesting M. myotis, these mites greatly affect time and energy budgets of their hosts: bat anti-parasite grooming activity as well as overall metabolism increase drastically with parasite load (Giorgi et al. 2001).

Host specificity in free-ranging bats

The traditional hosts of S. myoti across its range are M. myotis and M. blythii (Deunff 1977; Arlettaz, unpublished data). In Mediterranean populations, mouse-eared bats are typically cave-dwellers. They roost communally with M. blythii as well as with other cavernicolous bat species such as M. emarginatus, Miniopterus schreibersi and various Rhinolophus species (Estrada-Pena and Serra-Cobo 1991). All of these harbour different parasite species (Deunff 1977; Deunff and Beaucournu 1981). S. myoti may consequently occasionally encounter other potential host species. In contrast, S. andegavinus is specific to M. daubentoni, which forms monospecific nursery colonies. M. daubentoni is occasionally found in mixed roosts with Nyctalus noctula (which harbour Spinturnix acuminata; Deunff et al. 1997) and rarely with M. myotis. Thus, there are few opportunities for S. andegavinus encountering other bat species. Host preference may occur in wild populations because neither S. myoti nor S. andegavinus have been collected on the other bat species being studied here (Christe et al. 2000, 2003 and parasite phylogeny in progress).

Methods for handling, removing and counting parasites on bats

Mites used in these experiments were gently collected in the lab with soft forceps from the same populations as bats (S. myoti from the M. myotis populations and S. andegavinus from the M. daubentoni population). S. myoti and S. andegavinus were always collected separately in order to prevent an interspecific parasite pool. A morphological inspection of the mites (according to Deunff 1977) confirmed that the mites collected from different host species populations belonged to distinct parasite species. Length and width of dorsal idiosoma were measured on males and females: body width was taken as the distance across the widest part of the idiosoma, while body length was measured from the mid-point of the anterior body margin to the mid-point of the posterior body margin, thus excluding the gnathosoma (Rudnick 1960). All measurements were taken using Scion image software from a binocular microscope equipped with a camera. Males and females of S. andegavinus were each approximately 12% smaller than S. myoti (MANOVAs on body length and width with parasite species as factor; females: Pillai trace=0.63, df=1, 33, P<0.001; males: Pillai trace=0.71, df=1, 41, P<0.001; Table 1). Before each experiment, collected parasites were put in Petri dishes for no more than 30 min because neither S. myoti nor S. andegavinus can survive off the host for more than 3 h at ambient temperature (Fig. 1).

Table 1 Morphometric measurements of the species of parasite (Spinturnix myoti and S. andegavinus) and host (Myotis myotis and M. daubentoni) involved in experiments. All values are mean±SE. Numbers in parentheses indicate sample sizes
Fig. 1
figure 1

Survival of ectoparasitic mites (n=40 per curve) removed from their respective native host species and maintained separately, according to their original host species, in a shallow plastic cup at ambient temperature and humidity. Closed and open points are, respectively, data from Spinturnix andegavinus and S. myoti

Mites live exclusively on bat wings and tail membranes and have never been found in or on the fur (Christe et al. 2003). Because wings and tail membranes of bats are entirely bare, parasites have no place to hide and visual inspection produced reliable counts of parasite infestation. The repeatability of parasite counts during the same capture was 0.99 (n=52, P<0.001; Christe et al. 2003). In addition, because 20 s maximum are necessary to count parasites on each bat, this method of counting was absolutely non-invasive. Similarly, because parasites were clearly visible on wings and tail membranes, collecting parasites with soft forceps prior to the experiment guaranteed that bats were entirely deparasitized.

Host choice by parasites

In order to test for the patterns of choice of the two parasite species, dual host choice experiments were designed as follows. A pair of deparasitized bats, consisting of one M. myotis and one M. daubentoni (any given individual was involved only twice in the experiments, once with each parasite species), was exposed to S. myoti (n=34 trials) or S. andegavinus (n=31 trials). For each replicate, 20 parasites [13 males or unsexed deuteronymphs (both categories being visually nearly indiscernible) plus 7 females] were deposited into a small Petri dish stuck on the centre of a small wooden box (11×8×2.7 cm). The experiment began only when every parasite was immobile and waiting for a host either in, or at the apex of, the Petri dish. At this time, the pair of bats was placed into the box, which was small enough to force close bat body contact and consequently direct parasite transfer from the Petri dish to the bats and from bat to bat. After 3 h, the number of parasites present on each bat was counted. In a subsequent run, the same pair of bats was exposed to the other parasite species in the same way. After trial runs, bats were released into their colony of origin or re-allocated to fitness experiments (see below). For analyses, the percentage of mites on a given host species was calculated from the final number of parasites found instead of the initial 20 mites, because in some trials a few additional parasites appeared (birth of deutonymphs) or disappeared (mean number±SE of parasites lost per run: S. myoti 0.44±0.08; S. andegavinus 0.61±0.11).

Parasite fitness experiments

Forty subadult female bats (20 M. myotis and 20 M. daubentoni) were captured as described above and kept separately in ca. 8 m3 semi-outdoor aviaries with ambient temperature and photoperiod for 5 days to acclimatize to captivity. Bats were fed ad libitum a mixed diet consisting of crickets (Acheta sp.) and mealworms (Tenebrio molitor).

After these 5 initial days, two replicates of four groups (two groups of M. myotis and two groups of M. daubentoni) of five bats each were formed and assigned to different aviaries (day 0 of experiment; Table 2). In each aviary, only one roost was provided to constrain the bats to rest together, allowing complete horizontal parasite transmission among bats as in nursery colonies in the wild. All possible combinations of bat infestations were created, including patterns of infection for each host-parasite species pair (i.e. parasite on its normal host) and cross-infection (Table 2). Since Jaenike (1996), Kristoffersen et al. (2001) and Tyler et al. (2001) have shown that density-dependent intraspecific competition among individual parasites may affect their fitness, initial intensity of infection was manipulated to reflect the mean number of parasites observed in the populations under study. In M. daubentoni, mite burdens on subadult females were (average±SE) 5.5±0.5 S. andegavinus mites (n=150 bats from 1999 to 2002; Giorgi et al., unpublished data) whereas M. myotis loads were 11.5±1.4 S. myoti mites (n=216 bats from 1998 to 1999; Christe et al. 2000). Consequently, we placed 15 S. myoti mites on each individual deparasitized bat (i.e. totalling 75 mites per group), versus seven S. andegavinus mites per bat (totalling thus 35 mites in S. andegavinus aviaries; Table 2). Once the experiment was launched, parasite numbers were counted daily on every bat for 10 days. In addition, the percentage of remaining mites (with respect to the initial deposited number at day 0) was calculated at the end of the 10-day experiment in order to obtain demographic growth functions. Parasites not recovered from bats were assumed to have died in between, since neither S. myoti nor S. andegavinus can survive off the host for more than 3 hours at ambient temperature (Fig. 1). Demographic growth functions were averaged over the two replicate trials (except in Fig. 3). A 100% growth function represents a constant mite population while a growth function, respectively, lower or higher than 100% represents a decreasing or increasing mite population.

Table 2 Partitioning of bats and parasites among the four semi-outdoor aviaries (each table cell represents a distinct enclosure) for the 10-day fitness experiment. Species and number of bats and parasites are given. At day 0 (onset of experiment), either 15 (S. myoti) or seven (S. andegavinus) mites were deposited onto each individual bat within a monospecific pool of five bats. Myotis is the bat genus, Spinturnix the parasite genus

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using S-PLUS 2000 (MathSoft Inc 1988–1999, Seattle, Wash., USA). Prior to running statistical analyses, percentages were square-root arcsine transformed. Every variable was then tested for normality (Kolmogorov-Smirnov one-sample test) and heteroscedasticity (one-way ANOVA); no variable deviated from these two assumptions. Because there was no significant year effect as regards host preference between the datasets of 1999 and 2000, data from both years were pooled together. As regards host choice experiments, paired t-tests were performed on the (transformed) percentage of mites on both host species. To compare mites’ fitness, repeated-measures ANOVAs were applied on number of parasites recorded daily (repeated measure) over the duration of the experiments. Host species and replicate were assigned as between-factor terms, whilst time was used as the within-factor term. To compensate for a possible lack of compound symmetry in repeated measures ANOVAs, Huynh-Feldt adjustments of the degrees of freedom in the within-factor (time) analyses were accounted for (Gurevitch and Chester 1986; Glantz and Slinker 1991; Talan et al. 1996; Zar 1999). The adjusted degrees of freedom are directly reported in the text. All values reported are means±SE and all P-values are two-tailed.

Results

Host choice by parasites

S. myoti showed a highly significant preference for its own host, M. myotis (paired t-test: t=11.68, df=33, P<0.001; Fig. 2). Preference was evident in 33 trials out of 34, where at least 80% of mites had chosen M. myotis. In contrast, S. andegavinus showed no apparent host choice (paired t-test: t=0.52, df=30, P=0.604; Fig. 2): in about half the trials (15 trials out of 31), the percentage of mites on both hosts was similar, indicating a random distribution between hosts.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean percentage (+SE) number of parasites (S. myoti and S. andegavinus) counted on each individual bat of a mixed pair of hosts after running a 3-h dual host choice experiment. Twenty mites were placed in a small cup between the two bats at the onset of the experiment. Open bars percentage of mites on Myotis myotis; closed bars percentage of mites on M.daubentoni. Mm, Myotis myotis; Md,M. daubentoni. *** P<0.001

Parasite fitness experiments

Spinturnix myoti

The temporal pattern of fitness of S. myoti was explained by a single significant term, namely host species, whereas neither replicate nor time accounted significantly for the observed variation (repeated-measure ANOVA: r 2=0.44; host species: F 1,17=42.40, P<0.001; replicate: F 1,17=2.82, P=0.11; time: F 1,19=2.43, P=0.14). The mean demographic growth functions of S. myoti over the 10-day experimental period were 98% when the parasite was subjected to M. myotis, but 5% only when infesting the non-native host, M. daubentoni. In other words, the population of S. myoti on M. daubentoni had approached quasi-extinction at day 10 (Fig. 3). The demographic stability of S. myoti was noticeable on M. myotis; moreover, despite the fact that the recorded loss of individuals was not entirely compensated for by births, the population actually showed signs of reproduction on M. myotis (presence of unsclerified deuteronymphs).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Outcome of the 10-day infection and cross-infection experiments. Two demographic growth functions were obtained from each of the four possible experimental combinations (two parasite species×two host species). The four experimental groups consisted of a monospecific pool of five individual bats each, associated with a pre-determined number of parasites, and kept in separate aviaries (details about experimental design in Table 2)

Spinturnix andegavinus

In S. andegavinus, there was also a significant, albeit smaller effect of host species on fitness, whereas neither replicate nor time were significant (repeated ANOVA: r 2=0.44; host species: F 1,17=124.63, P<0.001; replicate: F 1,17=0.23, P=0.64; time: F 1,19=0.92, P=0.35). As above, the fitness of S. andegavinus on its native host species (M. daubentoni) was higher than on the substitute host (M. myotis): nevertheless the demographic decline over time was much less dramatic in the latter case than for S. myoti on M. daubentoni (Fig. 3). Over the 10-day period, the mean growth functions were 133%, a strong increase, on M. daubentoni and 57%, a marked decrease, on M. myotis.

Discussion

Host choice by parasites

In our experiments, the mite S. myoti exhibited a clear and immediate preference for its native bat host (M. myotis), while it tended to avoid the substitute host offered (M. daubentoni). In contrast, S. andegavinus showed no initial choice for any of the two host species, which were seemingly colonized at random. Thus, differences in strength of host preference were detected between the two parasite species. Similar divergent patterns of host preference in closely-related species have been observed in various parasite taxa (de Meeûs et al. 1990, 1995; Le Brun et al. 1992; Du Preez and Kok 1997; Du Preez et al. 1997; Thresher et al. 2000)

As described above, preference should evolve according to parasite fitness and dispersal ability. In mites, difference in host preference should occur because the strength of one or both of these conditions (parasite population dynamics or parasite dispersal capacity) varies across parasite species. The first condition is satisfied in both species: mites survived and reproduced significantly better on the native than on the alien, substitute host. However, mortality was much more pronounced in S. myoti on M. daubentoni than in S. andegavinus on M. myotis. The second condition is also basically satisfied as both parasite species have the opportunity to switch host due to occasional contacts with other bat species (in maternity colonies as well as in winter colonies; Estrada-Pena and Serra-Cobo 1991). However, the frequency of such contacts differs for the two parasite species. Across its range, S. myoti has two frequent hosts, M. myotis and M. blythii, which are sister species (Deunff 1977; Ruedi and Mayer 2001; Arlettaz, unpublished data). These hosts are known to sometimes be in contact with some others cavernicolous bat species. In contrast, S. andegavinus is rarely mixed with bat species. This lower range of potential hosts as well as a lower fitness cost of substitute host species would thus explain the absence of host selection of S. andegavinus, while a drastic decrease on substitute host as well as more frequent opportunities of host switching for S. myoti promote the evolution of host preference in this species.

Cues for host selection

Given the huge mortality of parasites when off their host, it appears that both Spinturnix species have evolved strong mechanisms preventing being dropped from the host and/or erroneous host transfer. Firstly, mites are difficult to remove from bat wings and tail membranes, to which they adhere strongly. Secondly, mites accidentally leaving their host towards an inanimate substrate quickly return to the host. Thirdly, parasites which have unexpectedly lost their host adopt, after a few seconds of searching, a host-waiting posture with widely open legs pointing up towards any passing obviously aerial bodies. It is striking to notice the rapidity of the reaction elicited in a vagrant parasite. This points to very simple cues enabling the parasite to discriminate between an inappropriate substrate and a potential host. As shown by Sonenshine (1993), Cox et al. (1998), Devine et al. (2000) and Osterkamp et al. (1999), mites can use a mixture of vibrations, shadowing, CO2 concentration and others chemical signals to recognize their hosts. In the absence of appropriate tests, however, the stimuli involved in host species recognition in Spinturnix mites remain unknown. We hypothesize that subtle differences in the chemical signature of the hosts must be exploited by S. myoti and S. andegavinus when host choice is operated.

Parasite fitness with respect to host species

In this study, mites survived and reproduced significantly better on native than on alien substitute hosts. Adaptation to particular hosts is not surprising for permanent parasites of vertebrates. It has been described previously in other parasites such as syringophilid mites and swiftlet lice (Kethley 1971; Tompkins and Clayton 1999), which spend their entire lifecycle on the same type of host. We have to note here that, similar to the study by Tompkins and Clayton (1999), we restricted our fitness comparison to a 10-day experiment. This was due to logistic and ethical reasons: maintenance of wild-captured bats in captivity for long periods is both difficult and questionable. Yet, since parasite demographic growth on the native bat host was approximately equivalent to, or higher than, 100%, we can reasonably consider our experimental conditions as representative of natural circumstances: in other words, a temporal prolongation of the experiments would probably not have altered the outcome of the tests.

As regards cross infections, it should be noticed that mortality was strikingly more pronounced for S. myoti on M. daubentoni than for S. andegavinus on M. myotis. For the former, there was a drop in parasite intensity from 15 to less than one mite per bat over 10 days. In contrast, S. andegavinus had a much higher level of survival on M. myotis. Differences in dispersal capacity leading to evolution of host preference (see above) may account for this huge difference. In addition, the fact that both parasite species showed few signs of reproduction (unsclerified mites that indicate freshly born individuals) on substitute hosts—whilst reproduction actually took place during infection experiments—support the hypothesis that mites are poorly adapted to substitute hosts.

Notwithstanding the quantitative difference in mortality when parasites were subjected to alien hosts, strong host-dependent fitness remains the most striking feature of our infection and cross-infection experiments. In our opinion, only long-term, fixed coevolutionary adaptations between the parasite and its host would lead to this pattern, revealing divergent evolutionary paths. In this context, the instantaneous preference shown by S. myoti might have been selected by host dependent fitness, which induces immediate, correct host choice via efficient host recognition cues. In other words, the benefits of reducing host selection error would tremendously outweigh the costs of developing new adaptations for a novel host (Jaenike 1990; McCoy et al. 2001).

Conclusion

In S. myoti, the causal mechanisms mediating host specificity appeared to be an intermediate dispersal capacity and species-specific adaptations of the parasite towards its host. In contrast, in S. andegavinus, despite a high specificity in the field, species-specific adaptations to the host have seemingly evolved to a lesser extent. We can conclude that current adaptive specializations maintain host specificity and may promote cospeciation events. The present study, carried out at the population and community levels, does not enable us to further infer conclusively about the selective pressures responsible for the potential coevolutionary adaptations within Spinturnicidae. Like in any evolutionary arms race, these adaptations probably proceed from reciprocal adjustments of parasite to host and host to parasite. In this respect, further investigations integrating micro-evolutionary (individual and population level; see Christe et al. 2000, 2003; Giorgi et al. 2001) and macro-evolutionary perspectives simultaneously (the present contribution and a cophylogenetic study of European bats and their parasites which is currently in progress) would provide decisive insights into the functional patterns of such host-parasite arms races.