Children with end-stage liver disease have been found to have cognitive deficits. For example, children with biliary atresia have deficits in their performance intelligence quotient (IQ) and spatial and sequential scores before liver transplantation [1]. Unfortunately, after liver transplantation many patients still have deficits in their performance IQ, learning and memory, and visuospatial function [2]. Many factors relate to posttransplantation cognitive deficits, such as surgery itself, the recovery process, and steroid and immunosuppressant treatment [3, 4].

In adults with liver disease, visuospatial function relates to clinical measures of liver function: serum albumin and serum ammonia levels [5, 6, 7]. Information regarding the long-term effects of cholestasis on cognitive performance is scarce, and the role of reducing the bilirubin level in the recovery of cognitive function remains largely unknown. In the present study, we examined the cognitive function in rats with obstructive jaundice using the Morris water maze, a standard test for assessing spatial learning and memory function in rodents [7].

The study was designed to address three specific areas: (1) the spatial performance of rats with obstructive jaundice; (2) the relation between spatial memory deficit and measures of liver disease severity; and (3) whether biliary drainage improves the spatial memory deficit caused by bile duct obstruction.

Materials and Methods

Animals

This experiment was performed under the Guidelines for Animal Experiments of Chang Gung Memorial Hospital and Chang Gung University. Male Sprague-Dawley rats weighing 250 to 300 g were used. Attempts were made to minimize the number of animals used. The animals were kept in individual cages and acclimated for ≥ 7 days before use. All animals were housed in a room maintained at 24°C with 12-hour light/dark cycles; they had free access to water and standard laboratory chow. Before each operation, animals were fasted overnight with free access to water.

Experimental Design

The experimental design is presented in Figure 1. All surgical procedures were performed under pentobarbital (Nembutal) (50 mg/kg) anesthesia with clean surgical techniques as previously described [8]. Briefly, preparation for obstructive jaundice was done by opening a midline incision in the abdomen and placing a Broviac 2.7F single-lumen CV catheter (Bard Access System, Salt Lake City, UT, USA) in the proximal common bile duct. After ligating the proximal and distal bile ducts, the common bile duct was divided. The distal end of the Broviac tube was sealed. For the 10 rats in the BDL group, the bile duct was ligated (BDL group) for 3 weeks. Three of these animals died during the experiment owing to a ruptured choledochocele. For the 10 rats in the TBD group, the bile duct was ligated for 2 weeks, then tube bilioduodenostomy (TBD group) was performed according to the method of Ryan et al. [9] with minor modification. After biliary obstruction for 2 weeks, the abdomen was reopened, and the sealed end of the tube was opened, inserted into the duodenum about 1.5 cm from the pylorus, and then tied in position with two 7-0 PDS purse-string sutures. Five TBD animals died during surgery or hours later, and the surviving rats were studied after another week. Finally, six rats underwent sham ligation of the bile duct (SHAM group) and served as the controls.

Fig. 1.
figure 1

Experimental design. n: number of rats; SHAM: sham operation; BDL: bile duct ligation; TBD: tube bilioduodenostomy.

From the 22nd to the 27th day after the first operation, all rats were tested in the Morris water maze. On the following day, after completion of the water maze task, blood was aspirated by cardiocentesis and assayed for total bilirubin, albumin, ammonia, and hemoglobin levels.

Morris Water Maze

Eighteen rats (SHAM, n = 6; BLD, n = 7; TBD, n = 5) were evaluated for spatial memory in the Morris water maze, as previously described [10]. The Morris water maze, a test of spatial learning and memory commonly used in rodents, is considered to be hippocampus-dependent [11]. The probe trial assesses spatial bias in the rat’s search pattern during retrieval [12]. A circular steel tank (180 cm diameter × 50 cm high) was filled with water (26 ± 16 cm) to a depth of 25 cm; the water was made opaque by adding 100 ml of evaporated milk, which prevented visualization of the platform. Room lights illuminated the pool, and visual clues around the room (window, cabinets, furniture) were kept at a constant location from day to day. Four points on the perimeter of the pool were designed—north (N), east (E), south (S), and west (W)—thereby dividing the pool into four quadrants (NW, NE, SE, SW).

A 10 × 10 cm Plexiglas platform, onto which the rat could escape, was positioned in the center of one of the quadrants 1 cm below the water surface. A video camera was set above the center of the pool and was connected to a video-traction system (Ethovision, Noldus, The Netherlands). On the first day, each rat was placed in the pool for 60 seconds without the platform present; this free swim enabled the rat to become habituated to the training environment (training trial). On days 2 to 5 the rats were subjected to 24 trials (six trials a day) to locate and escape onto the submerged platform (acquisition trial). The quadrant in which the platform was located remained constant for each rat, but the point of immersion into the pool varied between N, E, S, and W in a quasirandom order for the 24 trials so the rat was not able to predict the platform location from the point at which it was placed in the pool.

On mounting the platform, the rats were given a 30-second rest period, after which the next trial was started. If the rat did not find the platform in 120 seconds, it was manually placed on the platform for a 30-second rest. At the start of each trial, the rat was held facing the perimeter and dropped into the pool to ensure immersion. Latencies to escape onto the platform were recorded. On day 6, the platform was removed. The rat was allowed 60 seconds of free swimming. The search pattern was tested to assess memory for platform location (probe trial). The testing procedure for locating the hidden platform is considered a measure of spatial reference memory, and the probe trial is considered a measurement of memory retrieval and strength of spatial learning [12].

Blood Analysis

The hemoglobin level was measured in EDTA-mixed whole blood using an automated technique by Sysmex Ne-α (Sysmex, Osaka, Japan). Plasma was separated from heparinized blood to determine the albumin and total bilirubin levels using a Hitachi 7450 (Hitachi, Tokyo, Japan) and the ammonia level by a Beckman CX-9 (Beckman, Fullerton, CA, USA).

Statistical Analysis

Comparisons of the escape latencies during the 24 trials of place learning of the Morris water maze and the data of blood analysis were analyzed by one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the least significant difference (LSD) post hoc test and repeated measures. The swimming speed and the probe trial were analyzed with the nonparametric Kruskal-Wallis H test. Values were expressed as the mean ± SEM, and significance was defined as p < 0.05 for all tests.

Results

Morris Water Maze

Figure 2 shows the escape latency in the water maze as a function of days. Although all groups displayed improvements over the 4 days of training, the performance of this task among these groups was significantly different, with the maximum difference on the last testing day (F2,15 = 4.86, p < 0.05). Post hoc testing showed that there was no difference between the SHAM group and the TBD group over the four testing days. This finding demonstrated that obstructive jaundice (BDL group) impaired spatial learning of rats, and the early tube bilioduodenostomy correction in rats with jaundice ameliorated the spatial memory deficits. There was no significant difference in swimming speed among the three groups (p > 0.1). Additionally, in the probe trial, there was an increasing percentage of time in the target quadrant for all groups, but the differences did not reach statistical significance (p > 0.1).

Fig. 2.
figure 2

Latencies to escape in the Morris water maze (mean ± SEM) for the sham controls, jaundiced rats with BDL, and jaundiced rats corrected with TBD. There were significant differences among groups over the 4 days of the test, with maximum latency on the last day (F2,15 = 4.86; p < 0.05). Furthermore, the post hoc test revealed that, compared with the BDL group, the TBD group had differences on the first 2 days (p < 0.05), whereas the SHAM group showed significant differences over all 4 days (p < 0.05). §Statistical difference (p < 0.05) among three groups. *Statistical difference (p < 0.05) compared with the BDL group.

Blood sample Analysis The data from the blood analyses are shown in Table 1. The BDL group had significantly higher serum concentrations of ammonia and total bilirubin and a lower serum hemoglobin level than the SHAM or the TBL group (p < 0.05). The BDL and TBD groups both had lower serum albumin levels than the SHAM group (p < 0.05). In addition, there was no difference in serum bilirubin, ammonia, or hemoglobin concentration between the TBD and SHAM groups. These data suggest that biliary drainage improved the serum liver function test results (bilirubin and ammonia), but the serum albumin concentration in the TBD group was still lower than that of the SHAM group.

Table 1. Serum biochemistry in the experimental groups.

Discussion

The results of this study revealed that jaundiced rats had worse spatial memory and lower serum ammonia and hemoglobin levels than nonjaundiced rats. Our data also showed that early surgical correction in rats with obstructive jaundice might improve spatial memory deficits. Restoration of serum ammonia and hemoglobin levels is associated with the recovery of spatial deficits.

In the Morris water maze task, animals must learn and use spatial strategies to locate a hidden platform. In the present study, jaundiced rats had a poor performance regarding acquisition memory (days 2–5) compared with nonjaundiced rats. However, the spatial deficit was reversed by surgical correction of the biliary obstruction. The probe trial (day 6) is considered a measurement of memory retrieval and strength of spatial learning [12]. In this experiment, however, there was no significant difference in spatial retrieval between jaundiced and nonjaundiced rats.

Biliary atresia is the most common variant of infantile obstructive cholangiopathy, a major indication for liver transplantation in children. The present study clearly demonstrated that relief of jaundice resulted in better spatial memory performance in rodents. Although extrapolation of rodent experiments to human conditions is difficult, this study suggests that early surgical intervention for irreversible or progressive liver diseases before the development of obvious liver failure are beneficial for cognitive function.

Circulating neurotoxins play a role in cognitive deficits in liver failure patients [13]. In this study, the BDL group showed impaired spatial learning and memory that was related to the bilirubin level, nutritional state, and hemoglobin value. Liver function assessments were normalized after 7 days of biliary decompression. The concentrations of serum total bilirubin, ammonia, and hemoglobin recovered in the TBD group, accompanied by improved spatial performance. Because hypoalbuminemia persisted into the second week despite biliary decompression, our data do not indicate that the serum albumin level is related to spatial memory performance.

The hippocampus is critically involved in the acquisition and consolidation of memory for spatial learning in animals [14]. Ammonia exerts a deleterious effect on cerebral function [15]. Furthermore, excess ammonia is reported to inhibit excitatory neurotransmission and firing in hippocampal CA1 neurons [16]. Our data support the concept that a high ammonia concentration is detrimental to the brain. In addition, we provide new evidence that a high ammonia concentration plays a role in cognitive deficits in cholestatic rats. Anemia may impair cognitive function in both adults [17] and infants [18]. Similarly, perinatal iron deficiency may lead to decreased cytochrome c oxidase activity (an indicator of neuronal activity [19]) in selected brain regions involved in cognition [20]. The main targets of iron deficiency are hippocampal formation and prefrontal projection, both of which structures are implicated in the explicit memory circuit [21]. An increased hemoglobin concentration might deliver more oxygen and hence improve cognitive function [17, 22].

Conclusions

Cognitive deficits may be induced by obstructive jaundice before liver failure. Early biliary decompression might improve the cognitive deficits and associated anemia as well as the liver function tests. The findings of this study imply that biliary decompression has some value in reversing spatial memory impairment following obstructive jaundice.

Résumé.

On sait que certains enfants porteurs d’insuffisance hépatique terminale ont des déficits cognitifs. Le but de cette étude a été de déterminer si l’ictère choléstatique provoque des déficits spatiaux chez le rat et si ces déficits cognitifs peuvent être guéris par un drainage biliaire. Trois groupes de rats, traités différemment selon un tirage au sort, ont été étudiés: dans le premier groupe, la voie biliaire a été liée pendant trois semaines (groupe BDL); dans le deuxième groupe, la voie biliaire a été liée avec un cathéter Broviac CV inséré dans la voie biliaire proximale pendant deux semaines et ensuite une anastomose bilio duodénale a été réalisée sur tube (groupe TBD); dans le troisième groupe, une intervention factice a été pratiquée (groupe SHAM). Tous les rats ayant survécu ont été évalués pour l’apprentissage spatial et la mémoire, une fonction cognitive majeure chez le rat, par le test « tâches en labyrinthe d’eau » de Morris, trois semaines après la première opération. On a prélevé du sang par cardiocentèse pour doser la bilirubine totale, l’albumine, l’ammoniémie et l’hémoglobine le jour suivant le test de Morris. Pendant les quatre jours consécutifs du test de Morris, le temps que les rats ictériques (groupe BDL) mettaient pour s’échapper était plus long que pour le groupe SHAM (p < 0.05). Les rats ayant reçu une décompression biliaire pendant une semaine (groupe TBD) ont montré une amélioration du déficit spatial car ils ont nécessité moins de temps pour atteindre la plateforme pour s’échapper, une performance pas très différente des rats du groupe SHAM. Le groupe BDL avaient un taux d’ammoniémie et de bilirubinémie significativement plus élevé et un taux d’hémoglobine significativement plus bas en comparaison aux autres groupes. Après une décompression biliaire d’une semaine, les taux d’albumine sérique dans le groupe TBD n’est pas revenu aux niveaux de ceux du groupe SHAM. Les résultats de cette étude suggèrent que la cholestase à long terme provoque des déficits de mémoire spatiale chez le rat qui corrèlent avec le taux d’anémie et l’incidence d’encéphalopathie secondaire à l’hyperbilirubinémie. La décompression biliaire précoce de l’ictère par obstruction améliore les déficits de mémoire spatiale, possiblement en rapport avec la normalisation des taux de l’ammoniémie et de l’hémoglobinémie.

Resumen.

Se ha encontrado que los niños con enfermedad hepática terminal presentan déficits de cognición. El propósito del presente estudio fue investigar si la ictericia colestásica causa déficits espaciales y si tales déficits cognitivos pueden ser revertidos mediante drenaje biliar. Se hizo aleatorización de ratas en tres grupos: en el primero se ligó el conducto biliar por un periodo de 3 semanas (grupo LCB); en el segundo se practicó la ligadura en la región proximal con un catéter VC Broviac por 2 semanas y luego se realizó una bilioduodemostomía de tubo (grupo BDT); en el tercer grupo se realizó una operación “fantasma” (sham-operation) (grupo SHAM). Se procedió a evaluar la capacidad de aprendizaje espacial y la memoria en las ratas supervivientes, que es una función cognitiva mayor en la rata, mediante la prueba de agua y laberinto de Morris a las tres semanas después de la primera operación. Se tomaron muestras de sangre por cardiocentesis para determinación de bilirrubina total, albúmina, amoniaco y hemoglobina en el día siguiente a la prueba de agua y laberinto. En el curso de los cuatro días consecutivos de adquisición de la prueba de Morris, las ratas ictéricas (grupo LCB) exhibieron latencias de escape significativamente más prolongadas, en comparación con el grupo SHAM (p < 0.05). Las ratas que recibieron descompresión biliar por una semana (grupo BDT) mostraron mejoría del déficit espacial en cuanto a que requirieron menos tiempo para alcanzar la plataforma de escape y mostraron un rendimiento semejante al de grupo SHAM. El grupo LCB exhibió niveles séricos significativamente más elevados de amoniaco y de bilirrubina y niveles más bajos de hemoglobina en comparación con los otros dos grupos. Luego de la descompresión biliar por una semana, la concentración de albúmina en el grupo BDT todavía no alcanzó a retornar al nivel del grupo SHAM. Los resultados de este estudio sugieren que la colestasis a largo plazo resulta en déficits de memoria espacial en la rata, lo cual se correlaciona con anemia y encefalopatía de hiperbilirrubinemia. La descompresión precoz de una ictericia obstructiva mejora los déficits de memoria especial, por un mecanismo tal vez relacionado con la normalización de los niveles de amoniaco y de hemoglobina.