Introduction

Territorial species must trade off time allocated to territorial behavior with other activities that enhance their fitness (Ydenberg and Krebs 1987). Responding to predatory threats may, indeed, be especially costly, because by doing so individuals are unable to defend their territories. Thus, we expect the decisions about when and how to assess risk and respond to predators to be particularly important.

A commonly used metric to quantify risk is flight initiation distance (FID), which is minimum distance at which a predator approaches a prey before it escapes (Ydenberg and Dill 1986). FID is influenced by a variety of factors, such as predator lethality, group size, approach angle, prey size, and the habitat’s structural complexity (Blumstein et al. 2003; Cooper et al. 2002; Nunes et al. 2015). Optimal escape theory predicts that a prey should begin to flee its approaching predator when it reaches a point where the risk of predation is similar to the cost of escape (Ydenberg and Dill 1986); and reveals that flight initiation distance increases when predators pose a greater threat, and decreases when escape costs increase (Cooper et al. 2002).

Animals often adopt consistent defense strategies when faced with danger—they vary according to temperament or personality. For instance, bold individuals typically behave more actively and may tolerate closer approach before fleeing or actively fight off a predator, whereas shy individuals either flee at greater distances or they may try to avoid detection in the first place by remaining perfectly still. Consistent individual differences in prey behavior, especially in the propensity to take risks (“boldness”), are widespread in the animal kingdom. Theory predicts that individual behavioral types differ, such that bolder individuals may benefit from greater access to resources while paying higher predation-risk costs than less bold animals (Hulthén et al. 2017). Thus, anything that influences the probability of predation may influence the adaptive value of boldness.

In coral reef ecosystems, damselfish influence both benthic patterns and processes (Ceccarelli 2007). Damselfish defend their territories from intruders, and, by doing so, influence the structural makeup of diversity, algal biomass, coral zoning, and the structure of the benthic community within their territory (Helfman and Winkelman 1997; Ferreira et al. 1998; Ceccarelli et al. 2001).

Given the conflicting demands of territory defense and flight, we asked how an extremely territorial damselfish makes these trade-offs. Damselfish could respond two ways to variation in territory size. First, there could be a positive relationship between FID and territory size. This relationship may emerge, because prey are likely farther from their refuge, and by patrolling a larger territory, they are more exposed. Alternatively, there could be a negative relationship between FID and territory size. This could emerge if the number of refuges increases with territory size. Regarding territoriality, we expected a negative relation between flight initiation distance and territoriality (number of agonistic interactions). To more comprehensively understand these trade-offs, we also quantified life stage and habitat structural complexity.

Materials and methods

Species and study area

We studied Stegastes fuscus an endemic damselfish found in the Southwestern Atlantic, and present in the Brazilian coast between the states of Maranhão and Santa Catarina (Sampaio and Nottingham 2008). The species is abundant on reefs and rock shores (Ferreira et al. 2004, 2015). Its territories influence the diversity and biomass of the algal community, and provide hiding places and food resources for several mesoinvertebrate fauna in south-east Brazil (Ferreira et al. 1998). They are found in shallow waters (< 8 m), in both rocky and biogenic reef, and vary in color based on their age (Manegatti et al. 2003). They are diurnal herbivores that are extremely territorial, patrolling their and expelling intruders that enter their territory (Sampaio and Nottingham 2008; Júnior et al. 2010). They are small, at most 15 cm in length, and forage within relatively restricted areas (Ferreira et al. 1998). However, this territory provides space for the construction of their nests, shelter against predators, and food.

We studied damselfish in the reefs off Boa Viagem beach, Todos os Santos Bay, Salvador (TSB), Bahia. Data were collected between October and December 2017 by snorkeling around and identifying damselfish territories that were in water no deeper than 2 m. Fish territories were separated by at least 4 m. This fringing reef is characterized by low wave energy, clear and shallow waters, and easy access. This reef is occupied by filamentous algae, sponges, calcareous algae, sea urchins, and corals (Cruz 2008), as well as several other fish species.

Quantifying agonistic interactions

We quantified aggressive interactions (bites and chases) and estimated territory size by conducting 5 min focal animal samples. Observers were trained to estimate fish body size in the previous studies (Nunes et al. 2015, 2016) and in this by estimating the size of model fish placed on the reef. Once trained, we estimated fish body size visually and classified individuals up to 7 cm as juveniles, and > 7 cm as adults, which correlates with color pattern differences from their corresponding life stages (Sampaio and Notimham 2008). We counted the number of these agonistic interactions each time which a focal individual interacted aggressively with the same or any other species of fish during the 5-min period. Territory size was estimated during this same period of time, where markers were placed in the substrate at six extreme points of the area defended by the sampled individual. The three largest lengths between the points were measured, and from their mean value, it was possible to calculate the circular area of the territory (area = π. r2) (Aued 2012). Ninety focal animals were observed, and following the focal observation, we measured that individual’s flight initiation distance.

Quantifying flight initiation distance

All flight initiation distances began with the observer positioned 2 m from a focal subject. We estimated flight initiation distance by pushing a rod that had a replica of a 30 cm total length predator grouper (Epinephelus sp., Fig. 1) towards a focal subject at ~ 0.7 m/s. The observer swam in direction of the fish while following the bottom and thereby swam horizontally until reaching the target fish, upon which the observer stretched his arm to present the model to target fish. At the time the fish began to flee, the rod was placed on the substrate and the distance between the end of the model predator (mouth) and the place where the fish started to flee (FID) was measured with a measuring tape. It is important to stress that an escape occurred when the individual’s swimming speed increased beyond that of the model predator’s approach speed (Januchowski-Hartley et al. 2011, 2012).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Predator model used to stimulate flight of Stegastes fuscus (a). The moment that an individual S. fuscus began to flee (b)

Quantifying habitat complexity

After measuring flight initiation distance (n = 90 fish), we quantified rugosity, frequently used as a proxy of structural complexity, using Luckhurst and Luckhurst’s (1978) chain method, where a chain was placed on the substrate in such a way as to follow as closely as possible all the contours and crevices. This generated a measure of the surface that we expressed in relation to the linear distance (Luckhurst and Luckhurst 1978). The RI was calculated using the formula RI = linear/surface, in which “linear” means the measure of when the chain was stretched (1 m) and “surface” is the distance between the beginning and end of the chain when it is adjusted to all the contours and crevices. Three measurements of rugosity were made within all territories of the chosen fish (i.e., 270 measurements in total). We used the average of these three values as our measure of rugosity.

Statistical analyses

We used a backward stepwise algorithm to select the best model that explained variation in FID. Independent variables included structural complexity, prey size, territory size, and the number of agonistic interactions. Simple linear regressions were used to investigate the potential influence of habitat complexity on the size of the territory and the number of interactions and body size on FID. Normality and homogeneity were investigated using QQ plots and the Levene’s test. We set our alpha to 0.05.

Results

Territories averaged 1.71 m−2 (± 0.83 SD; 0.25–3.89; N = 90), fish size averaged 8.7 cm (± 2.43 SD; 4–13; N = 90), and rugosity averaged 1.23 (± 0.1 SD; 1.04–1.52; N = 270). The mean initial flight distance was 35.5 cm (± 13.03 SD; 19–80; N = 90), and the average number of interactions over 5 min was 3.2 (± 3.1 SD; 0–13; N = 90 fish, 286 interactions).

After accounting for non-significant variation in FID explained by territory size, the number of agonistic interactions (F = 0.327; p = 0.569), life phase (F = 0.007; p = 0.934), and body size (F = 1.01; p = 0.317), damselfish tolerated closer approach as rugosity increased (F = 4.28, p = 0.04; Fig. 2; Table 1).

Fig. 2
figure 2

The relationship between rugosity and flight initiation distance. Black line represents best fit for juveniles data, gray line for adults, and gray area is confidence interval (95%)

Table 1 Results from stepwise model selection showing the relation between rugosity and flight initiation distance of the Stegastes fuscus (Pomacentridae)

There was no relationship between rugosity and territory size (F = 0.46; p = 0.50; R2 = 0.0052), nor was there a relationship between the number of agonistic interactions and rugosity (F = 1. 30; p = 0.26; R2 = 0.01).

Discussion

We found no relationship between territory size and FID, and thus, our results did not support the hypothesis that larger territories, potentially with more refuges, modulate risk perceptions. However, we found that as rugosity increased fish’s perceptions of risk decreased, corroborating the previous studies with other species (Nunes et al. 2015; Januchowski-Hartley et al. 2015; Benevides et al. 2016). This is likely to occur, because environments that are more complex provide more refuges. As a result, prey are more likely to escape than in environments that are more homogeneous. Therefore, these individuals will adjust their FID in concordance with expectation from optimal escape theory (Cooper and Frederick 2007).

The previous studies have shown that habitat complexity, frequently estimated by rugosity, is positively associated with the abundance and diversity of fish species, precisely because complex environments provide more shelter (e.g., Luckhurst and Luckhurst 1978; Graham and Nash 2012). In addition, greater habitat complexity results in more areas suitable algae and invertebrates to grow and thrive (Nunes et al. 2013). Hence, increasing complexity not only provides more shelters, but can also increase other resources such as food availability (Cecarrelli et al. 2001).

Territories that have higher quality may have a greater value for the fish, since it may provide more shelters and more food (Johnsson et al. 2000). Animals seem to invest relatively more resources defending valuable habitats and are more reluctant to leave these environments, thus reducing their FID as habitat complexity increases. Complexity may, thus, be viewed as a metric of territory “quality”, and we assume that when individual’s make decisions about whether or not to defend a particular location, the rugosity plays an important role. Future studies are required to formally evaluate this hypothesis. For example, analyzing which variables (e.g., differences in shelter, food, and sexual partner availability, and the presence of different predators and competitors) explain the most variance in territory choice will be essential to better understand the importance of rugosity.

Territorial defense is costly, since it decreases individual foraging time, and increases both energetic demands and predation risk (Cleveland 1999). Therefore, the number of agonistic interactions was expected to explain variation the FID, since individuals defending territories must allocate more energy to defense. We hypothesized that as an individual’s number of interactions increase, its energy expenditure would rise, and thus, the prey would only escape when essential, therefore, reducing its FID. We did not find this expected relationship. We used a short measure of agonistic interactions what could contribute to the potential lack of effect, given that there are a variety of variables that influence agonistic interactions.

Individual body size is another variable that can influence flight initiation distance, but how it does so is not uniform (Gotanda et al. 2009; Feary et al. 2011). Our study did not find a significant relationship between these variables in the study area for the species studied. It is possible that, given high fishing pressure in the study area, the abundance of predators was massively reduced (especially groupers), and this limited the opportunity to learn from negative experiences with predators. However, the loss of some predators does not always result in a complete loss of antipredator behavior and the multipredator hypotheses predict, and a variety of species from diverse taxa reveal, that antipredator behavior persists when isolated from some, but not all predators (Blumstein 2006; Rasheed et al. 2017; Carthey and Blumstein 2018).

It is important to know which variables influence fish anti-predatory behavior, because this not only affects predator–prey relationships, but can have cascading effects and influence the entire structure of communities, and even ecosystems. In the case of territorial species such as damselfish, which are considered key species in reef environments, the presence of predators leads to behavioral changes and its consequences can alter the performance of an entire ecosystem.