Introduction

Ecosystems are consistently exposed to environmental variations where spatio-temporal modifications of abiotic conditions impact living organisms and challenge food-web stability. Indeed, primary productivity, which is strongly related to abiotic parameters, varies notably in time and space and affects both resource distribution and abundance and its accessibility to consumers (Coe et al. 1976; Olff et al. 2002; Townsend et al. 2003; Brown et al. 2010). In a context of global and long-lasting deterioration of ecosystems, behavioural plasticity of individuals plays a key role in a population’s ability to adapt to rapid environmental changes (Nussey et al. 2007). This is particularly true for long-lived species whose microevolutionary responses might be too slow (Wingfield 2003). The ability to handle food shortages depends on the variety of individual foraging tactics in response to multi-scale spatio-temporal changes in prey distribution. To determine whether long-lived species, such as marine predators, can buffer variations in food abundance and location (e.g. distance from the breeding colony), their behavioural plasticity should be tested. However, such testing is difficult in wild populations, mainly because of the difficulty of assessing simultaneously variations in the availability of prey and in predators’ foraging behaviours. Nevertheless, we can measure individual flexibility in foraging behaviour and differences (thus variability) showed by colonies in the same and subsequent years. The lack of such flexibility/variability can be interpreted as either there are no environmental changes influencing prey availability, or the individuals lack plasticity in their behaviour to compensate for environmental changes.

We evaluated the foraging variability in the Northern gannet (Morus bassanus, hereafter named ‘gannet’), a central place forager constrained during the breeding period to return to the colony to feed its single chick at short intervals. Gannets are large seabirds able to travel hundreds of kilometres from their breeding site, which feed on a broad spectrum of prey species and size (Montevecchi and Barrett 1987; Hamer et al. 2000). This mobility, and a broad diet, enables gannets to buffer environmental variability (Montevecchi et al. 2009), and previous studies have highlighted their ability to face inter-annual variations in prey availability (Garthe et al. 2011), consistency in foraging behaviour and foraging area fidelity at the intra-individual level (Hamer et al. 2007), but also contrasting foraging tactics between colonies (Garthe et al. 2006; Hamer et al. 2001). To our knowledge, however, no multi-scale analysis of gannet foraging variability has yet been made. We therefore explored how variability in gannet foraging responses is partitioned at both the intra- and inter-individual levels, as well as the importance of inter-annual and inter-colonial variations. In the light of the rapid recovery of the gannet populations and the rapid expansion of their distribution area with the establishment of new colonies over the last century (Nelson 2002), we hypothesized that variability in their foraging behaviour is largely expressed at an individual level. Individuals may develop foraging flexibility to face environmental variations and changes in food distribution. Additional variability may, however, emerge from groups of individuals (i.e. a colony), thereby extending the variety of foraging patterns. We predicted that any variability in foraging behaviour will thus occur at both the intra- and the inter-individual levels, as well as at and the inter-colony level, and that variability might depend on yearly environmental conditions and changing prey fields (Garthe et al. 2007, 2011). To investigate such foraging variability in gannets, we analysed GPS tracks of successive foraging trips for birds from two distant colonies during 3 consecutive years.

Materials and methods

The fieldwork was conducted in North Norway in July 2008 and 2009, at Store Ulvøyholmen (68°51′N, 14°51′E), Vesterålen and in July 2007, 2008 and 2009 at Storstappen (71°14′N, 25°30′E), near the North Cape (Fig. 1). Authorizations to enter the colonies were granted by the County Governors of Nordland and Finnmark, while permission to handle the birds was given by the Norwegian Directorate for Nature Management and the Norwegian Animal Research Authority.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Localization of the two studied colonies along the Norwegian coast: Store Ulvøyholmen and Storstappen

GPS deployments

We deployed GPS data loggers on breeding adults rearing 2–5-week-old chicks caught in the colonies using a noose pole. Birds were caught when both parents were present at the nest, and we selected the departing bird in the pair identified as the one adopting the ‘sky pointing’ posture (Ropert-Coudert et al. 2004a, b). Handling and attachment of the GPS data logger lasted <10 min in all cases. The bird’s head was covered to reduce stress. The devices were attached to the lower back of the birds using white Tesa tape (Hamburg, Germany) to match plumage colour. These techniques have been used in several previous studies without noticeable impact on the birds (Garthe et al. 2007; Grémillet et al. 2006; Hamer et al. 2009; Pichegru et al. 2010). In 2007 and 2008, devices were deployed for maximum 48 h and recorded at least one foraging trip. In 2009, devices were deployed for 5–6 days, recording at least three consecutive foraging trips for each bird. All birds returned to their nest after the deployment and continued to breed. We caught birds haphazardly to avoid any bias in sex ratio (Lewis et al. 2002) and within the outermost 10-m fringe of the colony. Several birds caught at both colonies had been ringed as chicks, indicating that old birds also bred in the area accessible with the pole (mean age of ringed birds in both colonies (N = 25) was 16.3 years old—eight birds were more than 20 years old). We therefore assumed that working on the fringe of the colony where one might expect to find young/inexperienced birds (Nelson 2002) did not bias the age structure of our sample.

GPS specifications and data filtering

Two models of GPS data loggers from Technosmart (Rome, Italy) were deployed after waterproofing in heat-shrink tubing. In 2007 and 2008, GPSs ‘Gipsy 1’ (95 × 48 × 24 mm; 65 g, i.e. 2 % of adult body mass) were programmed to record positions every 10 s for a maximum duration of 48 h. In 2009, GPSs ‘Gipsy 2’ (110 × 45 × 25 mm; 50 g, i.e. 1.6 % of adult body mass) were set to record every 10 s over 5 consecutive days. Successive trips recorded in 2009 were used to describe repeatability at the individual level. Using direct observations, we noted that trips of <30 min were of birds bringing back nest material and not food, and they were excluded from the analysis. For each trip, we calculated path length (adding all the distances between two consecutive GPS fixes), the maximum distance from the nest reached at sea, the time spent flying (i.e. when speed >10 km h−1) and a path sinuosity index to identify fishing events and locate feeding locations (see Grémillet et al. 2004, 2006 for details and validation). All GPS devices have been tested before deployment to validate accuracy of provided speed values (following Grémillet et al. 2004).

Statistical analyses of foraging path characteristics

Statistical analyses were performed using R 2.11.1 (http://www.r-project.org). First, we addressed the level of foraging variability at the intra- and inter-individual levels and the effect of spatio-temporal factors. A global analysis of variance was conducted using a comparison of generalized linear mixed models (GLMM) with year and colony as fixed effects and bird identity as random effect associated to an autocorrelation structure of order 1, to correct for potential pseudoreplication. A principal component analysis (PCA) was performed to identify correlations between the 6 descriptive variables of the foraging trips: trip duration, total path length, maximum distance to the nest, speed, flying time and sinuosity. Candidate models were run on the two first components of the PCA separately, and we compared them two by two (Zuur et al. 2009) to select the best model. For each comparison, a one-way ANOVA (fixed effects) and LRT method (random effect) were used to determine the model best supported by the dataset. For each component of the PCA (Dim.1 and Dim.2), model selection highlighted the mechanisms and the factors influencing variability in foraging behaviour.

In a second phase, we analysed variability of trip duration, path length, maximum distance and speed with GLMMs (year as fixed effect, identity as random effect associated to an autocorrelation structure of order 1) within each colony. The GLMM was compared with a null model (without year as fixed effect) using a one-way ANOVA. Inter-annual variability was tested using a one-way ANOVA and post hoc Tukey tests.

Spatial analyses

To identify and describe the use of the particular feeding areas by gannets and to understand whether they respond to changes in prey distribution by varying their behaviour, foraging trips and feeding positions were plotted and analysed using Arcgis 9.2 (ESRI, France) and Hawth’s Tools 3.27 extension. We used Minimum Convex Polygons (100 %) to define the visited area for individual trips. The Kernel Density Estimate tool was used to transform feeding positions into density estimates (Wood et al. 2000) and to determine the most intensively exploited areas within the visited area of each colony.

  1. (a)

    Intra-individual variability

We analysed individual variability using three complementary parameters measured on successive trips in 2009.

Visited area fidelity

For every bird, we calculated a cumulative visited area for each additional trip to determine if or how the prospected area increased over 5 days of recording. To assess repeatability at an intra-individual scale, we calculated the overlap percentage of each pair of consecutive trips and the overlap percentage of all trips. To do so, we pooled spatially the two surfaces (not the sum of areas values), and calculated the percentage overlap with respect to the pooled surface. For each colony and each bird, we compared the ‘first trip’ visited area with the overall ‘5-days’ visited area calculated from all trips over the 5 days.

Bearing fidelity

We measured the bearings followed by birds when they left and returned to the nest to examine bearing consistency across successive trips. Since tracks were not all linear, we measured the azimuth on the first linear part at a minimum distance of 3 km from the colony. Using circular statistics for paired samples (Zar 1998), we calculated for all pairs of two successive trips (1) the difference between the azimuth of the first outbound trip (O1) and the azimuth of the second outbound trip (O2) and (2) the difference between the azimuth of the first inbound (I1) trip and the azimuth of the second outbound trip (O2). A mean difference of angle was calculated for each bird. We then calculated differences of angles between foraging trips recorded from different individuals (outbound trip from bird 1 vs. bird 2, etc.). Using two sample t tests, we compared differences in angles from two successive trips of a single bird with differences in angles between two unrelated trips. If repeatability in bearing is high, then the difference in angles should be lower for successive trips of the same bird than for unrelated trips.

Feeding area fidelity

We determined for each bird the total number of distinct feeding areas (feeding events separated by a distance of 5 km) and the total number of feeding events. We also estimated the mean number of repeated visits to the same feeding areas.

  1. (b)

    Inter-individual and inter-colony variability

Inter-individual variability in visited area was examined for each colony via an analysis of preferred directions, feeding locations and visited area overlap amongst individuals of the same colony. The cumulative visited area for each additional bird was calculated for each colony.

  1. (c)

    Inter-annual variability

Kernel density estimates of an entire colony were used as proxies of inter-annual variability in feeding hotspot distributions. We did not, however, find a method to estimate overlapping percentage between 2 years using kernels results. To estimate this overlap, we thus used Minimum Convex Polygons (MCP 100 %) to calculate the total area visited during the first trip of each bird. We then compared the percentage overlap between areas visited on two consecutive years for each colony. We are aware that MCP introduces a bias (i.e. overestimation), but this bias is shared by years and the result gives a comparable measure of overlap.

Results

Sample sizes

We recorded a total of 341 foraging trips performed by 101 birds (N = 21, 23 and 14 on Storstappen in 2007, 2008 and 2009, respectively; N = 23 and 20 on Store Ulvøyholmen in 2008 and 2009, respectively). Each bird performed 1–14 foraging trips and we recorded a minimum of 3 consecutive trips for 39 birds. Because several analyses are informative on multiple scales, results are given for each type of analysis. The four considered scales (intra-individual, inter-individual, inter-annual and inter-colony) will be addressed in the discussion. Values are presented as mean ± SE.

Global variance analysis

The PCA analysis revealed a strong positive correlation between total trip duration, path length, maximum distance to the nest and flying time, and a negative correlation between speed and sinuosity. The two first components of the PCA explained 77.2 % of the variance. The first component ‘Dim.1’ aggregated four of the six variables in relation to the duration and distance to the nest during the foraging trip (i.e. total trip duration, path length, maximum distance to the nest and flying time) and explained 55.8 % of the variance. The second component ‘Dim.2’ (flight speed negatively correlated to sinuosity of the path) explained 21.4 % of the variance.

From the GLMMs (Table 1), we selected the following models:

$$ {\text{Dim}}. 1\quad {\text{Dim}}. 1 \sim {\text{year}} + {\text{colony}},{\text{ identity}} $$
$$ {\text{Dim}}. 2\quad {\text{Dim}}. 2 \sim {\text{year}},{\text{ identity}},{\text{ autocorr}}. $$
Table 1 Comparison of different GLMM models testing the effect of variables on the two first components of the gannet foraging trip PCA analysis (Dim.1 and Dim.2): ‘year’ and ‘colony’ as fixed effects, bird ‘identity’ as random effect and autocorrelation structure

Year and Colony had a significant effect on the observed variability of Dim.1, as well as inter-individual variability (expressed in the model by the random effect ‘identity’). Intra-individual variability was high, implying a weak correlation between two successive trips (autocorrelation factor ϕ = 0.06). Thus, the correlation structure did not improve the model significantly. Year and Identity had a significant effect on the observed variability of Dim.2. The correlation structure estimate (ϕ = 0.55) indicated a positive autocorrelation in speed and sinuosity of foraging. The Colony did not influence variability of Dim.2. We could not test any interaction effect between Year and Colony on the foraging behaviour, because no data were available in 2007 for Store Ulvøyholmen. To examine the foraging variability in detail, we pursued foraging behaviour analysis within colonies.

Spatial analyses

  1. (a)

    Intra- and inter-individual variability

At Store Ulvøyholmen, trip durations varied from 30 min to 22.8 h, with maximum distances from the nest of 3–98 km and total path lengths from 18 to 360 km, all years together. At Storstappen, foraging trip durations varied from 30 min to 23.5 h, with maximum distances from the nest ranging from 5 to 132 km and total path lengths from 18 to 595 km, all years together.

Visited area fidelity

Individual visited areas increased when new trips were recorded. On Store Ulvøyholmen, the size of most individual visited areas reached a plateau beyond four trips. On Storstappen, gannets seemed to prospect new areas on each new foraging trip, although the size of individual visited areas started to reach a plateau beyond seven trips (Fig. 2). On Store Ulvøyholmen, the overlap between two successive trips of a single bird varied between 0 and 96 %, with a mean of 27 %. The overlap for all trips of a single bird varied between 5 and 56 %, with a mean of 27 %. On Storstappen, the overlap between two successive trips varied between 0 and 76 %, with a mean of 11 %. The overlap for all trips of a single bird varied between 1 and 55 %, with a mean of 12 %. At Store Ulvøyholmen, the visited area increased from 1,052 to 1,796 km2 if the first trip of each bird and all trips are considered (‘first trip’ vs. ‘5-days’ visited areas, Table 2; Fig. 3a). In the same way, the ‘first trip’ visited area was 9,672 km2 and the ‘5-days’ visited area was 13,856 km2 at Storstappen (Table 2; Fig. 3b). Cumulative visited areas (calculated for both ‘first trip’ and ‘5-days’) increased in the relation with the number of birds, but the visited area size stopped increasing beyond six and seven birds at Store Ulvøyholmen and Storstappen, respectively (Fig. 4).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Cumulative visited area per individual gannet in relation with the number of foraging trips. Each solid line represents a single bird

Table 2 Characteristics of foraging trips of adult gannets rearing chicks in two Norwegian colonies (recorded with GPS loggers)
Fig. 3
figure 3

Visited areas calculated from the first trip (solid line) and all trips over a 5-day period (dashed line). a Store Ulvøyholmen (N = 12) and b Storstappen (N = 14)

Fig. 4
figure 4

Cumulative visited area per gannet colony in relation with the number of equipped birds and duration of GPS deployment. 5-day deployment in black dots, 1-day deployment in gray triangles

Bearing fidelity

Differences in azimuths from successive trips were similar to azimuths from unrelated trips at Store Ulvøyholmen (p = 0.22 for O1 vs. O2; p = 0.70 for I1 vs. O2). At Storstappen, differences in azimuth between two unrelated trips were lower than the difference of azimuth between 2 successive trips (p = 0.01 for O1 vs. O2; p = 0.04 for I1 vs. O2). At the inter-individual level, a wide range of bearings was used, but three main routes were preferentially followed by gannets in both colonies (Fig. 5).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Main foraging routes followed by gannets in 2007 (dotted line), in 2008 (dashed line and in 2009 (solid line in a Store Ulvøyholmen and b Storstappen

Feeding area fidelity

Gannets from Store Ulvøyholmen performed a mean of 6.3 ± 1.7 trips during 5 days of recording. Each bird foraged in average 8.8 ± 2.3 times in 3.9 ± 1.2 distinct areas. Each feeding area was visited at least twice by the same individual during the 5-day period. On Storstappen, each gannet performed an average of 8.3 ± 2.8 trips during 5 days of recording, and foraged 18.3 ± 5.2 times in 8.3 ± 2.1 distinct areas. Each feeding area was visited at least twice during the 5 days.

At the inter-individual level, kernel density analyses revealed that most of the gannets from Store Ulvøyholmen fed close to the colony, either inshore off Hadseløya or along the north coast of Lofoten (Fig. 6a). At Storstappen, feeding hotspots were northwest of the colony, north off the North Cape and in the fjord south off the colony (Fig. 6b).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Relative density of gannet feeding locations around the breeding sites in 2007, 2008 and 2009, from kernel density estimate analysis, Jenks Natural thresholds in a Store Ulvøyholmen (N = 23 and 20 in 2008 and 2009) and b Storstappen (N = 21, 23 and 14 in 2007, 2008 and 2009)

  1. (b)

    Inter-annual variability

At Store Ulvøyholmen, we found no significant difference between years in the speed of flight, trip duration and the maximum distance to nest between years (ANOVA: duration p = 0.31, max. distance p = 0.17, speed p = 0.11, Table 2) but path lengths were longer in 2008 than in 2009 (Tukey: p = 0.05, Table 2). At Storstappen, trip duration, path length, maximum distance to the nest and flight speed differed significantly between years (ANOVA: duration p = 0.003, path length p < 0.001, max distance p < 0.001 and speed p = 0.034). Trip duration was shorter in 2007 than in 2008 (Tukey: p < 0.001, Table 2) and in 2009 (Tukey: p = 0.02, Table 2). Maximum distance was significantly lower in 2007 than in 2008 (Tukey: p < 0.001, Table 2) and in 2009 (Tukey: p < 0.001, Table 2). Total path length was also significantly different between all years (Tukey: 2007–2008 p < 0.001; 2007–2009 p = 0.001; 2008–2009 p = 0.01, Table 2). Flight speed only differed between 2007 and 2008 (Tukey: p = 0.005, Table 2).

During the different breeding seasons of the study, the gannets from both sites showed preferential routes (Fig. 5), but there were higher inter-annual variations from Storstappen.

Gannets from Store Ulvøyholmen travelled in the same directions both years (Fig. 5a) and, although feeding locations were very similar, they were more tightly clustered in 2009 than in 2008 (Fig. 6a). Gannets from Storstappen foraged within 20 km of the coast in 2007, whereas in 2008, they travelled further offshore in two main directions 50–100 km from the colony. In 2009, they foraged 40–80 km from the colony, following the same 3 main routes as in 2007 (Fig. 5b); though, several birds also moved round the North Cape and southwards to Kamøyfjord and Porsangerfjord. Kernel density estimates showed important variations in feeding locations, although several feeding hotspots were consistent throughout the study period (Fig. 6b).

  1. (c)

    Colony visited area

We calculated the annual home range for each colony as a Minimum Convex Polygon (100 %) based on the first foraging trip of each bird. At Store Ulvøyholmen, visited areas in 2008 and 2009 were of similar size (Table 2) and overlapped by 70 % of the total area (Fig. 7a). At Storstappen, areas visited in 2007 were smaller than in 2008 and 2009 (Table 2). Visited areas overlapped by 34 % of the total area between 2007 and 2008 and by 75 % of the total area between 2008 and 2009 (Fig. 7b).

Fig. 7
figure 7

Annual visited areas in 2007 (hatched line), 2008 (dotted line) and 2009 (solid line), made from the sum of the first foraging trips of each bird in a Store Ulvøyholmen (N = 23 and 20 in 2008 and 2009) and b Storstappen (N = 21, 23 and 14 in 2007, 2008 and 2009)

Discussion

In this study, we analysed components of variability in foraging behaviour of Northern gannets within individuals and between colonies and years. Northern gannets are known to be flexible predators, exploiting a large panel of prey (Montevecchi and Barrett 1987) and responding rapidly to changes in prey distribution by increasing their foraging effort (Garthe et al. 2007; Hamer et al. 2007) or consuming alternative prey (Garthe et al. 2011; Montevecchi and Myers 1997), including fishery discards (Votier et al. 2010). To our knowledge, however, no study encompassed all levels of variability concurrently. Improvements in animal-borne GPS technology allowed us to successfully obtain a reasonable sample of successive foraging trips within individuals. Here, we showed that gannets exhibit high intra-individual flexibility, as well as different foraging tactics at the inter-individual level, which may facilitate their adaptation to variations in prey conditions in space and time. Variability in a top predator’s foraging tactics is the condition sine qua non for behavioural plasticity. Plasticity plays a key role in the ability of organisms to adjust to environmental variability. Understanding mechanisms and limits of animals’ flexibility is crucial for predicting population viability (Komers 1997).

Individual flexibility

Intra-individual flexibility in gannets was illustrated by the great variability in the quantitative and spatial features of their foraging trips. An individual may perform long and short foraging trips successively but its flight speed and path sinuosity remain consistent. The low percentage of overlap in foraging areas between successive trips of the same individual suggests that birds can prospect different areas over a short period of time, thus cumulating individual knowledge of their environment over successive trips. As a consequence, the area explored by the colony as a whole was larger when considering the foraging trips of all birds over several (in our study five) days than when considering only one trip (i.e. the first recorded) per bird.

On the other hand, some birds showed repeatability in foraging tactics, returning on average twice (up to 5 times) to the same feeding area before heading in a different direction on a subsequent foraging trip. Analyses of flight compass bearings revealed that birds switched regularly between them, strongly suggesting memory-based foraging tactics (Davoren et al. 2003a; Montevecchi et al. 2009). When birds were returning in the same area, foraging paths appeared to be very similar, suggesting that prey-patch position may be anticipated by birds (Pettex et al. 2010). Gannets might thus use experience gained during the course of the breeding season to return to profitable areas (Grémillet et al. 2004; Hamer et al. 2007), as shown in other seabird species such as black-legged kittiwakes Rissa tridactyla (Irons 1998) and great cormorant Phalacrocorax carbo (Grémillet et al. 1999) or in other marine top predators such as southern elephant seals Mirounga leonina (Bradshaw et al. 2004) and Antarctic fur seals Arctocephalus gazella (Bonadonna et al. 2001).

A previous study using Platform Transmitter Terminals (PTTs) on gannets showed marked differences between two colonies in individual fidelity to foraging areas (Hamer et al. 2001). At one colony in the North Sea (Bass Rock), birds followed very similar bearings on successive trips, while bearings followed by birds from a colony in the Celtic Sea (Great Saltee) were much less consistent. Our results from precise foraging positions recorded by GPS devices corroborate such findings, showing that individual gannets from the same colony can be both consistent and flexible in the routes followed, in the areas exploited and in the durations and distances of foraging trips. How gannets decide to switch from a foraging area to another is unknown, but public information may play a role in the choice of compass bearing when departing from the colony (Ward and Zahavi 1973; Weimerskirch et al. 2010) although the relevance of this theory for vertebrates is still widely discussed because of a lack of conclusive experimental studies (Richner and Heeb 1995). For example, gannets from Bass Rock were very consistent in the bearings of successive flights but the distance travelled varied amongst trips of similar bearing (Hamer et al. 2001). The authors therefore suggested that gannets were heading to a known area and may use local information from conspecifics at sea to shorten or extend their search. Although local enhancement at sea may also occur, in our case, variations in bearing at the departure from the colony between successive trips might be better explained by the use by individuals of information provided by flocks of gannets returning from the sea, or memory-based mechanisms.

Inter-individual variability

The GLMM analysis highlighted an important inter-individual variability in foraging effort (Table 1). In addition, we recorded a wide range of trip durations (from 30 min to 23 h in both sites) and maximum distances from the nest (from <5 to >100 km). The bearing consistency also differed amongst individuals, from birds switching constantly between exploited foraging areas in all directions to birds following the same routes each time. Decision-making could be related to the success of previous foraging trips and a decrease of prey in patches or an avoidance of intra-specific competition. Prey depletion experiments in bumblebees Bombus impatiens have highlighted that individuals rapidly respond to an unsuccessful foraging bout by shifting foraging areas, and by returning to known profitable areas, even of lower quality (Townsend-Mehler and Dyer 2012). Similar experiments in great tits Parus major revealed marked differences in performance between individuals in their ability to find a more profitable site after resource depletion (Smith and Sweatman 1974). Contrasting site fidelity between gannets might reflect differences in foraging flexibility amongst individuals.

Our results showed that gannets prospected the whole potential feeding area around their colony. Cumulative visited areas revealed an inter-individual variability, but spatial inter-individual variability remained relatively low compared with individual flexibility. Recording several trips of a limited number of birds was sufficient to depict the whole area visited by birds from the colony. Our results suggested that to define the ‘visited area’ of a given colony in a given breeding period, it may be more informative to track successive trips of a small number of individuals rather than just one trip of a large number of individuals. There was considerable consistency amongst colonies in the choice of routes and feeding areas. Gannets from both colonies preferentially followed three directions and there was an annual occurrence of feeding hotspots at both sites. Although predictability of prey is higher at large spatial scales, habitats with specific features such as shelf edges and frontal zones benefit from an enhanced productivity and providing relatively stable prey concentrations for predators (Weimerskirch 2007). The feeding grounds along the North Norwegian coast are very productive due to the convergence of the low-saline Norwegian Coastal Current and the warm-saline North Atlantic Currents close to land off the Vesterålen and the North Cape area (Barrett et al. 2006). Food samples collected from regurgitates around the colony and from the handled birds were mainly composed of large and energy-rich prey at both sites: 25–30 cm herring Clupea harengus and mackerel Scomber scombrus at Storstappen, and herring, mackerel and saithe Pollachius virens at Store Ulvøyholmen (Pettex et al. unpublished data). Marked colony-preferred routes and feeding sites might thus reflect a good predictability of prey at a small spatio-temporal scale in both colonies (Davoren et al. 2003a). This is also concordant with the short foraging ranges observed at both sites, considering the gannets’ ability to travel great distances to find prey (Hamer et al. 2007).

Inter-colony variability

Preferred routes and feeding hotspots occurred at both sites, with birds from Storstappen exploiting feeding areas twice as large as those from Store Ulvøyholmen. The degree of overlap for two successive trips was consistently much lower at Storstappen (11 %) than at Store Ulvøyholmen (27 %).

In 2008 and 2009, gannets from Storstappen explored and foraged an area 8 times larger, made longer foraging trips and travelled further than gannets from Store Ulvøyholmen. This is consistent with the hypothesis that foraging effort and visited area are positively correlated to the size of the colony (Lewis et al. 2001) with Store Ulvøyholmen and Storstappen holding 308 and 1,244 breeding pairs in 2008, respectively (Barrett 2008). One might thus expect gannets from Storstappen to put in a greater foraging effort than those from Store Ulvøyholmen due to possible intra-specific competition for food. The duration of foraging trips did not, however, differ between the two sites, suggesting that (a) there was no such competition or (b) the gannets from Store Ulvøyholmen may have rested for longer periods on the sea surface during their trips (Ropert-Coudert et al. 2004a, b) than their conspecifics from Storstappen. Whereas gannets from both colonies might experience good environmental conditions that limit competition, differences in travelled distances and visited areas may also reflect differences in the distribution and availability of prey around the two study colonies. Variability in foraging behaviour in relation to contrasting environmental conditions between breeding sites has already been documented in gannets (Garthe et al. 2006; Hamer et al. 2001) and amongst other marine predators, such as gentoo penguin Pygoscelis papua (Lescroel and Bost 2005) and Northern fur seals Callorhinus ursinus (Robson et al. 2004). Whereas further work on more sites would be necessary to reach any firm conclusion, birds from the two breeding sites in this study behaved differently.

Inter-annual variability

Changes in food distribution can affect predator foraging behaviour, and animals should adapt their foraging effort or exploit different feeding grounds to find their prey (McCafferty et al. 1998; Hamer et al. 2007). Environmental conditions near Store Ulvøyholmen were probably similar in 2008 and 2009, as gannets showed a very similar foraging behaviour. Visited areas were of identical size (ca 1,000 km2) and overlapped widely. Gannets had marked preferred areas and foraged close to the colony, suggesting conditions of favourable and stable prey availability during our study. Conversely, gannets from Storstappen increased their foraging effort (trip duration, maximum distance to the nest and distance travelled) and their visited area significantly between 2007 and 2009 (Table 2). Although several areas were consistently exploited during the three breeding seasons, the general distribution of feeding hotspots varied between years. For instance, in 2008, birds from Storstappen foraged intensely in two areas to the north and northwest of the colony (ca 80 km), while in 2009, most of the fishing activity was limited to a semicircle 40 km from the breeding site and towards Porsangerfjord. This resulted in important variations in the overlap of the colony visited areas between 2007 and the two subsequent years. These changes in foraging effort and feeding areas in gannets may be a response to variations in distribution and/or availability of their prey. Although the Barents Sea holds amongst the most productive grounds in the North Atlantic (Sakshaug et al. 2009), previous studies have highlighted pronounced interannual variations of its productivity and biomass in relation to the North Atlantic Oscillation (Drinkwater 2011). These changes resulted in important variation in the distribution of pelagic fish such as capelin Mallotus villosus and cod Gadus morhua (Loeng and Drinkwater Loeng and Drinkwater 2007), but also herring (Røttingen 1990), an important prey for Storstappen gannets (Montevecchi and Barrett 1987, personal observation).

Ecological, conservation and methodological implications of flexibility in foraging

Our study showed that gannet foraging behaviour varies at the four considered levels and that all derive from individual flexibility. Individual flexibility, and as a consequence inter-individual variability, may give to gannets an advantageous ability to cope with environmental changes. The world population of Northern gannets dramatically declined across the nineteenth century because of a massive exploitation of adults, eggs and chicks (Wanless et al. 2005), but gannets positively and rapidly responded to protection measures during the twentieth century, recovering and recolonizing a large part of their distribution area (Montevecchi and Hufthammer 1990; Wanless et al. 2005). The rapid recovery of gannets over the last decades might be related to their highly flexible foraging behaviour (Garthe et al. 2007; Hamer et al. 2001, 2007) and broad diet spectrum (Montevecchi et al. 2009). Young colonies often experience a long latency period in their development, during which only a small number of pioneers settles, before becoming attractive for other individuals and growing fast (Montevecchi et al. 1987; Moss et al. 2002). Pioneering individuals will not have the benefit of many conspecifics to attend efficiently all profitable feeding areas (Forbes and Kaiser 1994) either via local enhancement (Davoren et al. 2003b) or public information (Ward and Zahavi 1973). Instead, they must rely upon individual flexibility to cope with the unpredictable marine environment. Thus, the foraging flexibility of settlers might be essential for an efficient exploitation of resources and to ensure the durability of the colony during the early stage of its development. It is, however, essential to assess the magnitude of foraging flexibility across the year cycle (e.g. during and outside the breeding season), as well as for different life stages (fledglings and juveniles, breeding and non-breeding individuals, see Votier et al. 2011), to attain a complete picture of behaviours that fuel plasticity in marine top predators such as gannets.

Our results also showed that the distribution of foraging birds might differ amongst years. This must be considered in the future when developing conservation measures intended to protect marine predators. Marine protected areas (MPA) are increasingly implemented to protect pelagic habitats (Wood et al. 2008), but the mobility of targeted species is a challenge to define suitable limits for them (Game et al. 2009). Bio-logging technologies have greatly improved our knowledge of the spatial ecology of marine species (Ryan et al. 2004; Staniland et al. 2004; Piatt et al. 2006; Burger and Shaffer 2008), and concurrently, the dynamic character of marine ecosystems needs to be considered by providing multi-year data to conservation managers (Wilson et al. 2009). Multi-site studies might also be necessary to faithfully describe foraging behaviour on a regional scale and acquire a reliable overview of animal behaviour. These constraints should be taken into account in future studies and management decisions to avoid a mismatch between the complexity of animal behaviour and conservation objectives.