Introduction

Afferent input drives changes in the motor cortex (Kaneko et al. 1994a, b; Matyas et al. 2010). Based on this framework, peripheral sensory stimulation has been used as a method to influence the motor cortex (Celnik et al. 2007; Kaelin-Lang et al. 2002; Schabrun et al. 2012). For example, corticomotor neuronal excitability as measured using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) has been shown to increase during muscle vibration, compared to no vibration (Rosenkranz and Rothwell 2003; Rosenkranz et al. 2003). After 30 min of electrical stimulation of the hand, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signals during finger movement in the primary motor (M1) and somatosensory area (S1) increased, compared to that before the stimulation (Golaszewski et al. 2004). After 2 h of transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation, corticomotoneuronal excitability increases via GABAergic mechanism (Celnik et al. 2007; Kaelin-Lang et al. 2002). Therefore, electrical or vibratory peripheral sensory stimulation has been used as a means to induce neuroplastic changes to complement neurorehabilitation for movement impairment (Celnik et al. 2007; Meesen et al. 2011; Conforto et al. 2010; Lotze et al. 2017; Gomes-Osman and Field-Fote 2015; Cordo et al. 2013; Marconi et al. 2011). Unfortunately, most modalities of peripheral sensory stimulation involve wearing of a glove or similar device or suprathreshold stimulation. Gloves hamper dexterous finger movement and cause a sense of discomfort (Yu et al. 2019; Kinoshita 1999). Suprathreshold electrical stimulation causes tingling sensation (Golaszewski et al. 2004; Meesen et al. 2011), and increasing vibration amplitude may lead to an illusion of movement or tonic vibration reflex (Ferrari et al. 2018). Thus, most modalities of peripheral sensory stimulation are administered for a limited duration (ranging from 20 min to 2 h a day), while a person is in a sedentary posture. Furthermore, the effect diminishes after the stimulation (Kaelin-Lang et al. 2002; Smith and Brouwer 2005), requiring repeated receipts of the stimulation. These constraints substantially lower patient adherence to a stimulation regimen.

To improve the delivery mode while leveraging the benefits of sensory stimulation, a new stimulation method has been proposed (Enders et al. 2013; Seo et al. 2014). This new stimulation method involves application of vibratory stimulation to wrist skin, below a level perceptible to a person. The imperceptible nature of the stimulation, along with the location of the stimulation device being off from the fingers and hand, may enable natural sensorimotor processing for concurrent hand tasks. Furthermore, the stimulation can be delivered during the activities of daily living continuously for a substantially longer dosage than the existing stimulation methods. This stimulation may potentially be delivered via a watch-type wearable device to provide user-friendly stimulation to complement rehabilitation. Early studies using this specific stimulation are promising, with improved ability to detect light touch on fingertips (Enders et al. 2013; Lakshminarayanan et al. 2015) and to grasp and manipulate objects (Seo et al. 2014) in patients after stroke and healthy adults during stimulation. However, there is little understanding of its mechanism of action.

Early studies postulated a broad cortical mechanism given non-specificity of the stimulation site in which detection of light touch on fingertips improved during stimulation to volar wrist, dorsal wrist, dorsum of the hand, and thenar and hypothenar area (Enders et al. 2013; Lakshminarayanan et al. 2015). Yet, a question remained whether this imperceptible stimulation even reaches the sensorimotor cortex. Therefore, in the present study, we examined whether this stimulation results in measurable changes in the sensorimotor cortex. Specifically, we hypothesized that this stimulation would increase cortical motor excitability as measured using TMS and cortical sensorimotor activity as measured by electroencephalogram (EEG), compared with no stimulation.

Materials and methods

Participants

Eighteen healthy right-handed adults (8 males and 10 females) with a mean ± standard deviation (SD) age of 27 ± 5 years participated in the TMS study. Twenty healthy right-handed adults (12 males and 8 females) with a mean age of 25 ± 5 years participated in the EEG study. There was no overlap in subjects between the TMS and EEG studies. All subjects verbally disclosed that they had no history of upper limb injury or musculoskeletal or neurologic disorders. All TMS subjects were screened for TMS safety. The two studies took place at two universities (one study in each university). Each study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the university where the study was performed. All subjects read and signed written informed consent forms approved by the Institutional Review Board prior to participating in the study.

Experimental design and statistical analysis

Wrist stimulation

Vibration was applied using a C-3 Tactor (EAI Inc., Casselberry, FL, USA) attached to the left volar wrist using tape for both the TMS and EEG studies (Fig. 1). The vibrator was driven by white-noise signals low-pass filtered at 500 Hz. White-noise signal was used based on the literature in stochastic facilitation that collectively demonstrates broadband white-noise stimulation which enhances signal transmission (Collins et al. 1996) and neural communication (Ward et al. 2010). A review (McDonnell and Ward 2011) and many applications exist (Collins et al. 1996, 1997; Wells et al. 2005; Kurita et al. 2013). At the beginning of each study, subjects’ sensory threshold for this wrist vibration was determined as the minimum vibration intensity (i.e., amplitude) that the subject was able to feel. Specifically, the sensory threshold was determined by increasing or decreasing the vibration intensity until the subject could or could not perceive the vibration, respectively, and taking the average of the vibration intensities at which the perception changed, using the ascending and descending limits’ method. The vibration amplitude corresponding to the sensory threshold was 4.5 ± 5.1 µm (in root mean square). Vibration intensity was subsequently set to 60% of the sensory threshold for the TMS and EEG evaluation, as this intensity has been shown to affect finger sensation (Enders et al. 2013; Lakshminarayanan et al. 2015), finger sensory processing (Seo et al. 2015), and hand motor function (Seo et al. 2014).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Setup for TMS (a) and EEG (b) experiments

TMS evaluation

Hand motor cortex excitability assessed using TMS was compared between the vibration on and off conditions. Specifically, the resting motor threshold (RMT), short-interval intracortical inhibition (SICI), and intracortical facilitation (ICF) for the primary motor cortex area for the thumb muscle, abductor pollicis brevis (APB), were compared between the vibration on and off conditions. The vibrator was placed on the subject throughout the session (approximately 50 min), and was turned on or off depending on the vibration condition. All motor cortex excitability measures were taken in one vibration condition, and then again in the other vibration condition, with the order of the vibration on and off conditions randomized across subjects. For the vibration ‘on’ condition, the subthreshold vibration was on continuously during the evaluation (approximately 25 min). The experimenter administering the TMS was blinded to the vibration condition. The vibration intensity was set to be subthreshold (i.e., imperceptible); thus, subjects should not have felt when the vibration was on. When asked after the testing was complete, 1 out of 18 subjects reported that s/he could feel when the vibration was on during testing, and 2 subjects reported feeling vibration briefly at some point during testing.

Subjects were comfortably seated in a TMS chair (Rogue Research Inc., Montreal, QC, Canada) and at rest. TMS was applied to the motor ‘hotspot’ in the subject’s right hemisphere, using a 70 mm figure-of-eight coil connected to Magstim Bistim2 2002 (Jali Medical, Waltham, MA, USA). The coil was held tangential to the scalp with the handle pointing backward and laterally at 45° from the midline, and moved in a systematic grid search over the right primary motor cortex at a slightly suprathreshold stimulus intensity to find the hotspot. The hotspot was determined as the location which consistently induced the largest motor-evoked potential (MEP) amplitude in the left APB muscle. This stimulation location was kept consistent throughout the study with guidance from the Brainsight® system (Rogue Research Inc. Montreal, QC, Canada).

Surface electromyography (EMG) was recorded from the right APB muscle using disposable silver/silver chloride electrodes (Natus Medical Inc., Pleasanton, CA, USA) in a belly tendon montage with the ground electrode on the back of the hand (Fig. 1a). The EMG data were obtained using a CED 1902 Signal conditioner and 1401 interface (Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK) and recorded using Spike2 software (Cambridge Electronic Design Ltd., Cambridge, England) at a sampling frequency of 5 kHz.

The RMT, SICI, and ICF were assessed at rest following the guidelines of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology (Rossini et al. 2015). Specifically, the RMT was determined as the percentage of the maximum stimulator output (%MSO) to elicit a 0.05 mV peak-to-peak MEP amplitude on the resting hand muscle with a 50% probability using the Parameter Estimation by Sequential Testing (Awiszus 2003; Ah Sen et al. 2017). SICI and ICF were assessed using paired pulse protocols, involving a subthreshold conditioning stimulus followed by a suprathreshold test stimulus applied to the hotspot. The interstimulus interval (ISI) between the conditioning and test stimuli was 3 ms for SICI to inhibit the MEP response, while ISI was 15 ms for ICF to facilitate the MEP response. The reduction in the peak-to-peak MEP amplitude with the conditioning stimulus, compared to MEP amplitude with testing stimulus alone, indicated SICI. The increase in the peak-to-peak MEP amplitude with the conditioning stimulus compared to without the conditioning stimulus indicated ICF. The testing stimulus intensity was set as the percentage of the %MSO that evoked a peak-to-peak MEP amplitude of approximately 1 mV at rest consistently for three consecutive trials. The conditioning stimulation intensity was set to 5%MSO below RMT. The unconditioned and conditioned MEPs were obtained eight times each for SICI and also for ICF. The average peak-to-peak MEP amplitude values were used to compute SICI and ICF.

TMS analysis

A custom-made program in MATLAB (The MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) was used to extract the peak-to-peak MEP amplitudes. All extracted data were visually reviewed for accuracy. The generalized extreme studentized deviate test (Rosner 1983) was used to detect outliers in the distribution of data points for each cortical excitability measure that had multiple measurements (conditioned and unconditioned MEPs which had eight data points each per subject). In the event of an outlier in the distribution of values for a single subject on a single parameter, this number was deleted. This occurred 4.8% for all data points. The MEPs were averaged across repetitions for each subject for each measure. The repetition-averaged data were assessed for normality before commencing subsequent analyses. Non-normality was not evidenced (Kolmogorov–Smirnov normality test p > 0.05) for all measures (RMT, SICI, and ICF). The vibration on and off conditions for each measure were compared using a paired t test. The significance level of 0.05/3, adjusted for multiple outcome variables (Bonferroni), was used. Additional paired t tests were performed to compare testing stimulus intensities and unconditioned MEP amplitudes between the two vibration conditions. All statistical analysis was performed using SAS (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC, USA).

EEG acquisition

Cortical sensorimotor activity measured by EEG was compared between the two vibration conditions. Subjects were instructed to rest, then perform 2-s long grips, and rest again per visual cues. The beginning of subsequent grips was separated by a random time interval between 7 and 8 s (s) (jittered by the computer). Visual cues prompted subjects when to start the grip and when to rest. Subjects performed 20 grips in 1 vibration condition followed by 20 grips in the other vibration condition in a single run of EEG collection. Subjects completed a total of five runs (100 grips in each vibration condition). The order of the vibration condition was randomized for each run and each subject. The vibrator was placed on the subject throughout the study (approximately 45 min), and was turned on or off depending on the vibration condition by the computer. During the trials for which the vibration condition was ‘on’, the vibration was continuously on throughout the half run duration including 20 grips and rests in between. The vibration intensity was set to be subthreshold. Subjects were informed that there would be vibration applied to the wrist, but they did not know when the vibration would be applied. After the EEG testing was complete, when asked if they felt the vibration at any time during the EEG recording, all subjects reported that they did not feel the vibration during the EEG recording. Therefore, none of the subjects knew for which trials that the vibration was on.

Subjects were seated during the EEG testing. Subjects were instructed to perform precision pinch grip using the index finger and thumb of the left hand per visual cue. Visual cues were provided on a computer screen by a custom LabVIEW program (National Instruments, Austin, TX, USA) (Fig. 1b). For grip, subjects were instructed to produce and maintain 4 N grip force which they practiced prior to testing using force sensors (2 Mini40, ATI Industrial Automation Inc, Apex, NC, USA). This force level was deemed low enough not to induce excessive fatigue and also clearly distinguishable from the rest.

EEG signals were continuously recorded at 1 kHz using a 64-channel active electrode system (actiCAP, Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany) and a Synamps2 amplifier system (Neuroscan, Charlotte, NC, USA). The electrode position followed the international 10–20 system with an average reference and a ground at AFz. The EEG cap was placed on the subject’s head, such that the Cz electrode was at the vertex. Each electrode site was hydrated using SuperVisc gel (Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany). All electrodes’ impedance was checked to be below 20 kΩ. EEG signals were amplified, applied with a bandwidth filter at 0.10–200 Hz and a notch filter at 60 Hz, and recorded at 1 kHz using the Neuroscan software (SCAN 4.5). The timing of the visual cues to grip and rest was recorded together with the EEG data by the Neuroscan software via an external trigger input.

EEG analysis

The EEG data were analyzed using MATLAB and the EEGLAB toolbox. The data were band-pass filtered at 0.5–50 Hz. Independent component analysis was performed on the data to remove the sources of artifacts using the ADJUST algorithm (Mognon et al. 2011). Current source density estimates for surface potential were computed using a toolbox (Kayser and Tenke 2006). Then, data were divided into epochs ranging from − 2.5 to 4.5 s relative to the grip cue, with the time period before the grip cue (− 2.5 to 0 s) as the baseline (resting) brain activity. Epochs with values beyond − 450 to 450 µV were removed. This resulted in rejection of average 9% of the total trials for both vibration conditions, except for one subject for which over 90% of the trials were rejected. This one subject’s datum was excluded from the analysis.

Baseline power and event-related spectral perturbation (ERSP) were obtained. The baseline power represents the resting brain activity. ERSP was obtained using time–frequency analysis, and depicts the dynamic changes in power of the EEG frequency spectrum from the baseline, specific for the grip activity. Specifically, reduction in power from the baseline with the grip task (event-related desynchronization, ERD) represents the brain activity for the grip, while ERD during grip termination represents the brain activity for grip termination (Alegre et al. 2003; Nakayashiki et al. 2014; Platz et al. 2000; Toma et al. 2000; Ohara et al. 2000; Crone et al. 1998; Formaggio et al. 2015; Gross et al. 2005). ERSP was obtained in time bins of 32 ms and frequency bins of 1 Hz. The power for the alpha (8–12 Hz) and beta band (13–30 Hz) was computed by averaging over each frequency band. These frequency bands are chosen as they have been shown to be relevant for sensorimotor processing, especially with sensory stimulation (Melgari et al. 2013; Tecchiio et al. 2008; Pfurtscheller et al. 2001; Salenius et al. 1997).

For statistical analysis, two repeated-measures ANOVAs were performed for baseline power and ERD, separately. For baseline power, the independent variables included vibration condition (on vs. off), frequency band (alpha and beta), and electrode (FC4, C4, and CP4, covering the sensorimotor areas including premotor, motor, and sensory areas). For ERD, the independent variables included vibration condition (on vs. off), frequency band (alpha and beta), electrode (FC4, C4, and CP4), and task (initiating a grip and relaxing from a grip). Specifically, ERD for grip initiation was the event-related spectral perturbation averaged over the 1 s period immediately after the grip cue, and ERD for grip termination was the event-related spectral perturbation averaged over the 1 s period immediately after the rest cue. In addition, to examine any change in baseline power or ERD over the testing session, potentially due to desensitization to the continuous vibration, an additional independent variable of first vs. second half of the testing session was included for both ANOVAs. All second-order interactions were also included in both ANOVAs. The significance level of 0.025 (adjusted for multiple tests) was used. ANOVAs were performed using SAS. Furthermore, a topographical distribution of the differences between the two vibration conditions was visually examined using t maps.

Results

TMS

Motor cortex excitability was compared between the wrist vibration on vs. off conditions for each measure (Fig. 2). The vibration resulted in a significant reduction in SICI (i.e., less inhibition) compared to vibration off (p = 0.014): The conditioned MEP was, on average, 39% of the unconditioned MEP (61% inhibition) with the vibration off vs. 64% of the unconditioned MEP (36% inhibition) with the vibration on. The RMT and ICF were not significantly different between the two vibration conditions (p = 0.521 and p = 0.515, respectively).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Comparison between the vibration on and off conditions for the resting motor threshold (RMT) (a), short-interval intracortical inhibition (SICI) (b), and intracortical facilitation (ICF) (c). Means with SE are shown as bar graphs with error bars. Individual subjects’ data are shown as lines. b, c Conditioned MEP amplitudes expressed as % of the unconditioned MEP amplitudes. Increase in values in b indicates reduced inhibition. A significant difference between the vibration on and off conditions was found for SICI only (noted with asterisk in b)

The mean RMT ± SD was 39 ± 9%MSO. The conditioning stimulus intensity mean ± SD was 34 ± 9%MSO, or 87 ± 4%RMT. The mean testing stimulus intensity was 74 ± 16%MSO. The testing stimulus intensity for the paired pulse protocol was not significantly different between the vibration on and off conditions (p = 0.554). The unconditioned MEP amplitude was also not significantly different between the vibration on and off conditions (p = 0.298, mean ± SE = 1.0 ± 0.1 mV for vibration off, 1.1 ± 0.2 mV for vibration on).

EEG

Electroencephalogram power during rest and ERD for the grip was compared between the vibration on vs. off conditions. During rest, repeated-measures ANOVA showed that EEG power significantly differed by the vibration condition (on vs. off), frequency band (alpha vs. beta), electrode (FC4/C4/CP4), and session (first vs. second half) (p < 0.025). No interactions were found to be significant (p > 0.5). Specifically, the resting power was lower, while the vibration was on, compared to while it was off (p = 0.011, mean ± SE = 12.8 ± 0.3 µV2/Hz for vibration on, 13.3 ± 0.3 µV2/Hz for vibration off). The t map showing t statistics comparing vibration on vs. off for each electrode is shown in Fig. 3. The resting power was lower for the alpha than the beta frequency band (p = 0.001, mean ± SE = 12.7 ± 0.3 µV2/Hz for alpha, and 13.3 ± 0.3 µV2/Hz for beta). The resting power was lowest for the FC4 electrode (p = 0.001, mean ± SE = 12.6 ± 0.3 µV2/Hz for FC4, 13.4 ± 0.4 µV2/Hz for C4, and 13.1 ± 0.3 µV2/Hz for CP4). The resting power increased over the testing session (p < 0.001, mean ± SE = 12.6 ± 0.3 µV2/Hz for the first half, and 13.5 ± 0.3 µV2/Hz for the second half).

Fig. 3
figure 3

The t statistics comparing power during baseline (rest) between the vibration ‘on’ and ‘off’ conditions, for the alpha (a) and beta band (b), with the blue color indicating reduced power with vibration compared to without

During the grip initiation and termination, ERSP time course is shown in Fig. 4a, b and topography is shown in Fig. 4c, d. ERD occurred shortly after the grip cue (at 0 s) and again shortly after the rest cue (at 2 s) for both alpha (Fig. 4a) and beta band (Fig. 4b), consistent with the literature (Crone et al. 1998; Pfurtscheller and Lopes da Silva 1999; Gourab and Schmit 2010). ERD was topographically located in the bilateral sensorimotor cortical area for grip initiation and termination, for the alpha (Fig. 4c) and beta band (Fig. 4d), consistent with the literature (Alegre et al. 2003; Nakayashiki et al. 2014; Platz et al. 2000; Toma et al. 2000; Ohara et al. 2000; Crone et al. 1998; Formaggio et al. 2015; Gross et al. 2005). The t map showed the topography for difference in ERD between the vibration on vs. off conditions (Fig. 4, bottom row).

Fig. 4
figure 4

The time course of event-related spectral perturbation (ERSP) is shown for the alpha (a) and beta band (b) for the two vibration conditions, with shades showing the standard error for each vibration condition. ERSP is expressed as change from the baseline power in dB. The vertical lines represent the times that the grip cue was presented to the subject at 0 s and the rest cue at 2 s. ERSP time course averaged for three electrodes (FC4/C4/CP4) is shown. Topography of event-related desynchronization (ERD) is shown for the alpha (c) and beta band (d). In c and d, the first column shows the topography for grip initiation (averaged over 1 s period immediately after the cue to grip), and the second column shows the topography for grip termination (averaged over 1 s period immediately after the cue to rest). The first row shows ERD topography for the vibration off condition, the second row for the vibration on condition, and the third row for t statistics comparing the vibration on vs. off conditions with the blue color indicating greater ERD with vibration compared to without

Repeated-measures ANOVA showed that ERD significantly differed by the vibration condition (on vs. off), frequency band (alpha vs. beta), electrode (FC4/C4/CP4), task (grip initiation vs. termination), and interactions between frequency band and electrode, electrode and task, and frequency band and session (p < 0.025). The main effect of session (first vs. second half) and other second-order interactions were not significant (p > 0.3). Specifically, vibration increased ERD (p = 0.009, mean ± SE = 1.6 ± 0.1 dB for vibration on, 1.4 ± 0.1 dB for vibration off, Fig. 4). ERD was greater for alpha than beta (p < 0.001, Fig. 4). ERD differed by electrode (p < 0.001) and electrode × frequency band (p = 0.001), in which mean ERD was the largest for C4 followed in order by CP4 and FC4 for the alpha band, whereas, for the beta band, mean ERD was the largest for C4 followed in order by FC4 and CP4 (Fig. 4c, d). ERD differed by task (p = 0.018) and task × electrode (p = 0.009), in which mean ERD was larger for grip initiation than termination for C4 and CP4, and vice versa for FC4. As for the interaction between frequency band and session (p = 0.010), mean ERD decreased in the second half compared to the first half of the testing session for alpha (mean ± SE = 2.1 ± 0.1 dB for the first half and 1.9 ± 0.1 dB for the second half), whereas the opposite trend was seen for beta (mean ± SE = 1.0 ± 0.1 dB for the first half and 1.1 ± 0.1 dB for the second half).

Discussion

The main finding of this study is that subthreshold random-frequency wrist vibration activated the sensorimotor cortex (as seen by reduced EEG power) and released intracortical inhibition of the primary motor cortex (as seen by reduced SICI, i.e., disinhibition) at rest. For grip activity, vibration resulted in additionally increased brain activity (as seen by greater EEG ERD). We postulate the mechanisms of these vibration effects as follows.

Pathway from peripheral stimulation to M1

At rest, subthreshold vibration, although imperceptible to subjects, may activate mechanoreceptors in the wrist skin and induce action potentials in the afferent neurons, because the minimum intensity of tactile stimuli on the palmar and wrist skin to activate sensory neurons (i.e., neuronal threshold) is lower than the perceptual threshold (i.e., the minimum intensity that is perceptible to a person) (Vallbo and Johansson 1984). Subthreshold electrical cutaneous stimulation has been shown to induce evoked potentials in the somatosensory cortex (Nierhaus et al. 2015). In regards to EEG power, suprathreshold electrical or mechanical stimulation decreases the EEG power over the sensorimotor cortex (compared to that without stimulation) at rest (Tecchiio et al. 2008; Pfurtscheller et al. 2001; Salenius et al. 1997; de Moraes Silva et al. 2015). Specifically, beta power reduction indicates activation of the sensorimotor cortex for receipt and processing of the sensory stimulation (Tecchiio et al. 2008; Pfurtscheller et al. 2001; Salenius et al. 1997; de Moraes Silva et al. 2015). The present study shows that subthreshold vibration also decreased the EEG power over the sensorimotor cortex at rest (Fig. 3). To the authors’ best knowledge, this study is the first to show activation of the sensorimotor cortex in terms of resting EEG power reduction with subthreshold peripheral sensory stimulation.

Vibration affected not only beta (for sensory processing) but also alpha power. Alpha rhythm is generated by the thalamic oscillator function of the lateral thalamic nuclei, and sensory afference disrupts this oscillator function, thereby reducing alpha power (Schreckenberger et al. 2004). The lateral thalamic nuclei mainly project afferences to the sensory cortex, corroborating the observation that the alpha rhythm originates from postcentral sensory areas (Ohara et al. 2000; Ritter et al. 2009). Functionally, reduction in alpha power represents activation of a broad synaptic network with distributed cortical representations that provide an ancillary support (Crone et al. 1998) such as attention and release of inhibition (Jensen and Mazaheri 2010; Anderson and Ding 2011). Therefore, subthreshold vibration might have resulted in arousal and release of inhibition for the sensorimotor cortex.

Decrease in power with vibration was spread over the frontoparietal areas (Fig. 3), suggesting that stimulation was received in the somatosensory/parietal area and further processed in the premotor area, as secondary processing of sensory input in the premotor area has been documented (Zhang and Ding 2010; Auksztulewicz and Blankenburg 2013). Furthermore, the premotor cortex influences M1 (Civardi et al. 2001). Functional connections from the S1 to M1 have been demonstrated in the long-latency (~ 50 ms) cutaneomuscular reflex (Jenner and Stephens 1982; Chen and Ashby 1993; Corden et al. 2000) and the positive correlation between S1 and M1 excitability (Schabrun et al. 2012) and intracortical microstimulation studies (Kaneko et al. 1994a, b; Matyas et al. 2010).

Effect on M1

Through these pathways, it has been shown that suprathreshold electrical cutaneous stimulation (with intensity 2–3 times the sensory threshold) reduces SICI in M1 (Ridding and Rothwell 1999; Ridding et al. 2005). The present study showed that subthreshold vibratory cutaneous stimulation (with intensity 40% below the sensory threshold) also reduced SICI. Reduction in SICI was due to the sensory stimulation, not due to different stimulation parameters, as there were no significant differences in the unconditioned MEP sizes, test stimulus intensities, and conditioning stimulus intensities between the stimulation on and off conditions. While SICI decreased, ICF and RMT did not change. These findings in which effects were seen only on SICI but not on ICF and RMT are consistent with the previous studies using cutaneous electrical stimulation (Celnik et al. 2007; Rosenkranz and Rothwell 2003). SICI represents GABAergic inhibitory circuit activity, while ICF represents glutamatergic excitatory activity (Rossini et al. 2015). Therefore, change only in SICI could be because somatosensory-induced M1 excitability changes are predominantly influenced by GABAergic function (Kaelin-Lang et al. 2002). The lack of the overall excitability change (RMT and MEP) could be due to the low stimulation intensity and limited effects of this stimulation on spinal mechanisms, as the other studies have found effects on the overall excitability using suprathreshold stimulation (Schabrun et al. 2012; Ridding and Rothwell 1999), contributed by spinal mechanisms (Claus et al. 1988a, b). In summary, subthreshold vibration appears to have affected M1 through a GABAergic mechanism.

Stimulation of the wrist affected intracortical inhibition for the thumb. Spillover effects of sensory stimulation on TMS measures for muscles that are not directly related to the stimulation site were seen in the literature: cutaneous nerve stimulation of the index finger decreased SICI for not only the first dorsal interosseous (FDI), but also the APB and abductor digiti minimi (ADM), and cutaneous nerve stimulation of the little finger decreased SICI not only for ADM but also for FDI and APB using ISI of 3 ms (used in the present study) (Ridding et al. 2005). This spillover effect may in part be explained by the vibration affecting the alpha rhythm that tends to be topographically diffused (Crone et al. 1998) and provides ancillary support such as attention and release of inhibition (Jensen and Mazaheri 2010; Anderson and Ding 2011), and may parallel GABAergic intracortical inhibition (Jensen and Mazaheri 2010). Similar observations were made in the literature in which imperceptible random-frequency peripheral stimulation influenced broad brain areas (Ward et al. 2010; Kitajo et al. 2007; Mendez-Balbuena et al. 2015) and led to behavioral changes beyond the site stimulated (Seo et al. 2015; Lugo et al. 2008). This diffusivity supports the previous reports on subthreshold wrist vibration’s effects on fingertip sensation (Enders et al. 2013; Lakshminarayanan et al. 2015) and hand motor function (Seo et al. 2014).

Effect of vibration on grip-related ERD

The subthreshold vibration not only changed the resting EEG power, but also increased the extent of brain activity for grip initiation and termination. This grip-specific change with vibration was additional to the change at rest, i.e., EEG power was reduced with vibration at rest, yet more reduction in power was observed during grip with vibration compared to without. This amplified ERD for grip with sensory stimulation may have been enabled by decrease in cortical inhibitory circuit activity (as seen by decreased SICI in the present study) along with reduction in top–down inhibition (Klimesch et al. 2007; Hummel et al. 2002) (as seen by decreased baseline alpha power) at rest. Greater alpha ERD with vibration may reflect increase in general excitability and supporting network activity (Crone et al. 1998; Jensen and Mazaheri 2010; Anderson and Ding 2011) for the grip task. Greater beta ERD with vibration may reflect increase in either motor planning/coordination activity or movement-induced afferent input (Alegre et al. 2003; Nakayashiki et al. 2014; Formaggio et al. 2015; Gross et al. 2005). On the flip side, a lack of afferent input due to pure somatosensory stroke has been shown to result in reduced ERD (Platz et al. 2000). Together, the present study results and literature suggest that somatosensory afference contributes to the release of inhibition for movement and sensorimotor activity.

Change over time

The potential of changing effects of the vibration over time was examined only in the EEG study. The EEG testing lasted for approximately 45 min. Baseline power increased from the first half to the second half of the EEG session, which may be associated with reduced attention for subjects over the testing duration. However, the effects of the stimulation on the EEG resting power and ERD were consistent over time, as it did not significantly alter from the first half to the second half of the testing session (with the interaction between vibration condition and session of p = 0.481 for resting power and p = 0.731 for ERD). Potential sensory habituation may have been minimized possibly by the random characteristics of the vibration frequency or breaks in between.

Implication

The result of this study may be relevant as this vibration is considered for use during rehabilitation and motor learning. The GABAergic circuits play a role in maintaining the boundaries of the cortical motor map and a decrease in local GABAergic activity is essential in motor cortical plasticity and reorganization after injury (Jacobs and Donoghue 1991; Bachtiar and Stagg 2014). For instance, less intracortical inhibition in the acute phase of stroke was associated with greater improvement in motor function many months after stroke (Huynh et al. 2014; Liuzzi et al. 2014). Similarly, motor skill learning has been shown to be accompanied by reduction of SICI, indicating its role in use-dependent plasticity (Coxon et al. 2014; Berghuis et al. 2015). In addition, a decrease in alpha power during motor learning was attributed to increased attention to the task with maximal readiness and information processing capacity (Zhuang et al. 1997). Thus, a reduction of SICI and greater ERD for grip with imperceptible wrist vibration may potentially prime the sensorimotor cortex to facilitate hand motor activity, skill learning, and reorganization. However, practical utility for rehabilitation or motor learning must be further investigated.

Conclusions

The present study showed that subthreshold random-frequency vibration applied to the wrist led to release of inhibition for the APB muscle in M1, suppression of resting alpha and beta rhythms, and increased grip-related ERD activity, indicating increased excitability/activity of the sensorimotor cortex. These effects may explain improvements in hand motor function using the stimulation in earlier studies. The effects are similar to those of the existing peripheral sensory stimulation. The specific modality using subthreshold wrist vibration can be mobile and cannot be detected, and, thus, may have advantages in motor learning or rehabilitation settings.