Introduction

Stress is both a predisposing, triggering factor and consequence of drug taking. Individuals with high levels of previous stress and adverse life events are more likely to engage in drug use, initiate drug use at an earlier age, transition from occasional use to compulsive drug addiction more quickly, and relapse (Sinha 2001). Preclinical studies have largely paralleled these findings in both nonhuman primates and rodents. In particular, a model of intermittent social defeat stress in rats engenders long-lasting neuroadaptations, resulting in faster acquisition of cocaine self-administration, greater responding under progressive ratio schedules of reinforcement, increased cocaine self-administration during a 24-h unlimited access “binge,” and heightened context-induced reinstatement after abstinence (Covington and Miczek 2001; Holly et al. 2016; Miczek and Mutschler 1996; Miczek et al. 2011; Tidey and Miczek 1997).

However, despite substantial evidence linking stress and compulsive drug taking and seeking, not all individuals exposed to high levels of stress engage in drug use. Similarly, there is considerable variability in cocaine self-administration patterns and levels of consumption in rats exposed to models of intermittent social defeat stress (Covington and Miczek 2005; Covington et al. 2008; Kabbaj et al. 2001; Shimamoto et al. 2015). Understanding the behavioral sources and underlying mechanisms of individual differences in augmented cocaine self-administration following stress may lead to earlier and more effective therapeutic interventions for vulnerable individuals.

A possible underlying basis for this variability in drug taking is individual differences in behavior during social stress. Wood et al. (2010) found that active vs. passive coping during social defeat (operationally defined by latency to submit to the resident) was predictive of subsequent resilience or vulnerability, respectively, to maladaptive behavioral and physiological effects of stress. However, social defeat encompasses a myriad of behaviors, such as approach to a threatening situation, reaction to a fight encounter, escape behavior, and return to a safe environment (Koolhaas et al. 2010). How each of these distinct behaviors relates to subsequent drug self-administration has yet to be explored. In the current experiment, we dissected these phases of social confrontation and examined resulting effects on stress-associated physiological and behavioral measures. We designed an apparatus which allowed us to assess rats for their latencies to engage in each component of social defeat stress: entering a threatening zone, engaging in a confrontation, submitting to social defeat, escape, and return to a safe zone. After nine encounters, rats were tested for behavioral cross-sensitization to cocaine and catheterized for cocaine self-administration, which culminated in a 24-h binge. As other factors, such as reactivity to novelty, saccharin preference, impulsivity, and resistance to punishment have also been correlated with individual differences in cocaine self-administration (Belin et al. 2008; Carroll et al. 2002; Deroche-Gamonet et al. 2004; Piazza et al. 1989), we also examined whether these measures were associated with defeat-related behaviors.

Additionally, we evaluated whether behaviors during defeat were associated with neurophysiological differences in brain regions associated with stress and drug reward. Chronic stress downregulates hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) transcription (Duclot and Kabbaj 2013; Haenisch et al. 2009; Komatsu et al. 2011; Patki et al. 2013; Zhang et al. 2015), with individual differences in chronic stress-induced changes in hippocampal BDNF and its receptor, tyrosine receptor kinase B (TrkB) (Duclot and Kabbaj 2013). Individual differences in the physiological response to stress may also contribute to individual differences in stress-escalated (as opposed to drug history-escalated) cocaine self-administration. Clinical work has shown that cortisol release in response to stress is positively correlated with positive feelings in response to amphetamine (Hamidovic et al. 2010). Corticotropin releasing factor (CRF) is a neuropeptide involved in the initiation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis stress response, but also has widespread extrahypothalamic sites of action (Swanson et al. 1983). In particular, CRF in the extended amygdala and mesocorticolimbic dopamine system has been strongly implicated in stress-induced psychiatric disorders, including drug addiction (Zorrilla et al. 2014). Therefore, we also examined whether hippocampal BDNF and TrkB transcript, CRF expression, or corticosterone levels were correlated with defeat behaviors.

Methods

General methods

Subjects

Male Long-Evans rats (Charles River Laboratories, Wilmington, MA, n = 213) weighing 225–250 g upon arrival were individually housed in custom-built acrylic chambers (30.5 × 30.5 × 24.5 cm) lined with Cellu-Dri™ bedding (Shepherd Specialty Papers, Kalamazoo, MI) and provided food and water ad libitum. Rats were housed on a reverse light cycle (lights on 2000–0800 hours), with controlled temperature (21 ± 1 °C) and humidity (30–60 %). All behavioral testing was conducted during the dark phase, and all experimental procedures were approved by the Tufts Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee following the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (National Research Council 2011).

Experimental design

In experiment 1, rats were first tested for baseline saccharin preference and open field activity, after which they were exposed to social defeat stress or control handling, with weekly measurements of saccharin preference and locomotion in the open field. Saccharin preference and open field activity were tested again 1 week after the last social defeat. Rats were then assessed for locomotor cross-sensitization to cocaine, after which rats were implanted with intravenous catheters and trained to self-administer cocaine. In experiment 2, rats underwent social defeat or handling, followed by Go/No-Go testing for impulsivity. In experiment 3, rats were assessed for BDNF and TrkB messenger RNA (mRNA) expression in the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus after the last social defeat, while experiment 4 measured CRF protein expression in the nucleus accumbens, amygdala, and ventral tegmental area. Finally, experiment 5 measured plasma corticosterone after the first, fifth, and ninth defeat. Experimental design is shown in Fig. 1a.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Experimental and apparatus design. a In the first experiment, rats were tested for baseline open field activity (O) and saccharin drinking (S), which continued weekly throughout the course of intermittent social defeat stress (defeats represented by gray bars). After the last defeat, rats were catheterized and allowed to self-administer cocaine. In the second experiment, separate rats were tested for impulsivity in the Go/No-Go task after social defeat stress. Physiological measures were taken in the final three experiments, with one cohort assessed for BDNF and TrkB mRNA, another for extrahypothalamic CRF content, and a final cohort tested for corticosterone at baseline and after the first, fifth, and ninth defeat. b Three-chamber apparatus permitting entry and escape for the intruder. 1, The intruder voluntarily leaves the safe zone (home cage) and enters the threat zone. 2, After 5 min in the threat zone, the intruder is placed into the resident’s compartment for social defat. 3, After ten bites, 8 s supine, or 5 min, the porthole is opened and the intruder allowed to escape back to the threat zone. 4 After 5 min, the porthole to the safe zone is opened and the intruder allowed to return to its home cage

Social defeat stress

Upon arrival, rats were randomly assigned to two groups: defeat stressed or non-defeated control. Stressed rats were subjected to nine intermittent social defeat episodes over 21 days in a modified version of our previously described procedure (Covington and Miczek 2001), while non-defeated controls were briefly handled. Defeats occurred in a modified resident home cage (Fig. 1b) consisting of two compartments (the resident’s home compartment or “fight zone” and a neutral compartment or “threat zone” with clean bedding) connected by a porthole, with a second porthole allowing access to the experimental rat’s home cage (“safe zone”). See Electronic supplementary material for additional details.

During each defeat, the experimental intruder’s home cage was moved adjacent to the resident cage (Fig. 1b), the porthole opened, and the rat allowed up to 5 min to voluntarily move to the threat zone. After 5 min in the threat zone, the intruder was placed into the fight zone with the resident with the escape porthole closed until it showed 8 s continuous supine posture, received ten attack bites, or 5 min had elapsed. A screen was then used to separate the aggressive resident from the intruder and the escape porthole opened. The rat was allowed up to 5 min to voluntarily move from the fight zone to the threat zone. After 5 min in the threat zone, the safe zone porthole was opened and the intruder allowed up to 5 min to voluntarily return to its home cage. If an intruder did not move from one compartment to another after 5 min, the rat was placed into the adjacent compartment by the experimenter and the porthole closed.

Experiment 1: analysis of behaviors predictive of increased cocaine self-administration after social defeat

The aim of experiment 1 was to elucidate which behaviors during social stress were predictive of later augmented cocaine self-administration during the binge. Additionally, as individual differences in both saccharin drinking and open field locomotor activity have been associated with increased psychostimulant self-administration, we also investigated whether these factors were predictive of stress-escalated cocaine self-administration. Design for experiment 1 is shown in Fig. 1a, and a timeline shown in Fig. 2a.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Social defeat stress induces behavioral cross-sensitization to cocaine and escalated cocaine self-administration during a 24-h “binge.” a Timeline for the cocaine self-administration methods. Animals were challenged with cocaine (10 mg/kg, ip) 10 days after the last social defeat to assess for behavioral cross-sensitization and were catheterized for intravenous cocaine self-administration the following day. After 5 days recovery, animals were initially allowed to self-administer cocaine (0.75 mg kg−1 infusion−1) on a fixed ratio 1 (FR1) schedule until they obtained the maximal 15 infusions in two consecutive days. The FR schedule was then gradually increased to FR5, followed by 5 days FR5 maintenance. Three progressive ratio (PR) sessions were then alternated with three FR sessions across 6 days, and the experiment culminated in a 24-h binge. b Stressed rats (n = 39) had a significantly greater walking frequency in response to cocaine compared with non-stressed controls (n = 43), indicative of behavioral cross-sensitization. c There were no differences between stressed and control rats in mean response rate (responses/min) across the last three FR5 maintenance sessions d nor were there any differences in median breakpoint (defined as infusions self-administered) across the three PR sessions. e Stressed rats self-administered significantly more cocaine across the 24-h binge compared with controls. Data shown are mean ± SEM; *p < 0.01 vs. control

Saccharin drinking and open field testing

Rats were randomly assigned to stressed (n = 39) or control (n = 43) groups and were tested for 0.02 % saccharin preference in a weekly 1-h two bottle choice procedure. Rats were also tested weekly for locomotor activity in an open field. Rats were removed from their homecage during the dark phase and placed in the center of a large open field (71 × 45 × 45 cm). After 30 min habituation, a novel object was placed into the arena and one 5-min video sample was recorded for novel object exploration. Total distance traveled throughout the habituation and novel object exploration was measured by Ethovision. Baseline measurements were performed 1 week prior to the social stress phase. Further testing occurred once per week throughout the stress period and 1 week after stress exposure (for additional details, see Electronic supplementary material).

Locomotor cross-sensitization to cocaine

Ten days after the final social defeat episode (day 31), rats were tested for cross-sensitization to acute cocaine challenge (Boyson et al. 2014; Covington and Miczek 2001).

Intravenous cocaine self-administration

Methods for cocaine self-administration have been previously described (Boyson et al. 2014; Holly et al. 2016). Timeline for self-administration methods is shown in Fig. 2a and additional details in Electronic supplementary material.

Rats were permanently implanted with an indwelling catheter (Silastic® silicon tubing, ID 0.63 mm, OD 1.17 mm) into the right jugular vein under ketamine (100 mg/kg) and xylazine (6 mg/kg) anesthesia. Rats were allowed to recover for at least 5 days, then were moved from their home cage to permanent housing inside cocaine self-administration chambers.

Rats were initially allowed to self-administer cocaine (0.75 mg kg−1 infusion−1, administered 0.15 ml/kg at 0.018 ml/s) without priming or autoshaping on a fixed ratio 1 (FR1) schedule of reinforcement, followed by a 30-s timeout. Each daily session terminated after 15 infusions or 5 h. Once animals demonstrated reliable, stable responding at FR1, operationally defined as two consecutive days of maximal responding (15 infusions), the schedule was gradually advanced to FR5 to ensure rats were reliably performing for cocaine. Rats were maintained on a limited access FR5 schedule for at least five consecutive days. Rats were then subjected to three progressive ratio (PR) sessions (0.3 mg kg−1 infusion−1), alternated with three FR5 sessions (0.75 mg kg−1 infusion−1) across 6 days. The day after the final FR5 maintenance session, rats were given unlimited access to cocaine (0.3 mg kg−1 infusion−1) for 24 h.

Experiment 2: Go/No-Go task

The aim of experiment 2 was to assess whether individual differences in impulsivity could explain the individual differences in behavior during social defeat stress. A separate cohort of rats underwent social defeat (n = 21) or control handling (n = 11) as described above and was subsequently tested using a modified version of the Go/No-Go task (Helms et al. 2008).

Each session of the Go/No-Go task ended after 60 trials. For each trial of the Go/No-Go task, there was a variable precue period (9–24 s), signaled by the house light, and responses were recorded but not reinforced. During the initial training phase, the precue period was followed by a 30-s “Go” cue (constant light above the Go hole). When the Go cue was displayed, the first nosepoke response resulted in the delivery of 0.07 ml 0.02 % saccharin. After rats successfully completed >30 trials within 1 h for four consecutive sessions, the cue duration was reduced from 30 to 10 s. After completion of >30 trials within 1 h for four consecutive sessions at 10 s, the cue light was reduced to 5 s.

After stable responding was again demonstrated, the “No-Go” cue (flashing cue light of a different color above the opposite nosepoke) was introduced on five consecutive sessions. When the No-Go cue was presented, rats must inhibit nosepoke responding during the cue. A nosepoke response (false alarm) terminated the trial and no saccharin was delivered. Go and No-Go trials were randomly ordered, with Go trials occurring 75 % of the time. After either trial type, a 10-s intertrial period occurred with all lights off and no reward available.

Experiment 3: BDNF and TrkB mRNA measurement

The aim of experiment 3 was to determine whether altered BDNF signaling in the hippocampus or prefrontal cortex (PFC) was an underlying mechanism behind individual differences in social defeat behavior. One hour after the last defeat, a separate group of rats (n = 20 control, n = 20 stressed) were anesthetized with isofluorane and rapidly decapitated for quantification of BDNF and TrkB mRNA through quantitative reverse transcription polymerase chain reaction (qRT-PCR, see Electronic supplementary material).

Experiment 4: CRF measurement

The aim of experiment 4 was to evaluate whether individual differences in social defeat behavior resulted in altered CRF expression in limbic regions associated with stress and reward. A separate cohort of rats (n = 12 control, n = 12 stressed) was rapidly decapitated under isofluorane anesthesia 1 h after the final defeat to measure extrahypothalamic CRF content using a commercially available enzyme immune assay kit (EIA; Peninsula Laboratories, San Carlos, CA, see Electronic supplementary material).

Experiment 5: corticosterone measurement

Finally, experiment 5 aimed to determine whether individual differences in stress-escalated cocaine self-administration could be simply explained by differences in the physiological response to social defeat. A separate cohort of rats (control n = 16, stressed n = 18) was used to measure plasma corticosterone at baseline and 20 min following social defeat on days 1, 10, and 21. Blood was collected via tail vein puncture and centrifuged to obtain plasma. Corticosterone concentration (ng/ml) was determined using a commercially available EIA kit (Arbor Assays Detect X, Ann Arbor, MI). Standard curve range was 78.125–10000 pg/ml.

Statistical analysis

A principle component factor analysis was used to determine the relationship between specific behaviors during social defeat stress and subsequent cocaine self-administration (SAS, SAS Institute). To assess behaviors during aggressive encounters, the average latencies for the slow and fast groups were collapsed across the nine encounters and then were followed by a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). A two-way (group × time) repeated measures ANOVA was used to assess differences in body weight, saccharin drinking, and locomotor activity. Locomotor sensitization and IV cocaine self-administration were assessed by a one-way ANOVA. All post-hoc tests were analyzed by Holm-Sidak corrections for multiple comparisons. In experiment 2, a two-way (group × time) repeated measures ANOVA was used to assess precue, go hits, and false alarms (Sigma Plot version 11.0, Systat Software). All post-hoc tests were analyzed with Holm-Sidak corrections for multiple comparisons. In experiments 3–5, one-way ANOVAs were used to assess total BDNF and TrkB receptor mRNA and CRF content, while a two-way (group × time) repeated measures ANOVA was used to determine corticosterone secretion. A linear regression was used for correlational analyses of BDNF and average latency to enter into a threat zone as well as TrkB receptor mRNA and average latency to enter into a threat zone. All post-hoc tests were analyzed by Holm-Sidak corrections for multiple comparisons.

Results

Experiment 1: analysis of behaviors predictive of increased cocaine self-administration after social defeat

Rats with a history of social defeat stress showed behavioral cross-sensitization to cocaine, as measured by walking frequency (Student’s t = −2.826, df = 83, p = 0.006, Fig. 2b) and self-administered significantly more cocaine under 24 h unlimited access binge condition (Student’s t = −2.328, df = 72, p = 0.023, Fig. 2e). FR response rate (Fig. 2c) and PR breakpoint (Fig. 2d) were not affected by stress history.

Principal component analysis: latency to enter threat zone is predictive of stress-escalated cocaine self-administration

We next assessed whether any behaviors during social defeat (latency to enter the threat zone, fight duration, latency to leave fight, return to safe zone) were predictive of subsequent cocaine self-administration behavior (progressive ratio breakpoint, rate of fixed ratio maintenance, total binge infusions, binge duration). Principal component analysis (PCA) revealed two factors with an eigenvalue >1 (Fig. 3a). Five behavioral elements loaded onto factor 1; four of these behaviors were related to cocaine self-administration, and only one was related to the social defeat procedure (r = −0.45–0.90). The three other social defeat-related behaviors loaded onto separate factor with minimal cross-loading (r = 0.41–0.84). Based on the results from the PCA, latency to enter the threat zone was then used as a predictive factor to assess all subsequent behavioral and neurophysiological results. Differences in latency to enter the threatening zone emerged by the second defeat (Fig. 3b). To enhance contrast between groups, the upper and lower tails of the distribution for the latency measure were used to separate socially defeated rats, with the lower 33rd percentile termed “fast,” and upper 33rd percentile termed “slow.” The middle 33 % were removed from all further analyses (Fig. 3c).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Principle component analysis (PCA) reveals latency to enter threat zone is correlated with cocaine self-administration behavior. a Two factors were extracted from the PCA; factor 1, on the x-axis, accounts for 50.1 % of the total variance, while factor 2, on the y-axis, accounts for 30.8 % of the total variance. Latency to enter the threat zone was the only behavioral component of social defeat that loaded onto the factor with cocaine self-administration variables. b Latency to enter the threat zone significantly differed between “fast” (black, n = 13) and “slow” (gray, n = 13) rats across the nine social defeats, and group differences emerged by the second defeat. c Mean latency to enter the threat zone across nine social defeat encounters was a continuous distribution, and with the upper 1/3 (n = 13) of the distribution considered slow and the lower 1/3 (n = 13) of the distribution considered fast. Data shown are mean ± SEM. ***p < 0.001

Individual differences in latency to enter threat zone did not affect other defeat behavior

The fast rats had significantly lower latencies to enter the threat zone compared with the slow rats (Student’s t = 14.328, df = 24, p < 0.001), however, these groups did not differ in fight duration, latency to escape the fight, or latency to return to the safe zone (Table 1). Furthermore, there were no significant differences between fast and slow rats on specific behaviors before, during, or after the fight period (see Electronic supplementary material).

Table 1 Latencies of behaviors in “slow” (bottom 33 %, n = 13) and “fast” (top 33 %, n = 13) rats

Individual differences in latency to enter the threat zone do not affect body weight gain, saccharin preference, and open field activity

Body weight

Overall, in regard to body weight (g, Fig. 4a), there was a significant main effect of week (two-way repeated measures ANOVA F 4, 264 = 516.630, p < 0.001), with a significant main effect of group (control, fast, and slow, F 2, 66 = 3.874, p = 0.026) and week × group interaction (F 8, 264 = 5.689, p < 0.001). Post-hoc analyses revealed no significant group differences in body weight at baseline. When collapsed across all weeks tested (baseline, weeks 1–3 of stress, and 1 week post-stress), fast rats weighed significantly less than both controls (Holm-Sidak t = 2.575, p = 0.036) and slow stressed rats (Holm-Sidak t = 2.425, p = 0.036). However, when data were normalized to percent of baseline for each rat, there were no significant differences in weight gain across groups (Fig. 4b).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Latency to enter the threat zone does not affect body weight gain, saccharin preference, or open field activity. a Body weight (g) increased in all groups from baseline (BL), through the 3 weeks of social defeat stress, to 1 week post-stress (PS), although overall, “fast” (black circles, n = 13) rats weighed significantly less than “slow” (gray circles, n = 13) and control (white circles, n = 43) rats (*p < 0.05). b Percent change in body weight from baseline (%BL) did not differ between groups. c Saccharin intake (mg/kg body weight) increased from BL to the 1 week PS in all groups, but no differences in saccharin consumption between groups were observed. d Saccharin preference (%, saccharin intake/(saccharin intake + water intake) also did not differ between groups. e On the first day of baseline open field testing, distance traveled (cm) in 5 min bins decreased across the 30-min habituation period but did not differ between groups. Insertion of a novel object (N) into the open field caused increased locomotor activity, but distance traveled did not differ between groups. f Average locomotor activity at BL, during each of the 3 weeks of stress, or PS did not differ across groups. Data shown are mean ± SEM

Saccharin drinking

There were no significant differences between fast- and slow-defeated rats and non-stressed controls in saccharin preference (Fig. 4d). However, there was a significant main effect of day for total saccharin intake (two-way repeated measures ANOVA F 1, 62 = 24.118, p < 0.001, Fig. 4c), suggesting all groups increased total consumption across time.

Open field

There was a significant main effect of time on total locomotor activity in an open field during the first session of testing (“baseline”, two-way repeated measures ANOVA F 5, 283 = 124.673, p < 0.001), but no effect of group (Fig. 4e). There was a significant main effect of day on total distance traveled across the five weeks of testing (Two-way repeated measures ANOVA F 4, 231 = 3.350, p = 0.011; Fig. 4f), suggesting no locomotor activity deficits in slow rats.

Individual differences in latency to enter the threat zone affect cocaine self-administration

Cross-sensitization to cocaine

Escapable intermittent social defeat stress increased locomotor activity in response to an acute cocaine challenge. There was a significant main effect of group (control, fast, and slow, one-way ANOVA F 2, 64 = 3.846, p = 0.026, Fig. 5a), with stressed groups exhibiting significantly increased walking frequency in response to cocaine compared with controls (Holm-Sidak t = 2.715, p = 0.008).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Latency to enter the threat zone was predictive of cocaine self-administration. a Both “fast” (n = 13) and “slow” (n = 13) rats showed stress-induced cross-sensitization to cocaine compared with controls (n = 43) as assessed by walking frequency after cocaine challenge (10 mg/kg, ip). b Rate of cocaine self-administration (responses/min) during fixed ratio (FR) was lower in slow rats compared with fast rats. c There was no effect of group on breakpoint (number of infusions obtained) in a progressive ratio (PR) schedule of reinforcement. d Cumulative cocaine infusions self-administered across a 24-h period was significantly lower in slow rats compared with fast rats (left) with fast rats self-administering significantly more total cocaine infusions compared with both slow rats and non-defeated controls (right). Data shown are mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05 vs. non-defeated controls; # p < 0.05 vs. “fast” rats

Acquisition/maintenance

There were no significant differences in acquisition among the fast, slow, and control groups. However, once rats acquired self-administration, there was a significant main effect of group on the average rate of cocaine self-administration during the last 3 days of maintenance (one-way ANOVA F 2, 65 = 3.896, p = 0.025). Post-hoc analyses revealed a significantly lower response rate in the slow-defeated rats compared with the fast-defeated rats (Holm-Sidak t = 2.544, p = 0.039; Fig. 5b) and non-defeated controls (Holm-Sidak t = 2.475, p = 0.032).

Progressive ratio

There were no significant differences among the fast, slow, or non-defeated control rats on measures of motivation as assessed by total cocaine infusions (breakpoints) under a PR schedule of reinforcement (Fig. 5c).

Twenty-four-hour binge

There was a significant main effect of group (one-way ANOVA F 2, 62 = 5.117, p = 0.009) on the number of cocaine infusions self-administered during a 24-h continuous access binge. Post-hoc analyses fast rats self-administered significantly more infusions than both the slow-defeated rats and non-defeated controls (Holm-Sidak t = 2.766, p = 0.015; t = 2.965, p = 0.013, respectively; Fig. 5d).

Experiment 2: Go/No-Go task

Individual differences in latency to enter the threat zone do not affect impulsivity as measured by the Go/No-Go task

No significant differences between the fast, slow, and non-defeated control groups were found in any of the impulsive-like measures in the Go/No-Go task (precue responses, hits, and false alarms), suggesting that fast latency to enter the threat zone is not indicative of motor impulsivity (Fig. 6).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Latency to enter the threat zone did not significantly affect impulsivity as measured by the Go/No-Go task. a Precue responses during training (days 1–15) as well as No-Go testing (shaded box, days 16–20) for fast (black circles, n = 7), slow (gray circles, n = 7), and non-defeated controls (white circles, n = 11) did not significantly differ. b Go “hits” during the training phase (days 1–15) and Go/No-Go phase (days 16–20, gray box), did not significantly differ between groups. c False alarms during the Go/No-Go phase (days 16–20) also did not differ between groups. Data shown are mean ± SEM

Experiment 3: BDNF and TrkB mRNA measurement

Individual differences in latency to enter the threat zone is associated with altered hippocampal BDNF and TrkB mRNA transcription

There was no significant effect of group on BDNF or TrkB mRNA in the prefrontal cortex, but there was a significant effect of group on BDNF and TrkB mRNA within the hippocampus. Hippocampal BDNF was significantly affected by group (one-way ANOVA F 2, 29 = 4.178, p = 0.025), with fast rats expressing significantly more BDNF mRNA in the hippocampus relative to slow rats (p = 0.023). Similarly, hippocampal TrkB mRNA was affected by group (F 2, 29 = 3.534, p = 0.042), with fast rats again expressing significantly more hippocampal TrkB mRNA relative to slow rats (p = 0.037). There was a significant negative correlation between average latency to enter the threat zone and hippocampal BDNF (r = 0.621, p = 0.003) and TrkB (0.660, p = 0.002) mRNA (Fig. 7).

Fig. 7
figure 7

Latency to enter the threat zone was associated with altered hippocampal BDNF and TrkB mRNA. a “Fast” (n = 7, black) rats showed significantly greater hippocampal BDNF mRNA expression compared with “slow” (n = 7, gray) rats and non-defeated controls (n = 20, white), b resulting in a significant negative correlation between latency to enter the threat zone and hippocampal BDNF mRNA (intermediate 1/3 (n = 7) of defeated rats shown as gray triangles). c Similarly, fast rats showed significantly greater hippocampal TrkB mRNA expression compared with slow rats, d which also resulted in a significant negative correlation between latency to enter threat zone and hippocampal TrkB mRNA. Data shown are mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05 vs. slow

Experiments 4 and 5: CRF and corticosterone measurement

Individual differences in latency to enter the threat zone do not affect the physiological stress response

Corticosterone

Social stress resulted in a group x day interaction in plasma corticosterone 20 min after defeat (F 3, 94 = 5.93, p < 0.001). This difference appears to be a result of increased corticosterone in stressed rats limited to Day 1. Fast and slow rats did not differ in the corticosterone response to stress (Fig. 8a).

Fig. 8
figure 8

Latency to enter the threat zone was not associated with altered physiological responses to stress. a Plasma corticosterone (CORT) did not differ between groups at baseline (BL), and while there was a significant effect of stress on the first day of defeat (D1), there was no difference between “slow” (n = 6, gray) and “fast” (n = 6, black) groups (control n = 16, white). There was no effect of stress on plasma corticosterone on day 10 or 21. b Total CRF content (pg/mg protein) in the nucleus accumbens (NAc), amygdala (AMY), and ventral tegmental area (VTA) also did not differ between groups (control n = 12, fast n = 4, slow n = 4). Data shown are mean ± SEM

Extrahypothalamic CRF

There was no effect of group on CRF content in the nucleus accumbens (NAc), amygdala, or ventral tegmental area (VTA). However, there was a main effect of brain region regardless of group (two-way repeated measures ANOVA F 2, 33 = 22.262, p < 0.001), with significantly more CRF in the amygdala and VTA compared with the NAc (Holm-Sidak t = 6.488, p < 0.001; t = 4.580, p < 0.001, respectively; Fig. 8b).

Discussion

Through dissecting the cluster of behaviors involved in social defeat stress, we have shown that latency to enter into a threatening zone is highly predictive of the subsequent development of stress-escalated cocaine self-administration. Rats with a short latency to enter a threatening environment (“fast” group) self-administer cocaine at a higher rate and accumulate more cocaine infusions during a 24-h binge compared with rats with a long latency to enter the threatening environment (“slow” group). Importantly, separating animals based upon this measure did not result in significant differences in saccharin preference, open field locomotor activity, or motor impulsivity, all of which have been linked with increased rates of psychostimulant self-administration (Belin et al. 2008; Carroll et al. 2002; Piazza et al. 1989). This indicates that latency to enter a threatening environment is a distinct individual difference in behavior related to escalated self-administration. Furthermore, differences in cocaine self-administration between fast and slow rats are not the result of altered behavioral (immobility, reaction to attack bite) or physiological (CRF, corticosterone) responses to social defeat stress. Finally, we demonstrate that fast and slow rats show altered BDNF and TrkB transcription in the hippocampus, which may be an underlying mechanism for the persistent neuroadaptations engendered by social defeat stress which promote escalated cocaine self-administration.

Individual differences in factors that promote enhanced cocaine self-administration in rodents have received considerable attention. Of note, high preference for and locomotor reactivity to a novel environment, preference for sucrose, and impulsivity have all been associated with increased psychostimulant self-administration (Belin et al. 2008; Carroll et al. 2002; Piazza et al. 1989). The current series of experiments complement this growing literature, indicating that a consistent decision to rapidly and voluntarily enter into a threatening social environment, despite known consequences, is also highly predictive of intensified cocaine self-administration. Differences between rats fast and slow to approach social threat emerge by the second social defeat encounter and are sustained through the 21 days of social stress. Importantly, this maladaptive choice by some stressed rats is distinct from other previously studied innate traits associated with subsequent maladaptive behavior.

As a motor behavior, rapid approach to social threat could be function of high-responder (HR) and low-responder (LR) rats, which has been established as a highly predictive trait for faster acquisition of psychostimulant self-administration (Kabbaj et al. 2001; Piazza et al. 1989). Innate traits, such as HR or increased locomotor behavior in a novel open field environment, are highly associated with acquisition rates of cocaine self-administration, although there have been no reported effects on other measures of psychostimulant self-administration (Bardo et al. 2013; Blanchard et al. 2009; Piazza et al. 1989). Although the current study used different methods from previous HR/LR experiments, fast and slow rats showed no significant differences in reactivity to a novel open field and novel object exploration, as assessed by total distance traveled. This suggests that latency to enter a threatening environment may be a distinct trait from HR and LR phenotypes, although future work should assess this in a circular arena using methods initially promoted by Piazza et al. (1989). Importantly, however, differences in general locomotor behavior and locomotor activity in response to a novel object cannot explain differences in latency to enter the threat zone exhibited by fast and slow rats.

An alternative explanation for the behavioral differences observed between fast and slow rats is individual differences in motor impulsivity, also established as a predisposing factor for the transition from initial cocaine self-administration to escalated, compulsive self-administration (Belin et al. 2008; Murray et al. 2014). However, following social defeat stress, fast and slow rats did not differ in impulsivity from non-stressed controls in the Go/No-Go task. In contrast to motoric impulsivity, fast and slow rats may be exhibiting increased and decreased levels of social impulsivity, as exhibited by consistent low or high latency to approach a novel stimulus animal despite possible negative consequences.

Furthermore, we have demonstrated that fast and slow rats do not differ in two physiological measures of responsivity to social defeat stress, namely corticosterone secretion and extrahypothalamic CRF concentration. Although these physiological measures are not necessarily direct correlates of the amount of stress the animals experience and the single time point measurement cannot rule out differences in HPA axis negative feedback, they indicate that fast and slow rats may generally experience the same level of stress. This is significant because it precludes the possibility that fast rats self-administer more cocaine due to an increased physiological stress response and points to the important role of other factors, such as decision-making, altered cocaine responsivity, or altered hippocampal BDNF/TrkB signaling.

Latency to enter a threatening zone was positively correlated with hippocampal BDNF and TrkB (Fig. 5). This is consistent with other work demonstrating individual differences are regulated by hippocampal BDNF signaling. Greater hippocampal BDNF is associated with the promotion of resilience to anhedonia-like symptoms induced by chronic stress (Bergstrom et al. 2008; Duclot and Kabbaj 2013; Taliaz et al. 2011). Duclot and Kabbaj (2013) report that altered BDNF signaling is a key mechanism underlying individual differences in vulnerability to chronic stress; enhanced hippocampal BDNF signaling promotes resilience, whereas disruption promotes vulnerability to chronic social defeat-induced anhedonia-like behavior.

In the present series of experiments, as opposed to investigating individual differences in anhedonic-like behavior after chronic stress, we focused on individual differences in hedonic-like behavior (i.e., cocaine self-administration) following intermittent social defeat, which does not induce an anhedonic-like phenotype (Miczek et al. 2011). We demonstrate that BDNF and TrkB transcription is altered within the slow group, resilient to the hedonic-promoting effects of intermittent social defeat, but remains equivalent to controls in the fast or vulnerable group. BDNF/TrkB signaling may be functioning similarly to promote resilience to the divergent behavioral effects of chronic and intermittent stress. BDNF facilitates synaptic plasticity and enhances postsynaptic responsivity (Cordeira et al. 2010; Fortin et al. 2012; Madara and Levine 2008; Park and Poo 2013), so enhanced hippocampal BDNF/TrkB signaling observed in “resilient” rats may promote synaptic and behavioral adaptations after stress. We initially investigated hippocampal BDNF due to the well-described role of hippocampal plasticity in response to stress and notable glucocorticoid sensitivity (McEwen 1999), but it will be important to evaluate BDNF and TrkB in other limbic brain regions associated with drug self-administration, particularly the VTA and NAc. Additionally, in the current experimental design, it is unclear whether BDNF signaling is causal or resultant of individual differences in defeat, as individual differences in latency to enter the threatening zone by definition cannot be ascertained until after defeat experience.

This is the first study to our knowledge to thoroughly explore the full repertoire of specific behaviors involved in social defeat stress and link them with later cocaine self-administration. Understanding behavioral indices of stress associated with later maladaptive behaviors, as well as neurobiological differences between vulnerable and resilient subjects, may lead to more targeted and effective therapeutic interventions for stress-related disorders, such as addiction or depression. Ultimately, we propose that individual differences in latency to enter into a threatening environment are highly predictive of subsequent stress-related maladaptive behavior. Future work will continue to explore how this particular aspect of social defeat stress plays a role in escalated cocaine self-administration.