Introduction

Schizophrenia, a disease affecting approximately 1 % of the global population (Jablensky 1997), is characterized by subtle neurological abnormalities that result in disordered perception, cognition, and emotion. Schizophrenia is debilitating, often requiring short term and sometimes permanent hospitalization or institutional living. Of psychiatric inpatients and outpatients with a diagnosis of schizophrenia, up to 20 % (de Leon et al. 1994; Iftene et al. 2013) have been observed to also have primary polydipsia, i.e., either chronic or intermittent excessive drinking without homeostatic or physiologic cause. Comorbid polydipsia can have clinically significant physical complications (Illowsky and Kirch 1988) and continually ingesting large amounts of fluid increases mortality in this population (Hawken et al. 2009).

The pathophysiology underlying polydipsia associated with schizophrenia is largely unknown but dopamine (DA) has been implicated. The role for DA in the etiology of psychosis, a clinical hallmark of schizophrenia, is primarily based on evidence that dopamine D2 receptors are the pharmacological target of all antipsychotic drug treatments (Seeman et al. 1975; Meltzer and Stahl 1976; Seeman 1987; Nord and Farde 2011). Secondly, DA direct and indirect agonists (e.g., amphetamine [AMPH]) can produce symptoms that mimic an acute psychotic state in nonschizophrenic people and exacerbate positive symptoms in individuals with schizophrenia (Angrist and Gershon 1970; Angrist et al. 1974; Lieberman et al. 1987; Friedman and Sienkiewicz 1991). Recent evidence for a relationship between observed hyperactivity of subcortical DA pathways and schizophrenia comes from human imaging studies (Laruelle et al. 1996, 1999; Laruelle 2000; Abi-Dargham et al. 2009; Kegeles et al. 2010). It has been postulated that it is this hyperdopaminergia of schizophrenia that facilitates excessive drinking in polydipsic patients (Illowsky and Kirch 1988; Fukunaka et al. 2007). The evidence comes in part from reports of an associated increase in polydipsic behaviors during psychotic episodes that then abate when the psychosis remits (for reviews, see Illowsky and Kirch 1988; Mittleman et al. 1994). More direct evidence comes from Matsumoto et al. (2005) who demonstrated that polymorphisms in the D2 receptor gene confer susceptibility to polydipsia in schizophrenia. Increases in levels of antipsychotic medication or adding the atypical antipsychotic clozapine (a “broad-spectrum” antagonist; Naheed and Green 2001) may reduce symptom severity as well as the incidence of polydipsia (Zink et al. 2004; for review, see Bersani et al. 2007). Together, this suggests a similar mechanism may underlie psychosis and polydipsia (Raskind et al. 1975; Smith and Clark 1980; Illowsky and Kirch 1988).

In rodents, drinking is in part modulated by DA neurotransmission: intermittent administration of DA-releasing drugs like AMPH or the D2-like receptor agonist quinpirole increases drinking in the hours following injection (Rowland et al. 1981; Fraioli et al. 1997; Cioli et al. 2000). Animals pretreated subchronically with N-methyl-d-aspartate glutamate receptor antagonist MK-801 have also been reported to show spontaneous polydipsia (Hawken et al. 2013a). Excessive drinking behavior in rats has also been examined in a schedule-induced polydipsia (SIP) protocol, where food-restricted animals will drink large quantities of water when presented with intermittent food delivery (Falk 1961). Integrity of the DA systems is implicated in the development of this “adjunctive” behavior (Robbins and Koob 1980; Wallace et al. 1983; Weissenborn et al. 1996); both D1- and D2-like receptor agonists and antagonists disrupt the acquisition of SIP (Todd et al. 1992; for review, see Mittleman et al. 1994). Furthermore, animals that develop SIP show increased DA turnover (Hooks et al. 1994), and increased D2-like and decreased D1-like receptor binding throughout the nucleus accumbens, medial prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and ventral tegmental areas (Pellón et al. 2011) suggesting that an imbalance of DA receptor activation may facilitate SIP behavior.

Repeated AMPH treatment, termed “AMPH sensitization”, followed by a period of AMPH withdrawal produces a chronic and self-sustaining hyperdopaminergic state in rodents (for review, see Robinson and Becker 1986; Paulson and Robinson 1995; Lodge and Grace 2012) that may model some of the neurochemical and behavioral abnormalities associated with the psychotic aspects of schizophrenia (Akbarian et al. 1995; Peleg-Raibstein et al. 2008; for reviews, see Tenn et al. 2003; Yin et al. 2012). Here, we tested the hypothesis that an AMPH sensitization model of schizophrenia-like symptoms augments SIP in rats. We found (like Mittleman and Vallenstein 1985) following AMPH sensitization SIP behavior increased. Additionally, we demonstrated that SIP behavior may, in part, be a learned phenomenon.

Methods

Subjects

Twenty-four male Sprague–Dawley rats weighing 200–225 g (Charles River, QC, Canada) were housed separately in clear Plexiglas cages (45 × 23 × 20 cm deep). The floors were lined with bedding (Beta Chip, NEPCO, Warrenburg, NY, USA) and the cages were located in a climate-controlled colony room (21 ± 1 °C; humidity, 40–70 %) on a reversed 12-h light/dark schedule (lights off at 0700 hours). Animals initially had free access to both food (LabDiet rodent feed #5001,PMI Nutrition International, Brentwood, MO, USA) and water but were later food restricted (see next section). Rats were treated in accordance with the guidelines of the Canadian Council on Animal Care, and the Queen's University Animal Care Committee approved the experimental protocol.

Drug treatment

One week following arrival in the facility, animals were randomly assigned to either receive once daily intraperitoneal injections of d-amphetamine sulfate (Sigma, Oakville, ON, Canada) dissolved in saline at a dose of 1.5 mg/kg or saline (1 ml/kg) for five consecutive days. A 28-day washout period followed the last injection. During this time, animals were food (but not water) restricted, i.e., they were allowed free access to food for 2 h per day. Food restriction was continued for the duration of the study.

Apparatus

Four commercially built (Med Associate Inc., St. Albans, VT, USA) experimental chambers (30.5 × 24.1 × 21 cm) were housed inside sound-attenuating cabinets that contained an electric fan for ventilation that ran throughout the experiment. The boxes were made of polycarbonate with aluminum panels and steel grid flooring. A light was illuminated in the box for the duration of the session. Each reward consisted of one 45-mg dustless precision food pellet (Bio-serv, Frenchtown, NJ, USA), released into the recessed dispenser tray (5 × 8 × 4 cm). On the opposite wall of the pellet dispenser and sitting between two inactive levers was a metal drinking spout containing a ball bearing. The drinking spout was freely available the entire session. A photo beam sensor was positioned across the mouth of the drinking spout to measure head entries. Inactive levers recorded lever presses. The experimental chambers were controlled by a computer running MED-PC IV (Med Associates Inc.).

Behavioral testing

All animals completed 24 days of testing. Prior to each testing session, 22-h home-cage drinking (by weighing home-cage water bottles) and animal weights were recorded. For the first two 2-h testing sessions, all animals received 120 pellets freely available in a dish at the start of the session (free feed [FF]). On day 3, experimental animals (AMPH, n = 6; saline, n = 6) were then tested: for each session, the test box automatically dispensed a pellet every min for a total of 120 pellets over 120 min. Control animals (AMPH, n = 6; saline, n = 6) did not receive a scheduled delivery of pellets but instead had FF. On day 24, all groups again received FF. Animals were tested in squads of four in daily morning and afternoon sessions. The amount of water consumed in each test session was recorded by measuring the before and after weights of the water bottles. Following testing, the animals were returned to their home cages and allowed free access to food for 2 h.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were performed using SPSS version 17.0 (Chicago, IL, USA). Control data were used to determine if the SIP protocol was successfully established. Animals were deemed to have developed SIP when they consumed at least 15 ml of water per 2-h session for three consecutive days (Hawken et al. 2011). Amount of water drunk during the testing sessions was also compared to 22-h home-cage water consumption (percent of home-cage drinking). Drinking more in the 2-h session than that consumed daily is a reported definition of polydipsia (Flory 1971). Acquisition (testing days, 3–23) and expression (testing days, 2 and 24) were analyzed between the protocol (experimental × control) and the treatment (AMPH × saline) conditions across days using repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). Tests of simple effects were used to examine significant interactions. For repeated measures ANOVA, trend analyses (contrasts) were performed. Neither sphericity of variance nor a significant main effect of the within-subject variable is an assumption of running trend analysis (Glass and Hopkins 1996).

Results

SIP acquisition

The AMPH experimental groups that received intermittent food presentation drank substantially more during daily 2-h sessions than the other groups; there was some evidence of a greater increase in drinking in the saline experimental group in later sessions (Fig. 1; three-way ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of day (F[1, 20] = 10.9, p = 0.004), day × protocol interaction (F[1, 20] = 4.74, p = 0.042), treatment (F[1, 20] = 11.7, p = 0.003), protocol (F[1, 20] = 14.7, p = 0.001), and a significant treatment x protocol interaction (F[1, 20] = 9.08, p = 0.007)). Two-way ANOVA examining the within-protocol effects of treatment on daily session drinking revealed significantly more drinking in the AMPH group compared to the saline controls within the experimental protocol (main effect of day (F[1, 10] = 8.47, p = 0.016) and treatment (F[1, 10] = 14.0, p = 0.004)). The AMPH control group failed to drink more than the saline control group (Fig. 1). AMPH-treated animals in the experimental protocol gradually increased drinking over the days.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Five injections of amphetamine (AMPH) given weeks earlier significantly increased drinking in the schedule-induced polydipsia protocol across days. Daily mean (±SEM) water drinking (in milliliter) during the 2-h testing session for AMPH and saline groups in experimental and control protocols are shown. Free feeding (FF) sessions demonstrated AMPH-treated animals learned to drink more in the SIP protocol. *ANOVA revealed a significant effect of AMPH (p < 0.05)

Animals in the experimental protocol increased weight over the days (F[1, 10] = 83.0, p < 0.001), but weight gain across the two groups (AMPH and saline) was not significantly different (data not shown). Thus, increased drinking was not due to differences in body weight.

In the experimental protocol, two rats in the AMPH group developed SIP (three consecutive days consuming at least 15 ml) and two more nearly developed SIP (two consecutive days consuming at least 15 ml), compared to none of the saline-treated rats. At the end of the 21 days of testing, two of the six AMPH-treated rats drank more in the daily 2-h drinking sessions than they drank in 22-h in their home cages. None of the rats in the saline group achieved this. Control animals not subjected to the 1-min schedule of pellet delivery did not develop SIP. That is, control animals failed to drink more than 15 ml on three consecutive days and also failed on any day to drink more than their daily 22-h water consumption.

Home-cage drinking increased over days for all rats (Fig. 2; F[1, 20] = 40.3, p < 0.001) and was not different between protocols or drug conditions. When the amount of water consumed in each daily testing session was expressed as a percentage of home-cage drinking (data not shown), only the AMPH treatment group in the experimental protocol showed an across-day pattern of drinking a larger proportion of their daily intake during testing sessions (three-way ANOVA showed a significant day × protocol interaction (F[1, 20] = 6.09, p = 0.023) and significant effects for protocol (F[1, 20] = 12.9, p = 0.002), treatment (F[1, 20] = 8.67, p = 0.008), and a protocol × treatment interaction (F[1, 20] = 7.30, p = 0.014)). Two-way ANOVA confirmed the treatment effect was largest for the AMPH-treated rats in the experimental protocol (significant main effect of day (F[1, 10] = 5.14, p = 0.042) and treatment (F[1, 10] = 9.60, p = 0.011)).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Home-cage water consumption was not altered by either amphetamine (AMPH) treatment or intermittent food presentation. Mean (±SEM) of daily home-cage water consumed (in milliliter) during the 21-day testing period for AMPH and saline groups in experimental and control protocols are shown

When duration of time spent at the drinking spout was averaged across the 21 test days into 5-s bins (Fig. 3), AMPH and saline animals in the experimental protocol showed the postprandial pattern of drinking characteristic of SIP. Spout time for the experimental groups increased after pellet delivery to a maximum in the sixth 5-s bin and then declined to control levels toward the end of the 60-s interval. AMPH-treated animals in the experimental protocol showed the longest duration of drinking throughout most of the interpellet interval. Control animals spent a consistently low amount of time at the spout (Fig. 3). This is further evidence that the SIP protocol was successfully established in the experimental groups.

Fig. 3
figure 3

The experimental (intermittent food presentation) protocol increased time spent at the drinking spout. Mean (±SEM) duration (s) at the drinking spout during the inter-pellet interval (1 min; measured in 5-s bins) over 21 days for amphetamine (AMPH) and saline experimental and control groups is shown

SIP expression

Following 21 days of SIP or control training, in a final day of testing all animals (regardless of protocol) received a dish of 120 pellets (FF) during the their daily 2-h session (Fig. 1). The AMPH experimental group drank more in spite of no longer receiving intermittent food. A three-way ANOVA comparing drinking under FF conditions averaged for the two FF days prior to training and the FF day that followed training revealed a significant effect of day (F[1, 20] = 61.0, p < 0.001), nearly significant day × protocol interaction (F[1, 20] = 4.32, p = 0.051), and a significant day × treatment × protocol interaction (F[1, 20] = 9.02, p = 0.007). Further analysis of the experimental protocol groups by a two-way ANOVA revealed a significant day × treatment interaction (F[1, 10] = 5.09, p = 0.048) and a trend toward significance for treatment (F[1, 10] = 4.61, p = 0.057) suggesting that animals that experienced intermittent food presentation learned to drink more independently of the food schedule. One-way ANOVA for FF prior to SIP training showed no differences between AMPH and saline groups; however, following SIP training AMPH-treated animals drank significantly more during FF than saline-treated controls (F[1, 11] = 7.18, p = 0.023) demonstrating conditioning. This was not true in the control protocol where animals were not exposed to SIP training and did not show increased drinking following 21 days of FF exposure.

Discussion

We examined possible excessive drinking in the context of an AMPH sensitization model of schizophrenia-like symptoms. Animals exposed to a 5-day repeated treatment regimen of the DA-releasing agent AMPH drank significantly more weeks later in a schedule-induced polydipsia protocol than those that received saline alone. Furthermore, AMPH pretreated animals also drank more following the SIP protocol in the absence of an intermittent food schedule. These findings suggest a hyperdopaminergic animal model of polydipsia associated with schizophrenia akin to that observed in the human population.

A moderate amount of SIP was successfully established in our animals as demonstrated through both comparisons to home cage water consumption and control groups not exposed to the intermittent food delivery schedule (Falk 1961, 1969; Flory 1971). In humans, not all individuals with schizophrenia go on to develop polydipsia. The setup reported here produces SIP rates (roughly 30 %) comparable to the frequency of polydipsia reported in schizophrenia populations (approximately 20 %; de Leon et al. 1994).

Excessive drinking in the SIP protocol happens only after repeated pairing of intermittent food with access to a drinking spout. SIP cannot develop in the absence of intermittent food delivery (Falk 1971) and also fails to develop if water is introduced after animals have received a schedule of intermittent food presentation without concurrent water availability or a schedule that is paired with wheel running (Williams et al. 1992). Over the course of days, repeated acute AMPH treatments in the home cage cause increased drinking (Rowland et al. 1981) initially in the presence of food but over time food availability is no longer necessary to sustain the increased intake of water. It appears that eventually, the animals learn to drink in excess of need. We demonstrated this in the SIP protocol and postulate that over time polydipsia in humans may also become a learned pattern of behavior facilitated by a hyperdopaminergic state (for review, see Murray et al. 2008). For instance, in susceptible patients (i.e., acutely psychotic) schedules of institutional living may trigger unregulated drinking behavior that over time, becomes habitual, free of the routine that elicited the behavior in the first place. In SIP, further experiments are necessary to test if this learned behavior is sustained.

The findings of this study markedly contradict studies that have administered AMPH shortly before each SIP testing session (either during or after SIP acquisition): in neurologically healthy animals, such treatments have by and large prevented or attenuated SIP behavior (Yoburn and Glusman 1982; for review, see Mittleman et al. 1994; Didriksen and Christensen 1993). Acute treatments of AMPH increase DA activity via various DA-releasing mechanisms that allows DA to accumulate in the synapse of active DA neurons (Sulzer et al. 1995; Kahlig et al. 2005; for review see Fleckenstein et al. 2007) initially augmenting but then occluding the phasic DA signal. This exaggerated DA efflux (enhanced further by food restriction) into the synapse and its behavioral consequences (increased locomotion and other stereotypic behavior) may abolish the opportunity for SIP to develop (Hooks et al. 1991; Mittleman et al. 1994; Cadoni et al. 2003).

Unlike these studies, we administered AMPH in a 5-day period prior to behavioral testing, followed with a period of drug withdrawal after which animals were tested (entirely drug free). In a similar protocol but with higher and more frequent doses (5 mg/kg, twice daily for 5 days), Mittleman and Valenstein (1985) demonstrated that pre-exposing animals to a regimen of repeated AMPH treatments, followed by 7 days of withdrawal before the start of SIP testing augments SIP. Here, we used substantially lower doses (1.5 mg/kg once a day for 5 days) [Lodge and Grace 2008] of AMPH and a much longer withdrawal period (28 days) to achieve a similar result, demonstrating the sustained long-term effects of low-dose exposure to repeated AMPH treatment. AMPH pretreatment also increases DA activity but in a capacity different than that in acute administration by increasing the population of DA neurons that are active (Lodge and Grace 2008). Thus, the phasic DA signal is enhanced but in a way that maintains the integrity of the signal at the synapse. These two treatment protocols (acute versus the repeated treatment protocol used in our study) likely led to the differential behavioral outcomes on SIP development.

While the neuroanatomial and neurochemical mechanisms mediating SIP and its augmentation by subchronic AMPH treatment remain unknown, both studies of acute and subchronic AMPH regimens suggest the hypothesis that predisposition to nonregulatory drinking may be related to some property of the DA system (Mittleman and Valenstein 1985; López-Grancha et al. 2008). These abnormalities of DA neurotransmission may be secondary to a prefrontal (Deutch 1992) or glutamate dysfunction (Weinberger 1987; Grace 1991; Olney and Farber 1995; Lodge and Grace 2008). Furthermore, the effect of amphetamine sensitization on other monoamines, i.e., serotonin and noradrenaline, as a promoter of SIP cannot be ruled out as repeated treatment of AMPH has been reported to increase reactivity of noradrenergic and serotonergic neurons (Tassin 2008) and both serotonin and noradrenaline in turn have been implicated in SIP behavior (Lu et al. 1992; Tung et al. 2008). How these systems contribute to SIP pathophysiology, however, remains to be elucidated.

Conclusion

Repeated AMPH treatment is a model of schizophrenia-like symptoms that focuses on replicating the pathophysiology of schizophrenia and not its etiology (for review, see Tenn et al. 2003). Here, we demonstrate that repeated AMPH increases drinking behavior that may result from a hyperfunctioning of DA systems. This, along with the evidence that subchronic MK-801 treatment and social isolation also increase SIP and other polydipsias in rats (Hawken et al. 2011, a, b) further supports the idea that the pathophysiology of schizophrenia also participates in the development of polydipsia observed in schizophrenic populations. Future studies need to characterize the behavioral changes in rats treated with subchronic AMPH and to dissect the contribution of various brain regions, related circuitry, and neurotransmitter systems that facilitate excessive fluid intake associated with schizophrenia.