Introduction

Stimuli that become classically conditioned to alcohol by repeated pairing with drinking induce craving (Carter and Tiffany 1999), shift implicit attention toward alcohol-related stimuli (Field and Duka 2002), and promote relapse (Cooney et al. 1997; Litt et al. 2000; Cox et al. 2002). Studies have thus turned to determining the brain areas that most respond to alcohol’s sights, smells, and tastes (e.g., Filbey et al. 2008b; Kareken et al. 2010a, b; Myrick et al. 2008; Tapert et al. 2003). Brain regions responding to such stimuli comprise reward system areas, including the ventral striatum and the medial prefrontal regions projecting to it (Chiba et al. 2001; Haber et al. 2006; Williams and Goldman-Rakic 1998).

Such “naturally conditioned” stimuli have particular face validity, most so when the stimuli can be tailored to an individual’s habitual drinking. However, there are potential limitations to stimuli encountered in the routine course of drinking, as a naturally conditioned stimulus (CS) possesses many other associations besides intoxication—an aspect that will most certainly vary across subjects. Moreover, alcohol pictures are used extensively in advertising, resulting in frequent cue exposure in the absence of alcohol. Similarly, varied and idiosyncratic learning histories would occur between subjects, even with the taste or aroma of a preferred drink. This variation would be particularly prominent between heavier and lighter drinkers and likely extends to the circumstances of intoxication. These additional associations can evoke behavioral changes related to alcohol, drinking history, and expectancies about alcohol’s influence on behavior (Freeman et al. 2010; Kramer and Goldman 2003; Friedman et al. 2007). Demand characteristics that influence subjects to behave in ways they believe comply with the experimenter’s desires may also be a problem with cues of such overt significance. Finally, using a CS with a prior conditioning history precludes the ability to study early stages of reinforcement learning.

Some studies have conditioned novel cues to alcohol intoxication, producing conditioned responses such as heart rate (Staiger and White 1988), gaze time on the CS (Field and Duka 2002; McCaul et al. 1989), and skin conductance (Glautier et al. 1994; McCaul et al. 1989) with varying consistency. One source of variance in these studies is that alcohol’s rewarding properties could change as a function of differential alcohol pharmacokinetics across individuals (Glautier et al. 2000). This is particularly true since the timing of the unconditioned stimulus (UCS; alcohol), its rewarding properties, and even its arrival in the brain varies significantly from incontrollable absorption kinetics associated with oral ingestion. This results in uncertain delays between CS and UCS presentation, and hence varied UCS magnitude (Ramchandani et al. 2006).

In this study of heavy drinkers, our principal aim was to test a novel paradigm that classically conditions unfamiliar stimuli with alcohol intoxication. To accomplish this, we used physiologically based pharmacokinetic (PBPK) modeling (O’Connor et al. 1998; Ramchandani et al. 1999) to control UCS variability and embedded into a reaction time (RT) task a novel graphical symbol (CS+) that briefly preceded and continued through a rapidly rising limb of intravenously (IV) infused ethanol. Differential conditioning was used to similarly pair a CS− with saline infusion during a separate session. The following day, we employed functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to determine if responses in the brain’s reward network differed according to CS type.

Methods

Subjects

Fifteen heavy drinking males participated, with one subject excluded for excessive motion in fMRI. None of the final 14 subjects (Table 1) had a history of treatment for alcohol use disorders, and none were seeking treatment. Three fulfilled formal DSM-IV criteria for alcohol dependence. While we did not exclude prior drug experimentation (13 of 14 subjects), a review of drug use history/frequency did not indicate current/prior dependence on illicit drugs. Three subjects tested positive for cannabinoids at a screening interview, two of whom continued to test positive on study days. None tested positive for stimulants, opioids, barbiturates, benzodiazepines, phencyclidine, or tricyclic antidepressants at either interview or during the remainder of the study. All had breath alcohol (BrAC) measurements of 0.00 on admission for study. Given the 1.5-day procedure, the three participating smokers were allowed to smoke between experimental periods to avoid nicotine withdrawal.

Table 1 Subject characteristics

Procedures

Subjects were invited to participate in a study of alcohol’s effect on RT and were told that they would undergo two sessions of RT testing, once during IV alcohol infusion and once during IV saline infusion (single blind infusion conditions counterbalanced across morning and afternoon sessions). After spending the night in the Indiana University General Clinical Research Center, the subjects had fMRI sessions the following morning.

RT probe task

During an RT “probe task” (e.g., see Field et al. 2004), subjects saw the CS and an adjacent asterisk (randomized to the left and right visual fields), both of which disappeared to reveal a green arrow (response probe) that required a timed response (see Fig. 1). Briefly, the task allowed for the investigation of any possible implicit visual attention to CS stimuli (the green arrow probe) in the lateral visual fields. Nineteen CS presentations occurred per each of six conditioning runs (see below). The subjects were instructed to watch the stimulus array (CS and asterisk) attentively, and when the masking stimuli disappeared, to find and determine the direction of the green probe arrow by pressing an “up” or “down” arrow as quickly as possible. Although the symbols were systematically paired with a given infusate (CS+ with alcohol, CS− with saline), the subjects were never made explicitly aware of this pairing and were never told that the symbols served any function other than to hide the response probe. The side of the CS+ and CS−, the side of the response probe, and whether the probe was hidden by a CS or an asterisk were all counterbalanced and matched for frequency. An irrelevant CS (CSi) was employed for task practice, with the symbols used for the CS+, CS−, and CSi randomly assigned across subjects. To assess implicit attention drawn to the CS+, median RT was analyzed as a function of CS type and the side of the response probe relative to the CS.

Fig. 1
figure 1

fMRI choice reaction time paradigm. a Null crosshair fixation preceded a trial onset of b two stimuli: an asterisk adjacent to a CS+ or CS− (side randomized/counterbalanced). The stimuli then disappeared to reveal the response probe (c; arrow down or arrow up), which was counterbalanced by side, when subjects pressed an “up” or “down” button as quickly as possible. Intertrial intervals were jittered slightly in time. d Three stimuli randomly assigned to the CS types. CSi were used only in task practice and not during conditioning

IV infusion paradigm

Alcohol and saline infusion employed PBPK modeling (O’Connor et al. 1998; Ramchandani et al. 1999), using transformations of height, weight, and gender to calculate a scalloped infusion profile comprising six rapidly ascending limbs, each incrementing BrAC by approximately 18 mg% over 200 s, and each followed by a sharp drop (driven primarily by the distribution of infused alcohol into body water) (Fig. 2). The intent was six unconditioned stimulus presentations throughout the 90-min infusion session. The rate of ascent was intended to be as rapid as possible, resulting in a large “peak-to-trough” contrast in BrAC, and a final BrAC (45 mg%) that allowed a decline to 0 mg% by afternoon in the case of morning alcohol (Fig. 2 for detail and model estimates of alcohol concentration in total body water, venous and arterial blood, and brain). The infusions were conducted with the subject alone in a sound-proof booth while seated in a recliner; all communication with the subject used a microphone and speaker. Jittered 15-min resting periods occurred between pairings, during which the subject could read. The subjects were alerted 2 min in advance of the beginning of a new infusion trial/task. Breath alcohol measurements were obtained using a Dräger Alcotest® 7410 breath alcohol meter.

Fig. 2
figure 2

a PBPK modeling of brain, arterial, venous, and body water alcohol levels during the single alcohol conditioning session. Conditioning sessions included six infusions (duration of 200 s each) followed by a 15-min resting period over the course of 90 min. The RT task (and embedded CS) started coincidentally with the onset of six runs of the infusion pumps and continued for the same duration as the infusion pumps. This resulted in a CS onset just prior to ethanol’s rising limb in the brain (magenta curve, simulated by PBPK modeling) and short delay conditioning procedure. The saline session ran using the same timing, pump rates, and task with only the CS changed (saline infusions were predicted by the CS− instead of the CS+). To avoid frequent intrusion from BrAC measurements (which could not occur in imaging given the magnetic field), BrAC was obtained only once, immediately after completing the final (sixth) ascending limb. b Approximate experimental timeline. Morning and afternoon infusion sessions were randomized/counterbalanced between alcohol and placebo

Subjective responses

At the peak of each of the six ascending limbs, subjects rated subjective reactions, with items displayed on the same monitor used for the RT task. The subjects used the same response pad as for the RT task to position a cursor on a continuous line (range 0–100 units, without the unit numbers displayed) to rate “How intoxicated do I feel right now?”, “How anxious do I feel right now?”, “How tired do I feel right now?”, “How relaxed do I feel right now?”, and “How much am I enjoying the effects of alcohol right now?” Subjects also rated the number of drinks they had felt as if they were experiencing, (“Right now, I feel as if I’ve had ____ drinks.”), with responses incremented in 0.5 drinks. During imaging, these questions were again displayed, with subjects responding similarly on the MRI-compatible fiber-optic response box (Current Designs, Philadelphia, PA) used for the probe task during imaging.

fMRI of the probe task

Subjects performed the same probe task during fMRI as employed during conditioning, except that the task was performed as a mixed-event paradigm of 6:19 min in which all three CS were presented (16 events per each of the CS types, jittered in time with delays between 3,000 and 13,000 ms using OptSeq v. 2.0; http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/optseq/). Stimuli were back-projected onto a screen at the rear of the scanner bore, which subjects viewed with the head coil mirror. Intertrial interval parameters remained the same as during conditioning. To keep the same demand characteristics as during conditioning, the subjects retained their IV cannulae for imaging and were told that they could again receive either alcohol or saline. However, no infusion was performed during the imaging, with the IV pumps remaining off.

Four blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) scans were conducted on a Siemens 3T Magnetom Trio-Tim scanner equipped with a 12-channel head coil array using a BOLD echo planar imaging pulse sequence (193 image volumes, gradient echo, repetition time 2,000 ms, echo time 29 ms, flip angle 76˚, 35 interleaved axial slices with 3 mm thickness and no gap, acquisition matrix 88 × 88, 2.5 × 2.5 × 3.0 mm3 voxels, and GRAPPA acceleration factor 2) with a 3D prospective acquisition correction algorithm adjusting for real time head movement (Thesen et al. 2000). A T1-weighted magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence was utilized to acquire whole-brain high-resolution anatomical images (1.0 × 1.0 × 1.2 mm3 voxels) for co-registration and normalization to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinate system.

Data analysis

Image analysis used statistical parametric mapping (SPM5, Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, University College, London, UK). Functional volumes were corrected for slice acquisition timing and rigid body realigned to account for residual movement after prospective motion correction. Each subject’s anatomic MPRAGE was co-registered to the reference functional volume, segmented into tissue classes, with nonlinear spatial transformation parameters from this segmentation used to transform functional volumes into MNI coordinate space. Normalized functional images were interpolated to cubic (2 mm per side) voxels and smoothed by a 6-mm full-width at half-maximum isotropic Gaussian kernel.

Within-subject, fixed effects of the BOLD response to CS+, CS−, and CSi were estimated using SPM’s canonical hemodynamic response function. The response probe events (pooled over all CS) were also modeled to account for the effects of their appearance, resulting in four conditions per task-run. In within-subject SPM models, event onsets were specified to coincide with the appearance of each CS and response probe. Six movement parameters from realignment were included as regressors to account for residual motion. A high-pass filter with a cutoff of 1/128 Hz was applied to each voxel’s time series to remove low-frequency noise, while an autoregressive AR(1) model accounted for serial correlations. Given our primary hypotheses and the nature of the conditioning, second-stage random effects analyses focused only on CS+ and CS− in a 2 (stimulus presentation) × 4 (scan) SPM5 factorial model, where both stimulus and scan were allowed to correlate. To account for possible extinction from non-reinforcement (no alcohol) during imaging, BOLD responses were examined as averages across all four functional scans and from individual scans. A corrected (family wise error; FWE) cluster statistic at height threshold p < 0.001 (uncorrected) tested for significance, focusing on the insula, striatum, and medial and orbital frontal reward regions. An exploratory threshold of p < 0.001 (uncorrected; extent threshold k > 10 voxels) was also used. Where appropriate, the BOLD effects were extracted from functional regions of interest using MarsBar (Brett et al. 2002).

Behavioral data (subjective responses to alcohol, RT to the response probe) as well as extracted mean BOLD contrast values in functional regions of interest were analyzed in SPSS (Release 18, IBM Corp, Somers, NY).

Results

Pavlovian conditioning

Ethanol infusion, subjective perceptions

The average BrAC across all subjects at the end of ethanol conditioning was 45.1 mg% (SD = 7.0): nearly the exact intended target. Subjective effects were analyzed in session (2; alcohol vs. saline) × time point (6) general linear models (Fig. 3), which showed significant session main effects for “intoxicated,” “number of drinks,” and “enjoy,” as well as session × time interactions for “intoxicated” and “number of drinks.” A time main effect existed for “craving,” but without a main effect for session or an interaction. No other subjective effects showed main effects or interactions. Thus, the subjects appeared to subjectively differentiate between the effects of the two infusates during conditioning outside the scanner.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Subjective responses. Plots for subjective responses in which there were significant main effects or interactions. Ratings obtained at peak estimated ethanol concentration following each of the six conditioning runs (see Fig. 2). *p value adjusted with Greenhouse–Geisser correction for inhomogeneity of variance

Motor performance

Analyzed in a mixed effects linear model, task accuracy in judging response probe direction was high across both alcohol and saline sessions (grand mean = 93% accuracy across all six runs, SD = 1.9%) and did not differ according to infusion. RT (grand mean = 572 ms, SD = 129) was unaffected by infusion (ps > 0.3). Accuracy and RT were similarly analyzed only at the last (sixth) trial, at peak ethanol concentration, and also showed no effects for accuracy (grand mean = 18.6, SD = 0.6), or RT (grand mean = 567 ms, SD = 140) (ps > 0.5).

Neuroimaging

Subjective perceptions

Only two of 14 subjects reported any intoxication during imaging. The first reported intoxication after the third (rating = 20/100) and fourth (rating = 41/100) functional scan when this subject reported feeling 0.5 and 1.0 drinks, respectively. The second subject reported feeling 0.5 drinks only after the second scan, when he rated intoxication as only 1/100. This subject’s ratings then returned to baseline. The 12 remaining subjects reported no intoxication and no sense of having had alcohol during imaging. General linear models showed no other effects on the remaining ratings across the four measured time points (ps > 0.35), with the exception “relaxed” (F[3, 39] = 3.89, p = 0.02) where ratings prior to the last functional scan (mean 26.0; SD = 20.4) were significantly lower than before the first functional scan (mean 40.8; SD = 22.6).

Reaction time to CS

Accuracy in determining response probe orientation was high across all scans (median = 98%, range 92–100%). RT to decide probe direction (each subject’s median RT of correct responses only to avoid outliers) was analyzed as a function of CS (CS+ and CS−) and side of probe presentation (same as or opposite the CS). If implicit attention is drawn to the CS+, RT should be slower in judging probe direction when the probe appears opposite the CS+ (i.e., a CS × side interaction).

RT grand mean was 483 ms (SD = 89). A CS (2) × side (2) mixed linear model showed no significant main effects of CS or side, and the CS × side interaction was insignificant (ps > 0.4). Analyzed for the first scan only, there was a marginal main effect of side (p = 0.053), wherein RT was slightly faster when the probe appeared on the side of any CS (468 ms, SD = 81) compared to the probe’s appearance opposite the CS (484 ms, SD = 89). There was, however, no CS × side interaction (p > 0.6).

Brain responses

Relative to CS−, CS+ produced no significant BOLD activation in the [CS+ > CS−] contrast. However, the reverse comparison [CS− > CS+] showed a significantly large 496 voxel cluster (p < 0.001, corrected cluster statistic) where the peak voxel was significant after correcting for the whole-brain volume (p < 0.05 FWE corrected; Fig. 4a, Table 2). Analyzing the extracted mean BOLD activity from this significantly large cluster in a mixed linear model, there was no significant BOLD response to the CS− itself (p > 0.45), either with reference to the CS+ or the implicit baseline. Rather, for the CS+ as contrasted against the implicit baseline, there was a significant negative response (p < 0.001; Fig. 4b).

Fig. 4
figure 4

a BOLD response from the [CS− > CS+] comparison showing a single, confluent 496 voxel cluster (white oval, p < 0.05, FWE-corrected cluster statistic) in the medial superior frontal gyrus and anterior cingulate. Display threshold, p < 0.001, k > 10 voxels. b Plot of extracted activity from the delineated cluster in a. *p < 0.001, significantly less than the implicit baseline (zero)

Table 2 Activation foci

Extinction might be expected given the lack of alcohol reinforcement, with a diminishing response to the CS+ over time. To test this, the [CS− > CS+] comparison was used to define the region of response at the first scan only, with this region then being analyzed as a function of scan number. At scan 1, the [CS− > CS+] contrast again resulted in a significantly large (364 voxel, FWE-corrected cluster statistic p < 0.001) cluster of activation in the superior frontal gyrus (Fig. 5a; compared to Fig. 4a). Activity in this cluster was extracted with MarsBar and analyzed in a CS (2) × scan (4) mixed linear model, which showed a significant main effect for CS (p = 0.013), as well as a significant CS × scan interaction (p = 0.003) that reflected a changing CS effect over time (Fig. 5b). Consistent with what should be an exponential course of extinction (Rescorla and Wagner 1972), the CS+ BOLD response was significantly less than that for CS− during scan 1 (p < 0.001). The two CS types were insignificantly different during scans 2 and 4 (ps > 0.3), although they were significantly different at scan 3 (p = 0.03).

Fig. 5
figure 5

a BOLD contrast from the [CS− > CS+] comparison as defined by run 1 only showing a significantly large (364 voxel cluster; p < 0.05, FWE-corrected cluster statistic) in the left superior frontal gyrus. Display threshold, p < 0.001, k > 10 voxels. b Plot of extracted activity from run 1 in a with progression of habituation

Within our reward-related anatomic areas of interest, smaller effects (p < 0.001, k > 10 voxels) were present in lateral and medial orbitofrontal cortex (Table 2). Of these three areas, only the left lateral response at [−36, 32, −12] showed a clear directional response from one of the stimulus types. As with the significant medial frontal cluster, the BOLD response to the CS+ was significantly below zero (p = 0.002), while the response to the CS− was not (p > 0.5). Unlike the medial frontal cluster, this lateral orbital cluster did not vary across time.

Discussion

This study examined the brain’s response to stimuli experimentally paired with a rapid infusion of intravenous alcohol and placebo saline. As reflected by subjective ratings, the subjects distinguished between the infusates. In fMRI the morning following this Pavlovian conditioning (where subjects neither perceived nor received alcohol), there emerged a significant medial frontal BOLD response to CS+ associated with alcohol infusion in medial prefrontal regions that we reported to be activated by alcoholic drink odors in heavy drinkers (Kareken et al. 2010a). In the current sample, however, the BOLD responses to the CS+ were significantly negative (below implicit baseline), whereas the CS− response remained at the implicit baseline. Negative BOLD responses are accompanied by declines in both regional cerebral blood flow and the cerebral metabolic rate of oxygen, implicating reduced neuronal activity (Shmuel et al. 2002).

Medial prefrontal cortex—a region that projects to the ventral striatum (Haber et al. 2006)—responds positively to “natural” CSs, such as the sight, smell, or tastes of alcoholic drinks (e.g., Bragulat et al. 2008; Filbey et al. 2008a; Kareken et al. 2010a, b; Myrick et al. 2008), or to monetary rewards (Schott et al. 2008; McClure et al. 2007). Importantly, medial frontal activity reflects the subjective value of food (Hare et al. 2008, 2009) and money (Kable and Glimcher 2007). If such studies show that medial prefrontal cortex activity rises in response to stimuli associated with desired rewards, why would responses to our experimental CS+ be significantly negative?

The answer likely lies in the nature of conditioning and reward availability during fMRI. Most brain imaging studies of drug cues do not pair reward (drug) receipt with cue exposure in any contingent way, or even make the drug available during imaging. More particularly, we are unaware of imaging studies in which a displayed alcohol cue implies the arrival of alcohol, and subjects in previously published studies would have no expectation (implicit or explicit) of this occurring. In our design, the subjects were repeatedly exposed to novel stimuli just prior to a rapidly rising concentration of alcohol in the brain. During imaging, subjects remained connected to the IV infusion pumps when viewing the CS, but without alcohol infusion. This can be viewed as analogous to a “negative prediction error,” wherein a reward predicted by a CS+ fails to arrive. Moreover, differences between the CS+ and CS− were strongest during the first BOLD scan, with a general attenuation over time, as might be expected with persistent non-reinforcement of the CS+. Thus, extinction followed a roughly asymptotic course, albeit with some variability (stimulus differences emerging at the third scan and again becoming insignificant by the fourth).

As initially demonstrated by Schultz and colleagues (Mirenowicz and Schultz 1996; Schultz et al. 1997), negative prediction errors result in a phasic decrease of midbrain dopamine cell activity and in a graded manner that reflects the probability of reward delivery (Fiorillo et al. 2003). Using fMRI, a similar response to prediction errors has been demonstrated in humans, where the BOLD responses in the ventral striatum (a target of the dopaminergic midbrain) become negative with the absence of expected reward, such as juice or money (e.g., Abler et al. 2006; Bray and O’Doherty 2007; Knutson et al. 2001; McClure et al. 2003; O’Doherty et al. 2003). Some studies, however, also find increased BOLD responses to omitted reward or negative feedback (e.g., Pagnoni et al. 2002; Rodriguez et al. 2006). While striatal responses to reward prediction have received the most attention, decreased BOLD responses to cued yet withheld reward have been observed in medial and orbital frontal areas (O’Doherty et al. 2003; Ramnani et al. 2004; Knutson et al. 2001, 2003; Kim et al. 2006), which might be expected given the connectivity between medial- and orbitofrontal areas and the striatum. We did observe a smaller negative BOLD response to the CS+ in left lateral orbital cortex, although this area did not appear to extinguish with time.

Lack of striatal responses may seem surprising since their role in prediction errors is well documented. However, the design of this experiment could preclude observing this phenomenon, at least efficiently. Since alcohol’s slow kinetics do not lend themselves to a binary, on–off administration (as with other rewards such as juice or money that are often used in such studies), temporally precise striatal responses may be harder to detect, especially since midbrain responses are sensitive to temporal delays between a CS and the reward it predicts (Hollerman and Schultz 1998). And it is in this sense that we cannot precisely capture the “temporal difference” aspect of prediction errors, in which a discrete reward outcome is assessed at a particular time. While we can better calculate and control alcohol’s time course with PBPK modeling than with oral ingestion, the precise “time of reward” is not discrete (as with the juice or money delivery in many such experiments), but is more distributed over a longer time. Thus, our paradigm is not a direct translation of most prediction error experiments that measure temporal difference parameters, and responses to our stimuli occur averaged over a protracted period of non-reward. On the other hand, we have previously used positron emission tomography—an imaging technique with considerably less temporal resolution than fMRI—to find depressed ventral striatal dopaminergic transmission when a cued alcohol infusion failed to occur (Yoder et al. 2009)—a finding in accordance with prediction error theory. Therefore, rather than relying on a primary sensory reward (e.g., juice) or a secondary reinforcer (money), our paradigm may more validly reflect brain responses to cues predictive of a longer-acting pharmacological reward.

Failure to obtain a predicted reward may play an important role in addiction. Before a given stimulus has acquired associative properties linked to the drug experience, the unexpected arrival of a rewarding sensation would be “better than predicted;” by contrast, the absence of predicted reward would be “worse than predicted” and a reflection of disappointment (Redish et al. 2007). Thus, we speculate that the depression in prefrontal activity observed here is a manifestation of that disappointment, and one potentially related to the drive to seek alcohol. One example of this might be tolerance, when a given “dose” of alcohol fails to achieve the previously experienced degree of reward (see Lapish et al. 2006). Similarly, if alcohol is more valued by heavy drinkers, the medial prefrontal depression to an unreinforced CS+ might be a measurable neural reflection of that subjective value and disappointment (Hare et al. 2009; Kim et al. 2006).

The artificial stimuli used here as well as the non-oral route of rapid alcohol administration might limit the study’s relevance to the more common pairing of visual and oral compound cues with ethanol, as it would normally be ingested. This may be a valid criticism, and we are unaware of experiments conducted where neural responses are measured during presentation of conditioned stimuli that have been paired with orally administered ethanol (certainly MRI and positron emission tomography are inhospitable settings for such a design). Nonetheless, the procedure in this study has several distinct advantages over those with more naturalistic stimuli, where conditioning is implied rather than actually conducted. First, the neural responses here should be free of semantic priming. For example, many studies show that alcohol cues (absent alcohol’s actual presence) activate semantic networks, with marked effects on behavior and how other semantic stimuli are interpreted (Freeman et al. 2010; Friedman et al. 2007; Goldman 2002). While such priming is important and thought to play a causal role in alcoholism (Roehrich and Goldman 1995; Stacy 1997), it may or may not have anything to do with the implicit learning implicated in classical conditioning and more explicitly measured here. Thus, previous research using naturalistic stimuli may be assessing classical conditioning, semantic learning/activation, or (more likely) some combination of the two. This may also be part of the reason that the neural responses in the present study differ in sign from those of previous studies using such stimuli. In fact, our novel stimuli, after conditioning, did not differentially affect RT as a function of CS and probe side, as has been observed in studies employing alcohol photographs (e.g., see Field et al. 2004; Townshend and Duka 2001). It may be that stimuli with greater, more extensive, and complex conditioning histories are required for such effects.

A particular constraint merits comment: Our study design easily lends itself to examining extinction and negative prediction errors. However, even if intravenously infused, the time course of alcohol may make it difficult to examine positive prediction errors (unexpected reward), or even correctly signaled reward. That is, discrete delivery of alcohol in an on–off, binary fashion cannot be achieved in a realistic time scale that enables adequate statistical power in fMRI. Finally, the sole evidence of conditioning is that of the BOLD response itself, and the measurement of a behavioral conditioned response that mirrors the time course of the BOLD contrast could add confidence to the results.

In summary, the brain response to a CS embedded in a reaction time test differed in these heavy drinkers according to the experimentally induced CS association. CS+ that predicted the rapid rising limb of alcohol concentration induced a highly significant depression in BOLD response compared to CS− stimuli predictive of the same rate of delivery of saline placebo. This suggests that medial prefrontal cortex may play a role in reflecting the absence of predicted rewarding outcomes. The data also suggest that brain responses can be measured to novel stimuli that are experimentally paired with alcohol intoxication—a method that could be useful in the study of alcoholism and its risk factors.