Introduction

Relapse is the main impediment in the treatment of cocaine addiction. In cocaine users, craving and resumption of drug use can be precipitated by exposure to drug-associated cues, stress, or drug itself (Jaffe et al. 1989; Childress et al. 1993; O’Brien et al. 1998; Sinha 2001). Similarly, in the reinstatement and renewal animal models of drug relapse, cocaine-seeking behavior (i.e., nonreinforced responding on a previously cocaine-paired operandum) can be elicited by exposure to a drug-paired context, explicit conditioned stimuli (CS), foot shock-induced stress, or a cocaine-priming injection (de Wit and Stewart 1981; Carroll and Comer 1996; Fuchs et al. 1998; Crombag and Shaham 2002; Epstein et al. 2006). Studies using these and similar procedures have provided important insights into the neural circuitry of drug relapse and revealed a complex role for the nucleus accumbens (NAC) in cocaine seeking and other goal-directed behaviors (Shalev et al. 2002; Bossert et al. 2005; Everitt and Robbins 2005; See 2005).

The core and shell subregions of the NAC contribute differently to the reinstatement of cocaine seeking depending on the type of trigger used to elicit this behavior. γ-Aminobutyric acid (GABA) agonist-induced functional inactivation of or glutamate antagonism within the NAC core uniformly inhibits all forms of reinstatement (McFarland and Kalivas 2001; Fuchs et al. 2004; McFarland et al. 2004; Peters and Kalivas 2006; Bäckström and Hyytiä 2007). Conversely, NAC shell inactivation fails to alter cocaine-primed reinstatement (McFarland and Kalivas 2001), but the antagonism of dopamine receptors, glutamate receptors, L-type Ca2+ channels, Ca2+/calmodulin-mediated kinase II, or the inhibition of GluR1-containing α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptor cell surface expression within the NAC shell disrupts this behavior (Anderson et al. 2003; Anderson et al. 2008). NAC shell inactivation fails to alter CS-induced (Fuchs et al. 2004) but inhibits stress-induced reinstatement (McFarland et al. 2004). The NAC shell may also play a permissive role in cocaine-seeking behavior because dopamine receptor stimulation within this brain region is sufficient to produce reinstatement in the absence of any other trigger (Schmidt et al. 2006; Schmidt and Pierce 2006). However, it has not been investigated whether the NAC plays a role in cocaine-seeking behavior produced by re-exposure to a drug-associated context per se.

Supporting the possibility that the NAC mediates some aspects of context-induced cocaine seeking, this brain region has been implicated in a variety of context-induced Pavlovian-conditioned and instrumental goal-directed behaviors. The NAC core but not the shell exhibits Fos protein expression, a marker of neuronal activation, concomitant with expression of cocaine-conditioned place preference (Miller and Marshall 2005), whereas both NAC subregions exhibit Fos protein expression associated with instrumental cocaine-seeking behavior in a cocaine-paired context (Neisewander et al. 2000; Hamlin et al. 2008). However, Fos protein expression in the NAC core is not observed if the act of cocaine seeking is prevented through retraction of the response lever (Neisewander et al. 2000). In addition to this correlational evidence, lesion studies more directly implicate the NAC in context-induced behaviors, including the expression of sucrose-conditioned place preference (Everitt et al. 1991) and contextual fear conditioning (Levita et al. 2002). Furthermore, recent pharmacological studies have demonstrated that D1-dopamine receptor stimulation within the NAC shell and glutamate receptor stimulation in the NAC shell or core are necessary for drug context-induced heroin-seeking behavior (Bossert et al. 2006; Bossert et al. 2007). However, it is unclear whether the same applies to context-induced cocaine-seeking behavior given that partially divergent neural systems mediate some forms of heroin-seeking versus cocaine-seeking behavior (Grimm and See 2000; Fuchs and See 2002).

The present study aimed to directly evaluate general functional contributions of NAC subregions to context-induced cocaine-seeking behavior. To this end, baclofen and muscimol (B/M), GABA agonists that hyperpolarize cell bodies without affecting fibers of passage, were infused into the NAC core or shell of rats prior to a test of cocaine-seeking behavior in the environmental context where cocaine self-administration had occurred previously and in an alternate context in which cocaine had not been available. Given that the NAC has been implicated in motor behavior, the effects of B/M-induced NAC core and shell inactivation were also assessed on locomotor activity and food-reinforced instrumental behavior. Based on the cocaine reinstatement literature, which indicates that the NAC core plays a more critical role than the NAC shell in some other forms of cocaine-seeking behavior, we postulated that functional inactivation of the NAC core would inhibit the expression of context-induced reinstatement to a greater extent than that of the NAC shell. Remarkably, the results from the present study were not fully consistent with these predictions and have led us to re-evaluate our understanding of NAC function.

Materials and methods

Animals

Experimentally naïve male Sprague–Dawley rats (Charles-River, N = 52), weighing 250–275 g at the start of the experiment, were individually housed in a temperature- and humidity-controlled vivarium on a reversed light–dark cycle. Rats were maintained on 20–25 g of rat chow per day, with water available ad libitum. The housing and treatment of the rats followed the guidelines of the “Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Rats” (Institute of Laboratory Animal Resources, Commission on Life Sciences 1996), and the study protocol was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.

Food training

In order to expedite the acquisition of cocaine self-administration, rats were first trained to lever press on a fixed ratio (FR) 1 schedule of food reinforcement (45 mg pellets; Purina, Richmond, IN, USA) in standard sound-attenuated operant-conditioning chambers (26 × 27 × 27 m high; Coulbourn Instruments, Allentown, PA, USA) during a 16-h overnight food training session. The chambers were equipped with two retractable levers and a food pellet dispenser between the levers. During the session, each lever press on the right (active) lever resulted in delivery of a food pellet only. Lever presses on the left (inactive) lever had no programmed consequences. Following food training, food pellet dispensers were removed from the chambers.

Surgery

Forty-eight hours after food training, rats were anesthetized using ketamine hydrochloride and xylazine (66.6 and 1.3 g/kg, respectively, intraperitoneal). Chronic indwelling catheters were constructed using a bent-steel cannula with a screw-type connector (Plastics One, Roanoke, VA, USA), SILASTIC tubing (10 m, inner diameter 0.64 m, outer diameter 1.19 m, Dow Corning, Midland, MI, USA), Prolite monofilament mesh (Atrium Medical, Hudson, NH, USA), and cranioplastic cement, as described previously (Fuchs et al. 2007). The end of the catheter was inserted into the right jugular vein and was secured to surrounding tissue with suture. The catheter ran subcutaneously and exited on the rat’s back, posterior to the shoulder blades.

Immediately after the catheter surgery, the rats were placed into a stereotaxic instrument (Stoelting, Wood Dale, IL, USA). They received stainless steel guide cannulae (26 G, Plastics One), aimed at the NAC core (+1.4 mm anteroposterior [AP], ±3.1 mm mediolateral [ML], −4.8 mm dorsoventral [DV], relative to bregma) or the NAC shell (+1.3 mm AP, ± 2.5 mm ML, −5.9 mm DV, relative to bregma). The cannulae were angled laterally by 10° in order to avoid the lateral ventricles. Three small screws and cranioplastic cement secured the guide cannulae to the skull. Stylets (Plastics One) and Tygon caps were placed into the guide cannulae and catheter to prevent occlusion, respectively. Rats were given 5 days for postoperative recovery before the start of the experiment.

To extend catheter patency, the catheters were flushed through once daily for 5 days following surgery with 0.1 ml of an antibiotic solution of cefazolin (100.0 mg/ml, Schein Pharmaceutical, Florham Park, NJ, USA) dissolved in heparinized saline (70 U/ml; Baxter Healthcare, Deerfield, IL, USA). Thereafter, catheters were flushed with 0.1 ml heparinized saline (10 U/ml) prior to each self-administration session and with 0.1 ml of the cefazolin solution and 0.1 ml of heparinized saline (70 U/ml) after each session. Catheter patency was periodically verified by infusing 0.1 ml of propofol (10 mg/ml, intravenous; Abbot Labs., North Chicago, IL, USA), an ultrashort-acting barbiturate which produces a rapid loss of muscle tone only when administered intravenously.

Cocaine self-administration training

Cocaine self-administration training was conducted during 2-h sessions on a minimum of 10 consecutive days during the rats’ dark cycle. Rats (N = 35) were trained to press a lever according to an FR 1 schedule of cocaine reinforcement (cocaine hydrochloride; 0.15 mg/0.05 ml per infusion, approximately equal to 0.5 mg/kg per infusion; National Institute on Drug Abuse, Research Triangle Park, NC, USA) with a 20-s timeout period. Food reinforcement was no longer available. The catheters were connected to liquid swivels (Instech, Plymouth Meeting, PA, USA) via polyethylene 20 tubing that was encased in steel spring leashes (Plastics One). The swivels were suspended above the operant conditioning chamber and were connected to infusion pumps (Coulbourn). Data collection and reinforcer delivery were controlled using Graphic State Notation software version 2.102 (Coulbourn).

Self-administration training was conducted in operant-conditioning chambers that contained one of two distinctly different sets of visual, auditory, olfactory, and tactile contextual stimuli in addition to the active (right) and inactive (left) levers. Context 1 contained a continuous red houselight (0.4 fc brightness) on the wall opposite to the active lever, intermittent pure tone (80 dB, 1 kHz; 2 s on, 2 s off), pine-scented air freshener strip (4.5 × 2 cm, Car Freshener, Waterton, NY, USA), and wire mesh floor (26 × 27 cm). Context 2 contained an intermittent white stimulus light above the inactive lever (1.2 fc brightness; 2 s on, 2 s off), continuous pure tone (75 dB, 2.5 kHz), vanilla-scented air freshener strip (4.5 × 2 cm, Sopus Products, Moorpark, CA, USA), and a slanted ceramic tile wall that bisected the bar floor (19 cm × 27 cm). Rats had no exposure to these contextual stimuli prior to cocaine self-administration training. The stimuli were presented throughout each session independent of responding, as in our previous studies (Fuchs et al. 2005; Fuchs et al. 2007). Assignment of rats to cocaine self-administration training in Context 1 versus Context 2 was random. Active lever presses resulted in a 2-s activation of the infusion pump only. After each infusion, responses on the active lever had no consequences during the 20-s timeout period. During the sessions, responses on the inactive lever had no programmed consequences but were recorded. Daily self-administration training sessions were continued until a rat reached the acquisition criterion (i.e., ≥10 infusions self-administered/session on a minimum of 10 training days).

Extinction training

Rats underwent 2-h extinction sessions on at least 7 consecutive days, during which responses on either lever had no programmed consequences. Extinction sessions were conducted in Context 2 for rats that had previously self-administered cocaine in Context 1, and vice versa. Rats had no prior exposure to the extinction training context. On extinction day 5, the rats were adapted to the intracranial infusion procedure prior to placement into the chamber. During the adaptation procedure, stainless steel injection cannulae (33 G, Plastics One) were bilaterally inserted into the rat’s guide cannulae to a depth of 2 mm below the tip of the guide cannulae. Rats were held by the experimenter for 4 min, while the injection cannulae were left in place but fluid was not infused through the infusion cannulae. Extinction training was terminated when the rats reached the criterion for extinction (i.e., ≤25 responses per session on the last 2 consecutive days) with a minimum of 7 days of extinction training.

Intracranial Microinfusions

For intracranial microinfusions, the injection cannulae were connected to 10-μl Hamilton syringes (Hamilton, Reno, NV, USA) that were mounted on an infusion pump (KD Scientific, Holliston, MA, USA). A combination of B/M (1.0/0.1 mM; Sigma-Aldrich) or phosphate-buffered saline vehicle (Veh, pH = 7.0 for both) were infused at volumes of 0.3 μl over 2 min, and the injection cannulae were left in place for 1 min prior to and after the infusion. Muscimol, at doses of 1,000 and 20 ng/1 μl, inhibits glucose utilization in a 1.6-mm radius (Martin 1991) and electrophysiological activity in a 1-mm radius (Arikan et al. 2002), respectively. These estimates likely include tissue that exhibits hypoactivity due to reduced synaptic input from inactivated neurons. In the present study, muscimol was administered at 12 ng/0.3 μl; thus, the area of neural inactivation was significantly smaller due to reduced infusion volume. Similar information about the spread of baclofen hydrobromide is not available, but its spread is likely limited by low lipophilicity (Leisen et al. 2003). Importantly, we and others have used this dose of B/M in the past to demonstrate functional differentiation between the NAC core versus shell in CS-induced and cocaine-primed cocaine-seeking behavior (McFarland and Kalivas 2001; Fuchs et al. 2004).

Reinstatement testing

Two 2-h test sessions were conduced using a counterbalanced design. During one test session, rats were re-exposed to the previously cocaine-paired context, whereas during the other test session, they were re-exposed to the extinction context. Immediately prior to placement into each testing context, rats received microinfusions of B/M or Veh into the NAC core or shell using the infusion procedure described above. Assignment to test session order and B/M versus Veh treatment were balanced based on previous cocaine intake because this variable is a significant predictor of the magnitude of CS-induced reinstatement (Kruzich et al. 1999). During both test sessions, lever presses were recorded on the previously active and inactive levers but had no programmed consequences. During the test session in the cocaine context, rats were connected to the infusion apparatus in order to allow for similar perception of and interaction with the spatial/tactile elements of the context (e.g., slanted tile) as during cocaine self-administration training. However, fluid was not infused through the catheter as a consequence of lever pressing. Between the two test sessions, rats received additional extinction training sessions until they reached the extinction criterion (i.e., ≤25 responses per session on minimum 2 consecutive days).

Locomotor activity testing

To further assess possible nonspecific effects of intracranial B/M microinfusions on general activity, we examined the effects of intra-NAC administration of B/M or Veh on locomotion in a novel context. Seventy-two to 94 h following the last test session, rats received bilateral microinfusions of B/M or Veh into the NAC core or shell using the infusion procedures described above. Assignment to the B/M and Veh treatment groups remained the same as in the preceding reinstatement experiment. Immediately after microinfusion, horizontal locomotor activity was measured in novel Plexiglas chambers (42 × 20 × 20 cm high), as described previously (Fuchs et al. 2007). A computerized activity system (San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA, USA) recorded the number of times eight photobeams were broken by a rat moving in the chamber during a 2-h test session.

Food-reinforced instrumental behavior

To assess possible nonspecific effects of intracranial B/M microinfusions on instrumental behavior per se, we examined the effects of intra-NAC administration of B/M or Veh on lever pressing for a food reinforcer. Experimentally naïve rats (N = 17) received a single overnight food training session and stereotaxic surgery. Rats were food-deprived overnight prior to the food training session and were maintained subsequently on 20–25 g of rodent chow per day, as in the cocaine experiment. After recovery from surgery, rats underwent daily 2-h food self-administration sessions in the presence of the contextual stimuli that made up context 1 or 2. In addition, each chamber was equipped with a feeder and a food pellet dispenser. Active lever presses resulted in the delivery of a single food pellet (45 mg, Purina) into the pellet dispenser according to an FR 1 reinforcement schedule with a 20-s timeout period. Inactive lever presses were recorded but had no programmed consequences. After responding stabilized following minimum 10 days of training (i.e., ≤20% variability in active lever responding across two consecutive sessions), two 2-h test sessions were conducted using a fully counterbalanced within-subjects test design. Prior to each test session, rats received bilateral microinfusions of B/M or Veh into the NAC core or shell using the infusion procedures described above. During the test sessions, active lever presses continued to be reinforced with food pellets according to an FR 1 reinforcement schedule with a 20-s timeout period. Inactive lever presses were recorded but had no programmed consequences. B/M versus Veh treatment order was counterbalanced based on mean food-reinforced responding during the last two training sessions that preceded the first test session. Between the two test sessions, rats received minimum two additional food-reinforced training sessions.

Histological and data analysis

After the last experimental session, rats were fully anesthetized using sodium pentobarbital (100 mg/1 ml, intraperitoneal). Rats were then transcardially perfused using 1× phosphate-buffered saline (Fisher Scientific) and 10% formaldehyde solution (Sigma). The brains were dissected out and sectioned at the coronal plane at a thickness of 75 μm. The sections containing the cannula tracks were mounted on glass slides and were stained using cresyl violet (Kodak, Rochester, NY, USA). Cannula placements were determined using light microscopy. The most ventral portion of each cannula track was mapped onto schematics of appropriate plates from the rat brain atlas (Paxinos and Watson 1997).

Mean cocaine-reinforced and inactive lever presses as well as cocaine intake during the last 3 self-administration training days and the total number of sessions needed to reach the extinction criterion were analyzed separately using 2 × 2 between-subjects analysis of variance (ANOVA) with cannula location (aimed at NAC core, shell) and subsequent treatment (B/M, Veh) as factors. Extinction responding on the active lever was analyzed using separate 2 × 2 × 2 and 2 × 2 × 7 mixed factorial ANOVAs with cannula location and subsequent treatment as between-subjects factors and training day (last self-administration session versus first extinction session; first seven extinction sessions) as a within-subjects factor. Nonreinforced active and inactive lever presses on the test days were analyzed separately using 2 × 2 × 2 mixed-factorial ANOVA with NAC subregion (core, shell) and treatment (Veh, B/M) as between-subjects factors and testing context (extinction, cocaine-paired) as a within-subjects factor. Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated in order to evaluate the relationship between cocaine intake or active lever responding during cocaine self-administration and nonreinforced active lever presses on the reinstatement test day. To further assess treatment effects on context discrimination, nonreinforced active lever presses on the extinction test day and the immediately preceding extinction training day were analyzed using a 2 × 2 × 2 mixed factorial ANOVA with NAC subregion and treatment as between-subject factors and day (training, testing) as the within-subjects factor. Locomotor activity counts were analyzed using a 2 × 2 between-subjects ANOVA with NAC subregion and treatment as factors. Food-reinforced active lever responding was analyzed using a 2 × 2 mixed factorial ANOVA with NAC subregion as a between-subjects factor and treatment as a within-subjects factor. Significant ANOVA main and interaction effects were followed up by simple main-effects tests (t tests for factors with two levels or Tukey LSD post-hoc comparisons for factors with more than two levels), as appropriate. Alpha was set at 0.05.

Results

Histology

A schematic illustrating the distribution of injection cannula placements and photomicrographs showing representative cannula tracts are shown in Fig. 1. The target brain region was defined as the NAC core and the medial NAC shell with no distinction between dorsal versus ventral subareas of the NAC shell. The most ventral point of each injection cannula track was located bilaterally within the NAC core or in the medial NAC shell, including the NAC shell–olfactory tubercle transition zone, for the following number of rats per group: NAC core Veh (N = 8), NAC core B/M (N = 10), NAC shell Veh (N = 8), NAC shell B/M (N = 9), NAC core food control (N = 9; not shown), and NAC shell food control (N = 8; not shown).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Schematic and photographic representation of injection cannula placements within the NAC core (a) or shell (b). The symbols represent the most ventral point of the infusion cannula tracts for rats that received bilateral vehicle (Veh) or baclofen plus muscimol (B/M) infusions into the NAC core (open and filled triangles, respectively) or shell (open and filled circles, respectively), on schematics from the rat brain atlas of Paxinos and Watson (1997). Numbers indicate the distance from bregma in millimeters. The arrows on the photomicrographs identify the most ventral point of the infusion cannula tracts on representative cresyl violet-stained sections

Self-administration and extinction history

NAC core- and NAC shell-cannulated rats exhibited stable responding on the active lever during the last 3 self-administration training days with a within-subject variability of less than 10% in daily cocaine intake. Collapsed across groups, the mean daily cocaine intake (+SEM) was 25.67 + 1.84 infusions (approximately 17.11 + 1.22 mg/kg per session), respectively. There was no pre-existing difference between the four groups in active lever responding (cannula location and subsequent treatment group main and interaction effects, F (1, 31) = 0.002–1.32, p = 0.25–0.96) or inactive lever responding (main and interaction effects, F (1, 31) = 0.14–0.92, p = 0.34–0.71). However, the NAC core-cannulated groups exhibited a strong trend toward higher cocaine intake relative to the NAC shell-cannulated groups (cannula location main effect F (1, 31) = 3.96, p = 0.06) independent of subsequent treatment group (subsequent treatment group main effect and interaction effect, F (1, 31) = 0.06–0.23, p = 0.63–0.81). This trend occurred despite the fact that NAC core and shell experiments were conducted simultaneously, using identical procedures and three internal replications.

The mean active lever responding on the last day of self-administration training was 59.13 ± 7.02. Relative to this day, responding did not decrease significantly on extinction day 1 (mean = 42.82 ± 6.5; all-day main and interaction effects, F (1, 22) = 0.09–0.66, p = 0.42–0.76). The microinfusion adaptation procedure failed to alter responding on extinction day 5 (data not shown). Active lever responding gradually extinguished to criterion (see “Materials and methods”) in all groups (time main effect F (6, 144) = 7.65, p = 0.0001). There was no pre-existing difference between the groups in active lever responding during extinction training (cannula location and subsequent treatment main and interaction effects, F (6, 144) = 0.39–0.78, p = 0.53–0.88) or in the mean number of daily sessions needed to reach the extinction criterion (mean = 7.63 + 0.21; F (1, 31) = 0.02–0.97, p = 0.33–0.88). The mean active lever responding on the last day before the test session conducted in the extinction context and the cocaine context was 6.54 + 2.15 (EXTb shown in Fig. 2a) and 5.91 + 0.89 lever presses per session, respectively.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Responses (mean/2 h + SEM) on the active (a) and inactive (b) levers during self-administration (SA; last 3 days of training), extinction training (EXTb, last session before the extinction context test), as well as during testing conducted in the extinction (EXT CTX) and cocaine-paired contexts (COC CTX). Microinfusions of B/M (black bars; 1.0/0.1 mM, 0.3 μl per site) or Veh (open bars; 0.3 μl per site) were administered into the NAC core (N = 8–10 per group) or shell (N = 8–9 per group) immediately before each test session. In the main panel, symbols represent significant difference relative to the extinction context test (asterisk, ANOVA context simple main effect, p = 0.0001), relative to Veh treatment (cross, ANOVA treatment simple main effect, p = 0.002–0.045), and relative to EXTb (double cross, ANOVA day simple main effect, p = 0.03). In the insets, symbols represent significant difference relative to the extinction context test (number sign, ANOVA context simple main effect, p = 0.002–0.01) and relative to Veh treatment (carat sign, a ANOVA treatment simple main effect, p = 0.04; b ANOVA treatment main effect, p = 0.001)

Effects of NAC core or shell functional inactivation on context-induced cocaine seeking

Cocaine-seeking behavior

B/M-induced neuronal inactivation of the NAC shell or core attenuated cocaine-seeking behavior in the cocaine-paired context and increased responding in the extinction context, relative to Veh treatment (Fig. 2a). The 2 × 2 × 2 ANOVA of active lever presses revealed a significant treatment × context interaction (F (1, 31) = 16.60, p = 0.0001) and significant context main (F (1, 31) = 21.21, p = 0.0001) and treatment main effects (F (1, 31) = 4.67, p = 0.03) but no NAC subregion main or interaction effects. Collapsed across NAC subregions (Fig. 2a main panel), exposure to the cocaine-paired context produced greater active responding than exposure to the extinction context in the groups that received Veh treatment (ANOVA context simple main-effects test, p = 0.0001). Active lever responding in these groups in the cocaine-paired context was similar in magnitude to that during self-administration with a strong trend for more robust responding overall in NAC core-cannulated rats (trends for context × NAC subregion interaction and NAC subregion main effects, F (1, 31) = 3.89, p = 0.058 and F (1, 31) = 3.84, p = 0.06, respectively), relative to the NAC shell-cannulated rats. However, overall, in Veh-treated rats, the magnitude of active lever responding in the cocaine-paired context did not correlate with previous cocaine intake (R (16) = 0.012, p = 0.96) or cocaine-reinforced active lever responding (R (16) = 0.037, p = 0.89). Collapsed across NAC subregions, B/M treatment significantly attenuated active lever responding in the cocaine-paired context (ANOVA treatment simple main-effects test, p = 0.002) but increased responding in the extinction context (p = 0.045), relative to Veh treatment. Thus, the B/M-treated groups exhibited similar rates of responding upon exposure to the cocaine-paired and extinction contexts (ANOVA context simple main-effects test, p = 0.58). Similar to the omnibus ANOVA, separate treatment × context ANOVAs for the NAC core- or shell-cannulated groups revealed significant context main and context × treatment interaction effects (NAC core, F (1, 16) = 16.41–6.48, p = 0.001–0.02; NAC shell, F (1, 15) = 5.41–12.58, p = 0.03–0.003). Thus, exposure to the cocaine-paired context increased active lever responding in both the NAC core-cannulated (p = 0.01) and NAC shell-cannulated Veh-treated groups (p = 0.002). B/M treatment attenuated this behavior in the cocaine-paired context (NAC core, p = 0.04; NAC shell, p = 0.001), relative to Veh treatment.

To further assess the effects of B/M on responding in the extinction context, a separate analysis compared active lever responding in the four groups during the test day that was conducted in the extinction context compared with responding exhibited on the last extinction training day before this test day (see EXTb in Fig. 2a). This 2 × 2 × 2 ANOVA of active lever presses revealed a treatment × day interaction effect only (F (1, 31) = 5.52, p = 0.025) and no NAC subregion main or interaction effects. Interestingly, the source of the interaction effect was that the B/M-treated groups exhibited a significant increase in responding during the test session in the extinction context relative to the preceding extinction session (ANOVA day simple main effect, p = 0.028), whereas the Veh-treated groups did not (p = 0.38). Similar to the omnibus ANOVA, a separate treatment × context ANOVA for the NAC core-cannulated groups revealed a treatment × day interaction effect (F (1, 16) = 4.61, p = 0.048), but pair-wise comparisons failed to indicate statistically significant treatment or day simple main effects (p = 0.08–0.52; Fig. 2a insets). Furthermore, a separate ANOVA for the NAC shell-cannulated groups did not indicate significant treatment or day main or interaction effects.

During the test sessions, inactive lever responding was slightly increased by intra-NAC core but not intra-NAC shell and B/M treatment relative to Veh treatment (Fig. 2b insets). The 2 × 2 × 2 ANOVA of inactive lever presses did not indicate any significant main or interaction effects besides a marginally significant NAC subregion × treatment interaction effect (F (1, 31) = 3.89, p = 0.054). Contrary to the omnibus ANOVA, a separate treatment × context ANOVA for the NAC core-cannulated groups revealed a significant treatment main effect (F (1, 16) = 4.47, p = 0.050), indicating that B/M treatment in the NAC core increased inactive lever responding in both contexts, relative to the Veh (Fig. 2b insets). The ANOVA for the NAC shell-cannulated groups revealed a significant treatment × context interaction effect (F (1, 15) = 6.93, p = 0.02), but pair-wise comparisons failed to indicate statistically significant treatment or day simple main effects (p = 0.09–0.66).

Effects of NAC core or shell inactivation on locomotor activity and food-reinforced instrumental behavior

B/M treatment failed to alter locomotor activity in a novel context (Fig. 3a) or food-reinforced lever responding in contexts 1 and 2 (Fig. 3b), relative to Veh treatment. Consistent with this, the 2 × 2 ANOVA of photobeam interruptions failed to indicate NAC subregion or treatment main or interaction effects (F (1,31) = 0.14–3.18, p = 0.08–0.71). Similarly, separate 2 × 2 ANOVAs of active lever presses, inactive lever presses, and food pellets obtained during the two food self-administration test sessions failed to indicate NAC subregion or treatment main or interaction effects (F (1,15) = 0.002–3.04, p = 0.10–0.96). Thus, B/M failed to alter motor or instrumental performance, relative to Veh. Separate analyses for the NAC core- or shell-cannulated groups yielded similar results as the omnibus ANOVAs.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Locomotor activity (a; photobeam breaks/2 h + SEM) in a novel context and food-reinforced instrumental behavior (b; active lever presses/2 h + SEM) in contexts 1 or 2. Immediately before the test session, B/M (1.0/0.1 mM, 0.3 μl per site) or Veh (0.3 μl per site) were administered bilaterally into the NAC core (gray bars; N = 8–10 per group) or shell (hatched bars; N = 8–9 per group)

Discussion

While the contribution of the NAC has been extensively investigated with respect to goal-directed behavior, the present study provides the first demonstration that the functional integrity of this brain region is necessary for the ability of a cocaine-paired context to produce reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior. Briefly, passive re-exposure to a cocaine-paired context was sufficient to reinstate extinguished cocaine-seeking behavior relative to responding in the extinction context. Reinstatement likely reflected context-induced motivation for cocaine as opposed to dishabituation produced by a context shift, since exposure to a novel context fails to elicit an increase in lever responding in this animal model (Fuchs et al. 2005). GABA agonist-induced neural inhibition within the core or shell subregions of the NAC disrupted this behavior, which was indicated by a cocaine context-specific and lever-specific decrease in responding following B/M treatment relative to Veh treatment. B/M-treated groups responded at a similar magnitude in the cocaine-paired and extinction contexts. Remarkably, this was in part due to a B/M-induced increase in responding on the active lever in the extinction context relative to Veh and relative to the extinction baseline. This mitigates the possibility that the B/M-induced decrease in reinstatement was due to a performance deficit, especially since B/M failed to disrupt locomotor activity or food-reinforced instrumental behavior. Instead, these findings suggest that the functional integrity of the NAC is necessary for context-induced cocaine-seeking behavior.

Interestingly, the observed effects were not subregion specific. The failure to observe subregion-specific B/M effects on reinstatement was not likely due to a trend for greater cocaine intake in the NAC core-cannulated subjects. Neither cocaine intake nor cocaine-reinforced lever responding correlated with the magnitude of cocaine context-induced reinstatement in Veh control subjects, even though reinstatement responding appeared to be proportional to cocaine-reinforced responding. Furthermore, high reinstatement responding in the NAC core-cannulated Veh control group was unlikely to increase the sensitivity to detect a B/M-induced decrease in responding because the subtle effects of cannula placement on responding appeared to affect equally the Veh and B/M-treated groups. In fact, effect sizes appeared to be greater in the NAC shell (79% inhibition) than in the NAC core (57% inhibition), which is consistent with previous studies that revealed a selective role for dopamine D1 receptor stimulation in the NAC shell but a role for glutamate neurotransmission in both the NAC shell and core in context-induced renewal of heroin-seeking behavior (Bossert et al. 2006, 2007). Thus, it appears that the ventral striatum represents a point of convergence between the neural substrates for context-induced cocaine-seeking and heroin-seeking behaviors assessed using the reinstatement and renewal paradigms. To further evaluate the extent of this convergence, future studies will be needed to assess the pharmacological mechanisms of context-induced cocaine-seeking behavior within the NAC.

The lack of subregion-specific effects in the present study was not likely due to B/M spread between the NAC core and shell. The exact spread of GABA agonists in brain tissue is difficult to estimate because radioactive and fluorescent tags alter diffusion properties and measures of neural activity overestimate spread due to the synaptic inhibition of neurons in contact with GABA-inactivated cells (for a review, see Martin 1991; Martin and Ghez 1999). According to one such study, 20 ng muscimol delivered in a larger (1 μl) infusion volume than that used in the present study impairs electrophysiological activity directly and indirectly, via the inhibition of postsynaptic neurons, in a 1-mm radius (Arikan et al. 2002). In contrast, in the present study, B/M (including 12 ng muscimol) was administered at a volume of 0.3 μl into the NAC core or shell approximately 1.25 mm apart, thus outside of likely drug spread. More importantly, using the same B/M dose and microinfusion parameters, we and others have demonstrated functional differentiation between the NAC core and shell in CS-induced and cocaine-primed cocaine-seeking behavior, with effects observed in the NAC core only (McFarland and Kalivas 2001; Fuchs et al. 2004). These findings diminish the possibility that the lack of NAC subregion-specific B/M effects on reinstatement was due to drug spread between the NAC core and shell. However, the effects of intra-NAC shell B/M treatment may have been partially mediated by the adjacent medial olfactory tubercle, which was within the area of likely B/M diffusion. In fact, post-hoc analyses revealed that B/M treatment attenuated reinstatement to a similar extent relative to Veh regardless of placement proximity to the olfactory tubercle (data not shown). Similar to the NAC shell, the olfactory tubercle has been implicated in the rewarding and primary reinforcing effects of cocaine, morphine, and electrical self-stimulation (Fibiger et al. 1987; Kornetsky et al. 1991; Ikemoto 2003; Di Ciano et al. 2008; Sellings et al. 2006; Ikemoto 2007). Also, fundamental similarities exist between the anatomical connectivity of the NAC shell and olfactory tubercle, which suggests that the NAC shell–olfactory tubercle complex may comprise a functional unit with respect to context-induced goal-directed behavior (Sesack et al. 1989; Bossert et al. 2007; Ikemoto 2007).

The opposite effects of B/M treatment on lever responding in the cocaine-paired versus extinction context in the present study suggest that the ventral striatum may contribute to context-induced reinstatement by promoting drug context-induced motivation and context discrimination. The robust B/M-induced decrease in responding in the cocaine-paired context likely reflected a decrease in the incentive motivational effects of this environment. Indeed, previous studies have shown that the NAC is necessary for the expression of discriminative stimulus-induced or context-induced motivated behaviors. For instance, nonselective dopamine receptor antagonism in the NAC core inhibits Pavlovian approach to a food-predictive CS+ without altering the approach to a nonreward-predictive CS− (Di Ciano et al. 2001). Dopamine D1 receptor antagonism in the NAC as a whole attenuates instrumental responding selectively in the presence of a food-predictive discriminative stimulus (Yun et al. 2004). Furthermore, dopamine D1 receptor antagonism in the NAC shell or group 2 metabotropic glutamate autoreceptor stimulation in the NAC shell or core impairs reinstatement of heroin-seeking behavior in a heroin-paired context without altering responding in the extinction context (Bossert et al. 2006, 2007). This not withstanding, impairment in context discrimination may account for the B/M-induced modest increase in extinction responding observed in the present study. Similarly, 6-hydroxy-dopamine or neurotoxic lesions of the NAC as a whole or AMPA/kainate receptor antagonism in the NAC core disrupt discriminated conditioned approach behavior in rodents, as indicated by an increase in the approach to a nonreward-predictive CS− or a decrease in the response latency to an aversive outcome (Di Ciano et al. 2001; Parkinson et al. 2002; Schoenbaum and Setlow 2003). Perhaps, most relevant to the present study, tetrodotoxin-induced or ionotropic glutamate receptor antagonist-induced neural inactivation of the NAC disrupts discriminative stimulus control over food-seeking behavior by increasing the latency to respond to a food-predictive discriminative stimulus and increasing responding in the absence of cues and on the inactive lever (Yun et al. 2004). A B/M-induced context discrimination deficit could impair response suppression in the extinction context as well as response initiation in the cocaine-paired context without altering the motivation for cocaine. A discrimination deficit could reflect impairment in the ability to adjust behavior according to accurate context-based outcome expectancies. Alternatively, the same could result from context recognition deficit, since dopamine D2 or N-methyl-d-aspartic acid receptor blockade in the NAC disrupts spatial and object recognition memory in cocaine-naïve rats and mice (Léna et al. 2001; Sargolini et al. 2003). However, in the present study, B/M produced a greater decrease in responding in the cocaine context than the increase in responding in the extinction context. The context discrimination deficit hypothesis alone does not fully explain this pattern of findings. Accordingly, we postulate that B/M reduced context-induced motivation for cocaine and produced a discrimination deficit, which further attenuated residual goal-directed behavior in the cocaine-paired context and augmented it in the extinction context. Interestingly, SCH23390 or LY379268 administered into the NAC selectively disrupts drug context-induced motivation for heroin-seeking behavior (Bossert et al. 2006, 2007). Thus, it is possible that distinct ventral striatal subcircuits mediate context-induced motivation and context discrimination and the latter are inhibited by B/M but not by SCH23390 or LY379268.

In a previous study, GABA agonist-induced neural inactivation of the NAC shell failed to inhibit CS-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior (Fuchs et al. 2004). The apparent discrepancy between this and the present findings may be related to the maintenance of CS-induced and context-induced reinstatement by conditioned reinforcers and occasion setters, respectively. Conditioned reinforcers signal imminent drug reinforcement and can become the end goal of responding, whereas occasion setters are discriminative stimuli that indicate reinforcement availability contingent upon drug-seeking behavior (Gordon and Klein 1994; Ciccocioppo et al. 2001). Consistent with this distinction, partially divergent neural substrates appear to mediate the motivational effects of these cues (Fuchs et al. 2005). The NAC shell may mediate context-induced cocaine-seeking behavior by interacting with the dorsal hippocampus, the only known NAC shell efferent that selectively mediates context-induced cocaine-seeking behavior via interaction with the basolateral amygdala (BLA; van Groen and Wyss 1990; Amaral and Witter 1995; Ferbinteanu and McDonald 2001; Fuchs et al. 2005, 2007). Other sources of efferent input to the NAC shell may include elements of the shared context/CS and context/CS/drug reinstatement circuitries, such as the BLA, ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), and ventral hippocampus (Sesack et al. 1989; McDonald 1991; McDonald 1991; McLaughlin and See 2003; Fuchs et al. 2005; Fuchs et al. 2007; Rogers and See 2007; Peters et al. 2008; Fuchs, unpublished data). In turn, the NAC shell may influence the activity of the context-induced reinstatement circuitry via its afferent projections to the lateral hypothalamus (Hamlin et al. 2008) and reciprocal connections with the NAC core and the ventral tegmental area (VTA; Heimer et al. 1991; McFarland and Kalivas 2001; Crombag et al. 2002; van Dongen et al. 2005). Orexin/hypocretin-containing neurons in the lateral hypothalamus project to the BLA, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC), and VTA, and promote discriminative stimulus-induced ethanol-seeking behavior as well as morphine-conditioned place preference (Fadel and Deutch 2002; Narita et al. 2006; Dayas et al. 2008). Furthermore, dopamine neurons in the VTA innervate almost all elements of the known reinstatement circuitry and influence its activity, since dopamine receptor blockade systemically or selectively in the NAC shell disrupts context-induced renewal of cocaine- and heroin-seeking behavior, respectively (Crombag et al. 2002; Bossert et al. 2007).

Unlike in the NAC shell, B/M treatment in the NAC core inhibits both CS-induced and context-induced reinstatement (Fuchs et al. 2004; present study). Behavioral and anatomical evidence suggests that the NAC core may initiate context-induced or CS-induced cocaine-seeking behavior based on input from several of its afferents that play a critical role in these forms of cocaine-seeking behavior. These brain regions include the BLA, dmPFC, dorsolateral caudate-putamen (dlCPu), and the reciprocally connected VTA (Heimer et al. 1991; McDonald 1991; Crombag et al. 2002; Heidbreder and Groenewegen 2003; McLaughlin and See 2003; Fuchs et al. 2005; Fuchs et al. 2007). Disconnection of the NAC core from the BLA or dlCPu disrupts CS-induced cocaine-seeking behavior or responding on a second-order schedule of cocaine reinforcement (Di Ciano and Everitt 2004; Belin and Everitt 2008). However, similar disconnection studies have yet to be conducted to verify functionally significant interactions between the NAC and the above brain regions with respect to context-induced reinstatement.

It has been theorized that integration of information from the hippocampus, BLA, and other elements of the reinstatement circuitry occurs at the level of the dmPFC and vmPFC, which are theorized to coregulate the display of cocaine-seeking behavior via inputs to the NAC core and NAC shell, respectively (Kalivas and McFarland 2003; Peters et al. 2007, 2008). The complex anatomy of the reinstatement circuitry and recent electrophysiological data suggest that additional computations based on information from the BLA and hippocampus may also occur at the level of the NAC, increasing the influence of this brain region (O’Donnell and Grace 1995; Goto and O’Donnell 2002; Di Ciano and Everitt 2004). While the present study did not reveal NAC subregion-specific effects on context-induced reinstatement, it is probable that distinct neuronal subcircuits and neurochemical mechanisms within the NAC core and shell, as well as the larger reinstatement circuitry, mediate context-induced versus other forms of cocaine-seeking behavior, and these subcircuits may be differentially susceptible to cocaine-induced and experience-based neuroadaptations (Neisewander et al. 2004; Bossert et al. 2006; Fuchs et al. 2006; See et al. 2007). Therefore, future studies will need to investigate the neurobiology of these subcircuits to better understand and treat cocaine addiction.