Introduction

In the early 1990s Tattersall and Gill reported a series of unexplained nocturnal sudden deaths of young people with type 1 diabetes [1]. The evidence suggested that this ‘dead in bed’ syndrome was caused by hypoglycaemia, since the patients had a history of severe nocturnal hypoglycaemia. The circumstances of the deaths, in which the patients were found in an undisturbed bed, led to the hypothesis that fatal cardiac arrhythmia had been triggered by hypoglycaemia [2]. Experimental and spontaneous hypoglycaemia has been shown to cause alterations in cardiac repolarisation [36]. Atrial fibrillation [3] and ventricular ectopic beats [4] have been reported during hypoglycaemia but the most notable change is a prolonged heart rate corrected QT interval (QTc) [46]. Prolongation of the QTc has been associated with an increased risk of sudden death [7, 8] and is therefore the variable of interest when investigating the proarrhythmic effect of hypoglycaemia. The QT interval is dependent on the heart rate, so that heart rate correction is necessary in order to allow comparison of QT intervals measured at different heart rates. There are various methods for heart rate correction, of which Bazett’s formula [9] is the most popular. However, this method is criticised because of overcorrection at high heart rates and undercorrection at low heart rates [10, 11]. Any fixed correction formula, like Bazett’s, assumes the same QT/RR relation in all individuals. However, the QT/RR relation is highly individual, and a subject-specific correction formula is therefore preferred over a fixed correction formula [11]. The effect of spontaneous hypoglycaemia on QTc has been studied in both hospitalised [6, 12] and ambulant [4, 13] settings, but all studies use Bazett’s heart rate correction. Hypoglycaemia can increase heart rate [14], and a significant change in QTc during hypoglycaemia could merely reflect overcorrection by Bazett’s formula. In addition, fully automatic measurements of QT intervals from Holter monitor data without visual inspection by a human observer, as has been done in some studies [4, 13], might lead to significant errors [15].

In this study we used continuous glucose monitoring (CGM) and ECG monitoring in an ambulant setting of participants with type 1 diabetes to investigate whether episodes of spontaneous hypoglycaemia cause QTc prolongation. We investigated whether the choice of heart rate correction formula affects QTc prolongation during spontaneous hypoglycaemia.

Methods

Subjects

We studied 21 participants (13 men and eight women) with type 1 diabetes who were prone to hypoglycaemia, defined as at least two episodes of severe hypoglycaemia within the last year. Assuming that two-thirds of the participants would develop hypoglycaemia in the monitoring period, this sample size had 80% power to detect a mean increase in QTc of 15 ms or more during hypoglycaemia. All participants had a normal resting 12-lead ECG and did not have a history of known cardiovascular complications. No participant was on medication known to cause changes in the QT interval. The presence of autonomic neuropathy was assessed by standard cardiovascular tests [16]. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants and the study protocol was approved by the local ethics committee.

Procedures

Participants were admitted to the clinical research unit on Thursday at 20:00 hours. ECG was measured with a frequency of 1,000 Hz and 1 μV resolution from lead II using a digital Holter monitor (SpiderView Plus; ELA Medical, Montrouge, France). Simultaneously, interstitial glucose concentration (IG) was monitored using a Guardian Real-Time Continuous Glucose Monitoring System (Medtronic MiniMed, Northridge, CA, USA) with the prevailing glucose level blinded. At 23:00 hours a cannula was placed into an antecubital arm vein so that blood samples could be taken at hourly intervals until 07:00 hours the next morning. Blood samples were centrifuged and stored for later measurement of plasma glucose. At 08:00 hours, participants were sent home with the monitoring equipment and were instructed to calibrate the CGM at least four times a day. Monitoring ended on Sunday at 20:00 hours, so a total of 72 h of continuous CGM and ECG data were available for each participant.

ECG measurements

The ECG was analysed using custom analysis software developed in MatLab (Version 7.8.0.347; MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). The ECG was analysed in segments of 1 min. Templates representing the average PQRST complex in the segments were generated. The end of the T wave (Tend) in each template was determined automatically using the slope intersect method [17]. The QT interval was calculated as the time from the beginning of the Q wave to Tend in each template. All templates and associated fiducial points were reviewed manually on screen twice by each of two observers blinded to the corresponding blood glucose and the participants’ clinical data. Templates were rejected if artefacts precluded reliable measurements of the QT interval. Rejected templates constituted 4% of all templates. No adjustments to the QT interval were made to reduce the subjectivity of the measurements. The mean intra-observer coefficient of variation for QT intervals was 1.6% during euglycaemia and 2% during hypoglycaemia. The inter-observer coefficient of variation was 2% during euglycaemia and 3% during hypoglycaemia.

The median RR interval in each segment was used to correct the QT interval for heart rate. QT intervals were corrected by Bazett’s formula (QTcB) [9], Fridericia’s formula (QTcF) [18] and the nomogram method (QTcN) [19]. A subject-specific linear correction formula \( \left( {{\hbox{QTcS}} = {\hbox{QT}} + \alpha \times \left[ {{1} - {\hbox{RR}}} \right]} \right) \) was also used as it has been shown to be the optimum model in most individuals [11]. Each participant-specific coefficient, α, was calculated by fitting the euglycaemic QT/RR pairs from each participant to a linear regression model \( \left( {{\hbox{QT}} = \beta + \alpha \times {\hbox{RR}}} \right) \) using least-squares regression.

Statistical analysis

QTc was analysed at every measurement of hypoglycaemia defined as IG, overnight plasma glucose concentration or self-monitored blood glucose concentration ≤3.5 mmol/l. Measurements of QTc during euglycaemia were taken at intervals of 30 min throughout the monitoring period, discarding periods when glucose values <5 mmol/l or >12 mmol/l had been measured within 1 h. Median RR, QT and QTc durations during euglycaemia and hypoglycaemia were calculated for each participant. Changes in variables between euglycaemia and hypoglycaemia among participants were tested using a two-tailed paired Student’s t test. Differences in QTc due to sex and neuropathy were tested using a two-tailed unpaired Student’s t test. Results are reported as mean ± SD and a p value ≤0.05 was considered significant. Statistical analysis was performed using R version 2.9.1 (http://www.r-project.org/).

Results

Clinical characteristics

A total of 14 participants (67%) developed one or more episodes of hypoglycaemia in the monitoring period. Clinical characteristics of all participants and participants experiencing hypoglycaemia are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Clinical characteristics of all participants in the study and participants experiencing hypoglycaemia

Blood glucose

The root mean square difference between IG and measurements of blood glucose was 1.6 ± 0.4 mmol/l and the absolute relative error was 20 ± 7%. Glucose concentrations during hypoglycaemia and euglycaemia were 2.8 ± 0.4 and 9.0 ± 1.8 mmol/l, respectively.

ECG variables

The changes in measured ECG variables between euglycaemia and hypoglycaemia are summarised in Table 2. QTcB increased from euglycaemia to hypoglycaemia (422 ± 30 vs 432 ± 33 ms; p = 0.02). QTcF, QTcN and QTcS did not change significantly. During the monitoring period there was a case of prolonged hypoglycaemia with IG <2.5 mmol/l for more than 4 h. In this case a QTcS greater than 500 ms was observed (Fig. 1). Changes in QTcS from euglycaemia to hypoglycaemia were not different between men and women (9 ± 20 vs 8 ± 8 ms; p = 0.84) or between people with and without neuropathy (19 ± 26 vs 4 ± 9 ms; p = 0.35).

Table 2 Difference in ECG variables between euglycaemia and hypoglycaemia
Fig. 1
figure 1

Measurements of QTcS in euglycaemia (white circles) and hypoglycaemia (black circles) plotted against time of day for participant 1 (male, age 51 years). Also shown is the IG measured by the continuous glucose monitoring (solid line)

Heart rate correction

The participant-specific correction coefficient, α, was 0.20 ± 0.04 for the 14 participants with hypoglycaemia. Table 3 shows how the fixed correction formulas compare with QTcS during measurements of hypoglycaemia. QTcB was not significantly different from QTcS (p = 0.08), but QTcF (p = 0.02) and QTcN (p = 0.04) significantly underestimated the QTc during hypoglycaemia. In Fig. 2, the QT/RR intervals during euglycaemia are shown for two participants together with the results of correcting with Bazett’s, Fridericia’s and the participant-specific correction.

Table 3 Differences during hypoglycaemia between the participant-specific correction formula, QTcS, and the three fixed formulas, QTcB, QTcF and QTcN
Fig. 2
figure 2

QT/RR relationship (black circles) for euglycaemic measurements from participant 1 (a) and participant 7 (b). Also shown are QTc/RR relationships using Bazett’s method (white circles), Fridericia’s method (black squares) and a linear subject-specific method (white squares). In (a), overcorrection by Bazett’s formula is seen at low RR intervals (high heart rate), while Fridericia’s formula is closer to the subject-specific method. The opposite is seen on in (b), where Fridericia’s formula undercorrects the QTc, while Bazett’s formula is closer to the subject-specific method

Rate and rhythm disturbances

One participant had frequent ventricular ectopic beats but they were present during both euglycaemia and hypoglycaemia. No cases of sinus bradycardia (<40 beats/min) or rhythm disturbance were seen.

Discussion

In this study we confirm previous findings of prolonged QTc corrected by Bazett’s formula during spontaneous hypoglycaemia in adults with type 1 diabetes [4, 6] However, Bazett’s formula was the only correction method that produced a significantly prolonged QTc from euglycaemia to hypoglycaemia. In comparison with the participant-specific correction formula, we found Bazett’s formula to have the lowest mean difference of the three fixed correction formulas. Fridericia’s formula and the nomogram method significantly undercorrected the QTc during hypoglycaemia. Only a small, insignificant increase in heart rate was seen during hypoglycaemia, which could indicate either a reduced sympathetic response to hypoglycaemia or simultaneous vagal activation. Nevertheless, it is noteworthy that even an insignificant increase in heart rate results in significant differences among correction formulas. In our study we found no cases of the rate or rhythm disturbances reported by another study on spontaneous hypoglycaemia [4].

The prolongation of QTc observed in the present study is comparable to a previous report of ECG changes during spontaneous hypoglycaemia in an inpatient setting [12]. In outpatient settings using fully automatic measurements of QT and CGM, QTc has been reported to lengthen to a greater degree than seen in this study [4, 13]. However, the use in these studies of fully automatic measurements of QT without manual over-read has been associated with significantly longer QT intervals and measurement errors [15]. Therefore, results obtained using automatic measurements without manual review should be interpreted with caution.

Investigations of changes in QTc during spontaneous hypoglycaemia have only been done using Bazett’s formula [4, 12, 13]. Koivikko et al. [20] found that QTc using Bazett’s formula was significantly prolonged during clamped hypoglycaemia while Fridericia’s formula and the nomogram method resulted in insignificant changes in QTc. The results of our study confirm these findings.

The fact that we observed a case of severely prolonged QTc during hypoglycaemia in one participant while only a modest increase was seen on average could be explained by the concept of the repolarisation reserve articulated by Roden [21]. This concept suggests that a drug or metabolic change such as hypoglycaemia that affects repolarisation currents may be counteracted by variations in other currents (i.e. the repolarisation reserve), thus maintaining a normal action potential. Reduction in the repolarisation reserve can have a genetic basis or may be acquired and may explain why some individuals show larger prolongations of QTc during hypoglycaemia than others. It is possible that genetic analysis of dead-in-bed patients could reveal mutations responsible for increased susceptibility to QTc prolongation during hypoglycaemia.

We acknowledge that the number of participants developing hypoglycaemia in our study was relatively low. With a larger number of participants it is plausible that all correction formulas would have yielded significant changes in the QTc during hypoglycaemia. In addition, the study population was of greater age and had a longer duration of diabetes than the typical dead-in-bed patient. The sympathoadrenal response to hypoglycaemia causing QT lengthening declines with age and the duration of diabetes. Thus, the participants in this study may have been partly protected against significant QT prolongation, although a reduced adrenalin (epinephrine) response also increases the risk of severe hypoglycaemia. The fact that some participants in this study had diagnosed autonomic neuropathy could have affected the results, although such an effect was not observed. It has been hypothesised that autonomic neuropathy could cause increased QTc because of sympathetic overactivity [2]. On the other hand, neuropathy has been shown to attenuate the QTc interval prolongation during hypoglycaemia in type 1 diabetes [14].

Our use of automatic measurements of QT interval on single-lead ECG carries the possibility of inaccuracy, although we sought to minimise this risk by manual review of all measurements. Several factors could have altered the QT and RR measurements used for creating the participant-specific correction of QT. It has been shown that, because of hysteresis in the QT/RR relationship, the QT interval takes approximately 2 min to adapt to sudden changes in heart rate [22]. Additionally, participants in our study performed everyday activities in which conditions that affect the QT interval could have occurred. The degree to which these factors influenced our results is not known, but the relatively large number of measurements distributed over 72 h is expected to have minimised their effect. A circadian rhythm of QTc has been reported using fixed heart rate correction formulas, but it has been suggested that this merely reflects the circadian pattern of heart rate [11]. If a circadian pattern of QTc exists it may have affected our results, since it was not considered in our analysis.

The use of a CGM system could have resulted in false negative or false positive measurements of hypoglycaemia in our study. Bode et al. [23] tested the accuracy of the CGM system used in our study by pairing home blood glucose meter readings with CGM meter readings. The relative error between CGM and blood glucose measurements of 21.3% corresponds well with the 20.7% found in our study. The specificity of the CGM system for detecting hypoglycaemia (threshold, 3.9 mmol/l) is 90% [23], indicating that fewer than 10% of hypoglycaemia measurements are false positives. Using our threshold of >5 mmol/l for euglycaemia, the corresponding sensitivity of the CGM system is 91% [23], indicating that fewer than 9% of euglycaemia measurements are false negatives. These numbers show that the estimated number of misclassified CGM measurements in the present study was fewer than 10% of all measurements.

It is clearly important to clarify the possible proarrhythmic effect of hypoglycaemia and its role in relation to the dead-in-bed syndrome. Although hypoglycaemia in general was only associated with a modest prolongation of QTc, we observed a case of prolonged severe hypoglycaemia in which the QTc exceeded 500 ms. Larger trials investigating how QTc is altered by spontaneous hypoglycaemia are needed but should consider the effect of the heart rate correction formula as they may bias the results in either direction. If possible, participant-specific heart rate correction of QT should be considered in order to achieve QTc uncorrelated with heart rate.