Keywords

These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

1 The Call for Applications in Mathematics Curriculum is Quite Old!

Everything flows. The way of teaching mathematics undergoes steady changes. To be more precise, there are two aspects in particular that have been altered over the years at a slow but steady pace. Firstly, one strives to simplify the teaching subjects. … Secondly, the approach to teaching mathematics increasingly seeks to adapt the needs of everyday life. This effort is reflected especially in the selection and status of the so-called real-world problems. (Heinrich Kempinsky, 1928, p. 9. Translation by author).

Today, applications are in vogue: Even if there were always oscillations between utilitarian periods and puristic periods in teaching mathematics over the last 100 years (Kaiser, 1995; Niss, 2000) and, after a “weaker period” in the 1990s, today applications are brought back into focus by the TIMSS and PISA discussion – as one can see by newer German schoolbooks or in recent German curricula and didactical journals.

But applications still miss out in the mainstream classroom at least in German secondary schools: For years we asked freshmen at the Technical University of Braunschweig: “What applications of mathematics do you remember from school?” The most common answer was: “None.” Often applications were mentioned as pure mathematical applications like curve sketching as an “application of differential calculus.” There are still only a few research studies to support this thesis, but a lot of direct or indirect evidence by many studies (e.g., BIQUA, 2007; Kaiser, 1999; Hiebert et al., 2003; Hugener, 2008; Neubrand, 2004; Stigler and Hiebert, 1999).

The burning issue is: Will there be changes made by the new German standards-based curricula (i.e., Bildungsstandards, KMK, 2004; the Kerncurricula e.g., for Niedersachen, Niedersächsisches Kultusministerium, 2006)? Not necessarily, we think, and particularly not automatically, as the following analysis will show.

2 A First Root Cause Analysis: Focusing on Teachers

How teachers make sense of their professional world, the knowledge and beliefs they bring with them to the task, and how teachers’ understanding of teaching, learning, children, and the subject matter informs their everyday practice are important questions that need an investigation of the cognitive and affective aspects of teachers’ professional lives (Calderhead, 1996, p. 709).

Is it possible to identify issues that can explain the gap between the educational demands for applications and modelling and the instructional practice in the classroom? We use the following didactic triangle (see Fig. 8.1) as a simple model for teachers’ actions (Tietze et al., 1997, pp. 74 f.). The model incorporates the mathe­matical subject, students, and teacher and, as indicated by the arrows, the interactions between teacher and students, and within the group of students and the involvement of the teacher and the student with the subject. From the qualitative point of view, the dialogue between the persons involved and the subject matter is a dialogue that can change both person and subject matter. Last but not least, we take into account the conditions that frame school teaching.

Fig. 8.1
figure 1_8

Didactic triangle

The key persons in changing or reforming mathematics education and to apply new curricula are teachers (e.g., Fernandes, 1995; Wilson and Cooney, 2002). Focusing on teachers as the main actors for planning and performing teaching in school is just one possible perspective. One could also focus on the framing by investigating the effect of curriculum, assessment, or even social expectations (of parents, society, or even the business world) on mathematical teaching. One could focus on subject matter as the context of schoolbooks or learning material for teaching applications that also have a significant influence. Alternatively, one could focus on the students, including their mathematical competencies, their attitudes to applications and modelling, and the students’ expectations of their mathematical teaching as the research of Maaß (2004) has shown, a factor, that should not be underestimated.

For now, the focus will be on teachers as the main actors in the educational process. Two, at first glance, very simple questions arise: Do not teachers want to teach appli­cations and modelling in the classroom? Which would correspond with the teachers’ motives? Or: Cannot they teach applications and modelling? Taking into account the application competencies of the teachers and/or the objective or subjectively felt barriers that hinder them from teaching applications.

3 Why a Qualitative Case Study?: Methodology and Methods

Questionnaire-based quantitative studies of teachers’ cognitions and attitudes to applications and modelling show that the great majority have a quite positive attitude to applications and want to increase the number of applications but see a lot of barriers connected with applications in the classroom and even self-distrust of their own application competencies. Barriers mentioned are, for example, “too few materials for teaching applications,” “applications are hard stuff and therefore only relevant for high-performers,” “applications are difficult to assess” and most of all: “There is not enough teaching time for applications” (e.g., Grigutsch et al., 1998; Humenberger, 1997; Tietze, 1990, 1992; Zimmermann, 2002).

From this quantitative research, a lot of questions remain open, especially: If teachers want to teach applications and modelling in the classroom, why do they not create the framing conditions to reach their goals? And that’s where the qualitative study starts with the following research questions: (1) What are the teachers’ reasons to integrate or to ignore modelling in their teaching practice? (2) Is it ­possible to identify issues that can explain the gap between the educational demands of modelling and the instructional practice?

3.1 Theoretical Constructs

The research is based on the following constructs: Teaching and planning of teaching are actions (and not behaviour) of teachers (Theory of action, Hofer, 1986). Sources and reasons for actions are not observable, but have to be reconstructed by interpretation (Interpretative paradigm, Wilson, 1973). We have an epistemological conception of man (psychology of the reflexive subject, Groeben and Scheele, 1977, 2000), which means especially that researcher and researched are structurally equal, so it is possible to communicate about the reasons and intentions of the teachers – and to validate these reconstructions.

Building on results of research on beliefs (Leder et al., 2002; Thompson, 1984, 1992), the aspects of research (see Fig. 8.2) are the following: We look for “global instructional goals,” the “picture of mathematics,” and the “reasons for or against applications,” as well as the connections between them. To describe these aspects, we use the following descriptive constructs, which are explained below: “Subjective theories” as a background theory for beliefs, “goal hierarchies” and again the “picture of mathematics” as a descriptive tool. All together, we call it the subjective structure of a teacher.

Fig. 8.2
figure 2_8

Subjective structures

Subjective theories resemble scientific theories in structure as well as in function (explanation, prediction, technology) but are, in comparison to scientific theories, less coherent and consistent, are usually implicit and not explicit, and have an important function of orientation for the teachers (Groeben and Scheele, 1977, 2000). Furthermore, most importantly, they are subjective and not objective theories.

To structure the subjective theories we use goal hierarchies, which we gain by goal-tool-argumentation (“Ziel-Mittel-Argumentation” according to König, 1975; q.v. Scheele and Groeben, 1988). Added are specific assumptions about the structure and the function of subjective theories (Groeben et al., 1988) which we use but will not mention any further in this chapter.

A brief example is given. From an interview there is the statement: “Applications motivate the students.” This statement is now transferred into a descriptive sentence: “If one wants to motivate students one can teach applications.” From this description, two prescriptive sentences can be derived: “One should motivate students” and “One should teach applications” as shown in schema in Fig. 8.3.

Fig. 8.3
figure 3_8

An example of goal-tool-argumentation

So, we have “motivation” as a goal and the “teaching of applications” as a tool to achieve “motivation” in this hierarchy. By adding further information from the interview, we can develop a chain of arguments and a more or less complex goal-tool-structure – either in search of higher goals (goal-perspective) or in search of suitable tools (tool-perspective).

To sum up with the example, the teacher might have also said: “Applications convey a representative picture of mathematics”. There are at least two ways to interpret this and you have to reconstruct from the data, which interpretation is adequate. (1) To teach a representative picture of mathematics and to motivate the students are goals at the same level and for both goals the teaching of applications is a suitable tool. (2) To teach a representative picture of mathematics is the main goal. The tool for this goal (i.e., applications) is now becoming a goal for the next level. The first interpretation of the sentence “Applications motivate the students” is altered to “If you teach applications you motivate students” which means application leads to motivation as a consequence. The important difference between these interpretations is that in the first case we can see applications as a tool and in the second one as a goal in the hierarchy.

Note: Due to space restriction, we leave out an explication of the construct “picture of mathematics” in this chapter – roughly speaking, it is a mixture of beliefs about mathematics and content knowledge of applications (see Förster, 2008).

3.2 Study Design

Understanding action as an inner process depending on situations determines an inquiry in the form of case studies (Stake, 2000). The definition of the cases is according to theoretical sampling (Glaser and Strauss, 1967). The main study involved the questioning of eight in-service teachers grade 7–13 of secondary schools (A-level) in Northern Germany with several years of teaching experience (at least 2, up to 20 years). Data were mainly collected by (in-depth) interviews up to 4 h with open and semi-structured parts. The interviews were prepared by evaluating a standardized questionnaire (mainly statistical data such as age of the teacher, type of school, and also schoolbooks and teaching material used).

Interpretation was based on an adoption of qualitative content analysis (Mayring, 1995) and methods of qualitative teaching research (e.g., Corbin and Strauss, 2008; Jungwirth and Krummheuer, 2008) and went through the following steps: transcription of the interview, sequential interpretation of the transcription (to gain the subjective theories), summing up global analysis (to gain patterns in the goal hierarchy), intrapersonal description of the teachers (to gain the subjective structure of the teacher), and interpersonal analysis (to gain different types of ­subjective structures).

4 Discussion and Some Selected Results

The following discussion focuses on one main result of the study. The process of interpretation of the interviews is not outlined. While primarily one case will be discussed, some results of other cases are used to complete the case description.

Figure 8.4 shows an “overview map” derived from the goal-tool-structure of Teacher A representing the global instructional goals, as part of the subjective structure. Omitted are subsidiary goals, the connections between the two excerpts, as well as most of the substructure of the connections within the excerpts.

Fig. 8.4
figure 4_8

Global goals of Teacher A (excerpts)

Teacher A’s main instructional goals are significance for the future and fulfillment of curriculum plan. Significance for the future means university and vocational preparation and school attainment. Firstly, students have to learn logical thinking. This goes with the transfer hypothesis that logical thinking in mathematics leads to logical thinking towards everyday-life problems. That of course is objectively not true – but that does not matter to Teacher A as long as his subjective structure is coherent! (An explanation of why in the subjective structure of Teacher A’s logical thinking can be achieved by accuracy in working would take too long to explain here, but it “makes sense” within the subjective structure (see Förster, 2008).

Looking at the School attainment: For Teacher A “no one is left behind” in his teaching – to ensure students pass the examinations means repetition and exercising and training of techniques as a stockpiling of knowledge. This altogether needs a lot of teaching time – which of course is not an explicit goal, but a necessary tool in the goal-tool hierarchy – and the needed time is missing for other goals.

Where are the applications? We look at the right side of Fig. 8.4: Fulfillment of plan goes hand in hand with the goal (!) to motivate the students for the content they have to learn at any rate. One way to motivate the students is to teach simple everyday-life applications – simple enough to motivate. However, Teacher A can also show the significance of mathematics to be motivated for example by episodes of mathematical history, which can be told to the students “in passing.” Last but not least, the enthusiasm of the teacher can motivate by his choosing the right ­packaging for mathematical content. We will come back to these aspects, but first have a look at the type of applications Teacher A uses in his teaching: Everyday life ­applications have to be simple to understand (nonmathematical background) and simple to teach (teaching time). As a consequence, there can be no complex modelling. Examples are height determinations of trees, simple financial mathematics (especially interest calculations), volume of a conically shaped wine glass, volume of a cigar, the Pythagorean knotted rope, pieces of cake (fractions) and some applications in physics. These examples correspond very well with Teacher A’s subjective definition of application derived from the interviews: “Everything (!), that students know from their everyday life (or can at least imagine) and that can be associated with mathematics, is (!) an interesting application.”

This brings us to the right packaging: Teacher A mentions a task from a schoolbook: In a picture, an expander is hanging from the ceiling of the room. By Hooke’s law with a proportional function the elongation of the expander, respectively, the weight-force, can be computed by elongation  =  const. × weight of person. After a short explanation of the expander and after introducing “Silke and Dirk,” this is a quite normal, fairly boring word problem. So what’s the point to Teacher A that’s worth mentioning this task in the interview? Teacher A transfers the situation in the picture into his classroom. He brings an expander with him. He lets the student try out the expander and finally he screws the expander to the ceiling of the classroom and comes to the same questions – but with the real expander. Surely not a real-world problem, but his students are interested by the packaging. As mentioned before, time for applications is limited by the main goals – so for this teacher applications are merely a tool for motivation.

To explain (or at least illustrate) the origin of the three different types of “teacher’s subjective substructures according to applications,” we are closing with a brief look at the position of applications in the hierarchy of goals of two other teachers and summarize these three types in the following Fig. 8.5.

Fig. 8.5
figure 5_8

Position of applications in the hierarchy of goals

Teacher B sees applications as a central goal of teaching corresponding with the goal (and tool) of teaching applied mathematics. The goals have higher aims as empowered citizen, problem solving (inside and outside mathematics), a positive attitude towards mathematics, and learning to ask questions. This teacher also has high professional pretension and he gets the needed teaching time by using the handheld computer as a tool in the hands of the students. Motivation is not a goal, but a consequence of the other goals.

In contrast, Teacher C is a structuralist sensu (Eichler, 2007), happy with context-free mathematics. Therefore, you have to search for applications, which are merely a tool for illustrating mathematical content and quite isolated from the rest of the goal hierarchy.

The first column of Fig. 8.5 leads to attaching word problems to a traditional form of mathematical teaching without modelling. The second propagates context-free problems – and sporadic use of applications – corresponding with very high, nearly unrealisable expectations in terms of a realistic context for the applications. The third approach allows more complex applications, sometimes corresponding with high expectations of the mathematics involved in the applications. For type one and three, there will be no search for tools to overcome the barriers that hinder these teachers to have applications in their teaching.

5 Conclusion

Quite clearly, there is a fundamental need to understand everything that underlies the way in which mathematics teachers approach their subject before suggestions and recommendations concerning good classroom practice can be made. (Eichler, 2007, p. 208).

There is an unexpected high consistency between the picture of mathematics, the global goals concerning teaching and the selection, and reasons for or against applications. It was not expected to be so clearly defined. This is also important because one aspect of the research on teachers’ beliefs is the conclusion that they have a high impact on students’ beliefs (Chapman, 2001). And, the students of today are the teachers of tomorrow. Motivation is the dominant argument for applications. The assumption, mathematics is per se formative toward active, creative, and flexible individuals, makes applications as an independent and clear goal abundant. And, therefore potential applicability of mathematics is sufficient for these teachers – in correspondence with their picture of mathematics. Realistic modelling and further educational demands do not play any (important) role in the classroom and there is often a mixture of applications and applied mathematics.

Interesting is the role of the second teaching subject of the German teachers. We expected higher competence in applications especially with physics and other natural science teachers, but we also found limiting factors as the immense time exposure for the second subject (especially physics) when taught as an experimental subject and different approaches that teachers have to applications in their different subjects – “applications in physics: of course” but “applications in mathematics: no need for them.”

Tertiary Education has effects on teaching of applications: For instance, application examples often come from the teachers’ own university education. Also the German traineeship for teachers has effects, because frequently the trainees are encouraged by the instructors to teach applications. However, if the closer contact with applications does not start until this traineeship, this “retrofitting” of competencies in applications is considered by the teachers as amateurish (dilettantish) and after their traineeship they will not teach applications any further. So a positive attitude to applications and knowledge about ­applications and modelling should be set up in school or in university study at the latest. Starting late will be too late!