Keywords

These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Introduction

The barn owl’s nucleus laminaris remains an outstanding preparation for analysing ITD coding. A major advantage is its large size and accessibility. The nuclei of the auditory brainstem occupy most of the floor of the fourth ventricle, allowing for precise mapping of ITDs in vivo. NL also is a useful preparation for the experimental manipulation of developmental events, because the barn owl’s head size more than doubles in size during the first month posthatch (Köppl et al. 2005). We have therefore used the neurophonic potential to measure the conduction times needed to create maps of ITD and developed a simple linear model of the delay-line conduction velocity.

Neurophonics are extracellular potentials well correlated with auditory stimuli and are found in mammals and birds (Köppl and Carr 2008; Sullivan and Konishi 1986; McLaughlin et al. 2010; Schwarz 1992). Although its precise source(s) is still uncertain and may differ between species (Köppl and Carr 2008; Mc Laughlin et al. 2010; Schwarz 1992), the neurophonic is generally accepted as a valid reflection of local activity within the NL.

Materials and Methods

The experiments were conducted at the Departments of Biology of the University Maryland and University of Oldenburg. Eighteen barn owls (Tyto alba) were used to collect the data presented in this and other studies (Wagner et al. 2005, 2009; Kuokkanen et al. 2010). Procedures conformed to NIH Guidelines for Animal Research and were approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the Universities of Maryland and Oldenburg. Anaesthesia was induced by intramuscular injections of 10–20 mg/kg ketamine hydrochloride and 3–4 mg/kg xylazine. Supplementary doses were administered to maintain a suitable plane of anaesthesia. Body temperature was maintained at 39 °C by a feedback-controlled heating blanket. For mapping experiments, electrodes were moved in defined amounts in the rostrocaudal and mediolateral axes. More details may be found in Wagner et al. (2009).

Recordings were made in a sound-attenuating chamber (IAC, New York). Tungsten electrodes were used, with impedances between 5 and 20 MW. A grounded silver chloride pellet, placed under the animal’s skin around the incision, served as the reference electrode. Amplified electrode signals were passed to a threshold ­discriminator (SD1 (Tucker-Davis Technologies (TDT) Gainesville, FL)) and an analogue-to-digital converter (DD1 (TDT)) connected to a personal computer via an optical interface (OI (TDT)). Acoustic stimuli were digitally generated by ­custom-made software (“Xdphys” written in Dr. M. Konishi’s lab at Caltech) ­driving a signal-processing board (DSP2 (TDT)). Acoustic signals were fed to miniature earphones and inserted into the owl’s left and right ear canals, respectively. At a given recording site, we measured frequency tuning, then tuning to ITD, and responses to monaural clicks. For most penetrations, an electrolytic lesion (1–10 μA, 3–10 s) was made at or near the response to a best ITD of 0 μs. After a survival time of 5–14 days, owls were perfused transcardially with saline, followed by 4 % paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffer. Brains were blocked in the same stereotaxic apparatus as for in vivo recordings, and sections through NL were reconstructed with the aid of a Neurolucida system.

Young owls were raised from about posthatching day 20 with earplugs designed by Gold and Knudsen (1999) to introduce a time delay in the 3–5 kHz inputs from one ear. Maps of ITD were investigated as above in owls raised with earplugs and lesions made as above.

Results

NL receives bilateral inputs from the nucleus magnocellularis (NM) such that axons from the ipsilateral NM enter NL dorsally, while contralateral axons enter from the ventral side. These afferents, and their synapses on NL neurons, generate the ­neurophonic, which varies systematically with position in NL (Fig. 24.1; Kuokkanen et al. 2010; Sullivan and Konishi 1986). From dorsal to ventral within NL, the best ITD of the neurophonic shifts from contralateral space to best ITDs around 0 μs and on to ipsilateral ITDs. Neurophonics were used to map best ITD, accompanied by small electrolytic lesions, generally at the location of the response to 0 μs ITD. Based on our 3D reconstructions of NL architecture and our measures of delay, we generated an explanation of how conduction velocity of the incoming delay-line axons from NM creates these maps and used modeling to constrain measurements of conduction velocity.

Fig. 24.1
figure 00241

(a) 5 kHz tone stimulus (top) and neurophonic response (bottom). (b) 5 ms interval of the neurophonic at tone onset (Modified from Kuokkanen et al. 2010)

Dorsoventral Maps of ITD in Owls Derived from Tone-Induced Responses

Within NL, binaurally evoked neurophonics vary with interaural phase difference (IPD), as do ITD-sensitive responses recorded from NL neurons (Carr and Konishi 1990; Peña et al. 2001). The ITD that evokes the maximum neurophonic response changes systematically, always shifting towards ipsilateral space with increasing depth (Sullivan and Konishi 1986); Fig 24.2a). These location-dependent shifts in best ITD occur only within NL; lesions marking the onset of the systematic shift are found on the dorsal edge of the nucleus, and those marking the end of the phase shift are on the ventral edge (Sullivan and Konishi 1986).

Fig. 24.2
figure 00242

(a) Neurophonic responses recorded at 200 μm intervals in a single penetration through NL. (b) Reconstructed section through NL, showing location of lesions placed at 0 μs ITDs in multiple penetrations and revealing that iso-ITD lines were not parallel to the borders of NL. Inset: schematic owl head showing range of ITDs (von Campenhausen and Wagner 2006)

We measured the changes in the neurophonic obtained when traversing the short, dorsoventral axis of NL. From dorsal to ventral within NL, the phase delay of a contralaterally elicited potential decreased and that of its ipsilateral counterpart increased (Sullivan and Konishi 1986). This neurophonic delay reflects the delay of phase-locked spikes originating from interdigitating ipsi- and contralateral axons from NM and generates an orderly representation of delay disparities.

NL is tonotopically organized, and its isofrequency laminae are oriented along a plane that is inclined about 45° to the midline. We quantified the distribution of ITDs within a single isofrequency lamina by stereotactically sampling ITD at multiple locations within this plane. Each isofrequency lamina contained a map of optimal binaural delays. The map is best illustrated by iso-delay contours (Fig. 24.2b).

Medial portions of each tonotopic lamina mapped best IPDs and ITDs near 0 μs dorsally in NL, with a representation of ipsilateral space below (Fig. 24.2b). The central portions of the tonotopic lamina contained maps of best ITD that were centered on frontal space. The most lateral lesions were characterized by representations of contralateral space, and the most extreme lateral positions did not include a representation of 0 μs. Thus, there was a steady shift in the mapping of 0 μs from dorsal in medial NL to ventral in lateral NL.

ITD at 0 μs Can Be Derived from Latencies of Click-Induced Responses

Since the representation of frontal space shifted systematically to more ventral ­locations with mediolateral progression within each tonotopic band, we measured delay latency at different mediolateral positions to determine the basis of the shift and to show how regulation of conduction velocity could underlie map formation. Click stimuli provide a useful measure of conduction delay, since click phase is precise (Wagner et al. 2005). We compared the neurophonic responses to ipsi- and contralateral clicks at best ITDs at or near 0 μs and observed overlaid, phase-locked but not identical ipsi- and contralateral click responses (Fig. 24.3a).

Fig. 24.3
figure 00243

(a) Left and right neurophonic click responses recorded at 0 μs best ITD. (b) Model of neurophonic conduction velocity accounts for observed shifts in best ITD. Mediolateral lengths are marked l1-4, while dorsoventral lengths are marked d1-4

Models of Conduction Velocity and Maps of ITD

We developed a simple linear model of NL that incorporated click delays (Fig. 24.3b). Conduction velocities along the delay lines inside (d1–d4) and outside NL (L1–L4) were assumed to be different but constant, consistent with our measures of axon diameter and internodal distance (Carr and Konishi 1990; Carr and Boudreau 1993). Also, we assumed a common path from NM to the midline, followed by a measured path from the midline to the edges of NL. Axon lengths were estimated using a predefined straight-line geometry, derived from reconstructions of axon paths. The parameters of this geometry were fit to three-dimensional reconstructions of the NL borders and the locations of lesions reconstructed from counterstained sections.

The model was used to evaluate possible conduction velocities along the delay lines in NL. The model yielded a range of possible conduction velocities lying between 1 and 10 m/s with velocities within the nucleus being smaller than those outside NL. The model also showed that iso-ITD lines cannot run parallel to the dorsal and ventral borders of NL in the mediolateral direction (Sullivan and Konishi 1986), because parallel iso-ITD lines yielded biologically unrealistic conduction velocities, which is consistent with our experimentally observed iso-ITD contours.

Plasticity in Maps of ITD

Young owls (from P21) were unilaterally fitted with an ear-canal insert designed to introduce a time delay to inputs from that ear (Gold and Knudsen 1999). After growing up with this altered auditory experience, the representation of ITD in NL was mapped using neurophonic responses and tested for any differences to normal. Our hypothesis was that if NL adjusts for the temporally altered input, the maps of ITD would be shifted compared to normal. In addition, complementary shifts are expected in the NL of both sides. Specifically, on the side ipsilateral to the ear insert, the point of zero ITD should shift ventrally and dorsally on the contralateral side. Preliminary analysis did not reveal any shifts in the predicted, adaptive ­direction. This suggests that nucleus laminaris does not show experience-­dependent plasticity, at least not after P20–30. The data are consistent with the observation that myelination of the delay lines peaks at the third week posthatch (Cheng and Carr 2007).

Discussion

Based on NL architecture and our results, we have generated an explanation of how conduction velocity of the incoming delay-line axons from NM affects maps of ITD in NL, and we have used a computational model to constrain estimates of NM axonal conduction velocity.

In the barn owl and chicken, the bilateral projection from NM to NL conforms to the requirements of the Jeffress model. In the barn owl, NM axons innervate dorsoventral arrays of neurons in NL in a sequential fashion. For each frequency band, recordings from these interdigitating ipsilateral and contralateral axons show regular changes in delay with depth in NL (Carr and Konishi 1990). These conduction delays are similar to the ±250 μs range of ITDs available to the barn owl (von Campenhausen and Wagner 2006).

The delay-line circuit in the chicken is organized on the same plan as the circuit described for the barn owl, except that the chicken NL is not a large nucleus, but a monolayer of bipolar cells oriented in the mediolateral dimension of the brainstem (Parks and Rubel 1975; Young and Rubel 1983; Seidl et al. 2010). In the barn owl and chicken, NL cells receive input from the ipsilateral NM axons, which splay out to innervate NL neurons with approximately equal lengths to each cell, so that ipsilateral inputs arrive fairly simultaneously along the mediolateral extent of NL. The contralateral axons act as delay lines; each axon runs along the ventral surface of NL, giving off collateral branches in the nucleus. Patterns of axons in the cat medial superior olive are very similar to those of the chicken, in that axons from the contralateral ventral cochlear nucleus project across the rostrocaudal axis of the nucleus, while the ipsilateral axons form a less organized projection (Smith et al. 1993; Beckius et al. 1999). The advantage of electrophysiology in birds is that NL is fairly straight and delays can therefore be mapped within a single tonotopic band. We therefore were able to measure delays with a high enough precision to determine how continuous maps of IPD are formed. We found that the three-dimensional network of delays representing response latencies is well suited for the derivation of IPDs by coincidence detection, with long delays in the dorsoventral dimension and short delays in the mediolateral dimension.