Skip to main content

Fundamental Behavioral Methods and Findings in Classical Conditioning

  • Chapter
A Neuroscientist’s Guide to Classical Conditioning

Abstract

This chapter is intended to provide a solid foundation in the methods and findings of classical conditioning at a behavioral level. We take readers from an introduction to the basics of classical conditioning through to its more complex and recent findings that appear related to higher-order processes that are commonly labeled as attention, perception, and even cognition. Many textbook descriptions of classical conditioning give the mistaken impression that it is an area of completed science. In fact, the methods of classical conditioning have continued to evolve, and the range of important phenomena has continued to expand. Although this chapter primarily describes the empirical methods and outcomes of classical conditioning, we introduce important theoretical principles along the way. These principles have proven crucial in organizing research findings and in linking classical conditioning to other learning phenomena. In addition, our introduction to behavioral theory should help the reader place the phenomena described in this chapter in the context of the theories described in more detail in the next chapter (Brandon, Vogel, & Wagner, this volume).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 89.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ashton, A.B., Bitgood, S.C., and Moore, J.W. (1969). Auditory differential conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response: III. Effects of US shock intensity and duration. Psychonomic Science, 15, 127–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ayres, J.B., Benedict, J.O., and Witcher, E. (1975). Systematic manipulation of individual events in a truly random control in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 97–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ayres, J.J., Moore, J.W., and Vigorito, M. (1984). Hall and Pearce negative transfer: assessments in conditioned suppression and nictitating membrane conditioning experiments. Animal Learning & behavior, 12, 428–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A.D. (1997). Human memory: theory and practice (revised edition). Hove, England: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailo, A.G. (1985). Learning by statistical cooperation of self-interested neuron-like computing elements. Human Neurobiology, 4, 229–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belasco, J.A., and Trice, H.M. (1969). The assessment of change in training and therapy. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bellingham, W.P., and Gillette, K. (1981). Spontaneous configuring to a tone-light compound using appetitive training. Learning and Motivation, 12, 420–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bellingham, W.P., Gillette-Bellingham, K., and Kehoe, E.J. (1985). Summation and configuration in patterning schedules with the rat and rabbit. Animal Learning & Behavior, 13, 152–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blazis, D.E.J., and Moore, J.W. (1991). Conditioned stimulus duration in classical trace conditioning: test of a real-time neural network model. Behavioral Brain Research, 43, 73–78.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bonardi, C. (1996). Transfer of occasion setting: the role of generalization decrement. Animal Learning & behavior, 24, 277–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boring, E.G. (1950). A history of experimental psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandon, S.E., and Wagner, A.R. (1991). Modulation of a discrete Pavlovian conditioned reflex by a putative emotive Pavlovian conditioned stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, 299–311.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brelsford, J., Jr., and Theios, J. (1965). Single session conditioning of the nictitating membrane in the rabbit: effect of intertriai interval. Psychonomic Science, 2, 81–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Britton, G., and Farley, J. (1999). Behavioral and neural bases of noncoincidence learning in Hermissenda. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 9126–9132.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brogden, W.J. (1939). Sensory preconditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 323–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bromage, B.K., and Scavio, M.J., Jr. (1978). Effects of an aversive CS+ and CS-under deprivation upon successive classical appetitive and aversive conditioning. Animal Learning & behavior, 6, 57–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J.S., Kalish, H.I., and Farber, I.E. (1951). Conditioned fear as revealed by magnitude of startle response to an auditory stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 317–328.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, T. (1820/1977). Sketch of a system of the philosophy of the human mind, Edinburgh. In D. N. Robinson (Ed.), Significant contributions to the history of psychology. Washington, DC: University Press of America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, A. (1965). UCS properties in classical conditioning of the albino rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 186–192.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buhusi, C.V., and Schmajuk, N.A. (1999). Timing in simple conditioning and occasion setting: a neural network approach. Behavioral Processes, 45, 33–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buonomano, D.V., and Mauk, M.D. (1994). Neural network model of the cerebellum: temporal discrimination and the timing of motor responses. Neural Computation, 6, 38–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buresova, O., and Bures, J. (1974). Functional decortication in the CS-US interval decreases the efficiency of taste aversive learning. Behavioral Biology, 12, 357–364.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Canli, T., Detmer, W.M., and Donegan, N.H. (1992). Potentiation or diminution of discrete motor unconditioned responses (rabbit eyeblink) to an aversive Pavlovian unconditioned stimulus by two associative processes: Conditioned fear and a conditioned diminution of unconditioned stimulus processing. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, 498–508.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carew, T.J., Walters, E.T., and Kandel, E.R. (1981). Classical conditioning in a simple withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. Journal of Neuroscience, 1, 1426–1437.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, M.E., and Hupka, R.B. (1974). The effects of stimulus duration and frequency of daily preconditioning stimulus exposures on latent inhibition in Pavlovian conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4, 225–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, S.R., and Gormezano, I. (1971). Classical conditioning of the rabbit’s (Oryctolagus cuniculus) nictitating membrane response under symmetrical CS-US interval shifts. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 77, 447–455.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cox, J. (1990). Conditioned response topography in the rabbit nictitating membrane response as a function of conditioned stimulus intensity and duration: implications for models of adaptive timing. Honours thesis, Sydney: University of New South Wales.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. (1986). Pharmacological and anatomical analysis of fear conditioning using the potentiated startle paradigm. Behavioral Neuroscience, 100, 808–818.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deaux, E., and Gormezano, I. (1963). Eyeball retraction: classical conditioning and extinction in the albino rabbit. Science, 141, 630–631.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Desmond, J.E. (1990). Temporal adaptive responses in neural models: the stimulus trace. In M. Gabriel and J. W. Moore (Eds.), Learning and computational neuroscience (pp. 421–456). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Desmond, J.E., and Moore, J.W. (1988). Adaptive timing in neural networks: the conditioned response. Biological Cybernetics, 58, 405–415.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Desmond, J.E., and Moore, J.W. (1991). Altering the synchrony of stimulus trace processes: tests of a neural-network model. Biological Cybernetics, 65, 161–169.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Donegan, N.H., and Wagner, A.R. (1987). Conditioned diminution and facilitation of the UR: a sometimes opponent-process interpretation. In I. Gormezano, W.F. Prokasy, and R.F. Thompson (Eds.), Classical conditioning. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egger, D.M., and Miller, N.E. (1962). Secondary reinforcement in rats as a function of information value and reliability of the stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 174–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, H. (1965). The transfer of learning. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitzgerald, R.F. (1963). Effects of partial reinforcement with acid on the classically conditioned salivary response in dogs. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56, 1056–1060.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, P., Maisiak, R., and Dugue, G. (1976). Unconditional stimulus characteristics in rabbit eyelid conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2, 175–190.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, P.W., and Misfeldt, T.J. (1967). Rabbit eyelid conditioning as a function of the intertrial interval. Psychonomic Science, 9, 137–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, J., Ervin, F.R., and Koelling, R.A. (1966). Learning with prolonged delay of reinforcement. Psychonomic Science, 5, 121–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, CM., Latham, S.B., and Gormezano, I. (1978). Classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response: effects of reinforcement and resistance to extinction. Animal Learning & behavior, 6, 209–215.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, CM., Cool, V., Land, T., Kehoe, E.J., and Gormezano, I. (1991). Second-order conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response: interstimulus interval and frequency of CS-CS pairings. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science, 26, 282–295.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gluck, M.A., and Bower, G.H. (1988). Evaluating an adaptive network model of human learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 166–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gluck, M.A., and Bower, G.H. (1990). Component and pattern information in adaptive networks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 105–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gormezano, I. (1966). Classical conditioning. In J.B. Sidowski (Ed.), Experimental methods and instrumentation in psychology (pp. 385–420). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gormezano, I. (1972). Investigations of defense and reward conditioning in the rabbit. In A.H. Black and W.F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning. II: Current research and theory (pp. 151–181). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gormezano, I., and Coleman, S.R. (1975). extinction of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Pavlovian Journal of Biological Sciences, 10, 13–22.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gormezano, I., and Kehoe, E.J. (1975). Classical conditioning: some methodological-conceptual issues. In W.K. Estes (Ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gormezano, I., and Kehoe, E.J. (1981). Classical conditioning and the law of contiguity. In P.M. Harzern and M.D. Zeiler (Eds.), Advances in analysis of behavior, Vol. 2. Predictability, correlation, and contiguity (pp. 1–45). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gormezano, I., Kehoe, E.J., and Marshall, B.S. (1983). Twenty years of classical conditioning research with the rabbit. In J.M. Sprague and A.N. Epstein (Eds.), Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology (pp. 197–275). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grant, D.A. (1943). Sensitization and association in eyelid conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 201–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graves, CA., and Solomon, P.R. (1985). Age-related disruption of trace but not delay classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Behavioral Neuroscience, 99, 88–96.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, T.S., McMaster, S.E., Harvey, J.A., and Gormezano, I. (1981). Localization of retractor bulbi motoneurons in the rabbit. Brain Research, 226, 93–106.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grevert, P., and Moore, W. (1970). The effects of unpaired US presentations on conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response: consolidation or contingency. Psychonomic Science, 20, 177–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg, S., and Schmajuk, N.A. (1989). Neural dynamics of adaptive timing and temporal discrimination during associative learning. Neural Networks, 2, 79–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E.R. (1930). Conditioning as a principle of learning. Psychological Review, 37, 412–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haberlandt, K., Hamsher, K., and Kennedy, A.W. (1978). Spontaneous recovery in rabbit eyelid conditioning. Journal of General Psychology, 98, 241–244.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J.F. (1976). Classical conditioning and instrumental learning: a contemporary approach. Philadelphia: Lippincott.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H.F. (1949). The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review, 56, 51–65.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heinemann, E.G., and Chase, S. (1975). Stimulus generalization. In W.K. Estes (Ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (pp. 305–349). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hesketh, B. (1997). Dilemmas in training for transfer and retention. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 46, 317–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilgard, E.R., and Marquis, D.G. (1940). Conditioning and learning. New York: Appleton-Century.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinson, R.E. (1982). Effects of UCS preexposure on excitatory and inhibitory rabbit eyelid conditioning: an associative effect of conditioned contextual stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 8, 49–61.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hoehler, F.K., and Leonard, D.W. (1976). Double responding in classical nictitating membrane conditioning with single-CS dual-ISI pairing. Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, 11, 180–190.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hoehler, F.K., and Leonard, D.W. (1981). Motivated vs. associated role of the US in classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Animal Learning & Behavior, 9, 239–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, P.C. (1983). Occasion-setting in Pavlovian feature positive discriminations. In M.L. Commons, R.J. Herrnstein, and A.R. Wagner (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of behavior: discrimination processes (pp. 182–206). New York: Ballinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, P.C. (1992). Occasion setting in Pavlovian conditioning. In D.L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 69–125). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, P.C., Hamlin, P.A., and Parsons, J.P. (1997). Temporal specificity in serial feature-positive discrimination learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 23, 95–109.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Holt, P.E., and Kehoe, E.J. (1985). Cross-modal transfer as a function of similarities between training tasks in classical conditioning of the rabbit. Animal Learning & Behavior, 13, 51–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C.L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hume, D. (1969). A treatise of human nature. New York: Penguin Books. (Original work published 1739.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hupka, R.B., Kwaterski, S.E., and Moore, J.W. (1970). Conditioned diminution of the UCR: differences between the human eyeblink and the rabbit nictitating membrane response. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83, 45–51.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ison, J.R., and Leonard, D.W. (1971). Effects of auditory stimuli on the amplitude of the nictitating membrane reflex of the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 75, 157–164.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kamin, L.J. (1968). “Attention-like” processes in classical conditioning. In M.R. Jones (Ed.), Miami symposium on the prediction of behavior: aversive stimulation (pp. 9–31). Miami: University of Miami Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kandel, E.R., Abrams, T., Bernier, L., Carew, T.J., Hawkins, R.D., and Schwartz, J.H. (1983). Classical conditioning and sensitization share aspects of the same molecular cascade in Aplysia. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia in Quantitative Biology, 48, 821–830.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J. (1979). The role of CS-US contiguity in classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response to serial stimuli. Learning and Motivation, 10, 23–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J. (1982). Overshadowing and summation in compound stimulus conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 8, 313–328.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J. (1983). CS-US contiguity and CS intensity in conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response to serial compound stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 307–319.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J. (1986). Summation and configuration in conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response to compound stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 12, 186–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J. (1988). A layered network model of associative learning: learning-to-learn and configuration. Psychological Review, 95, 411–433.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J. (1992). Versatility in conditioning: a layered network model. In D.S. Levine and S.J. Levin (Eds.), Motivation, emotion and goal direction in neural networks (pp. 63–90). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J. (1998). Can the whole be something other than the sum of its parts? In C.D.L. Wynne and J.E.R. Staddon (Eds.), Models of action: mechanisms for adaptive behavior (pp. 87–126). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., and Gormezano, I. (1980). Configuration and combination laws in conditioning with compound stimuli. Psychological Bulletin, 87, 351–378.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., and Graham, P. (1988). Summation and configuration in negative patterning of the rabbit’s conditioned nictitating membrane response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 320–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., and Holt, P.E. (1984). Transfer across CS-US intervals and sensory modalities in classical conditioning in the rabbit. Animal Learning & behavior, 12, 122–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., and Macrae, M. (1994). Classical conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane can be fast or slow: implications for Lennartz and Weinberger’s (1992) two-factor theory. Psychobiology, 22, 1–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., and Macrae, M. (1997). Savings in animal learning: implications for relapse and maintenance after therapy. Behavior Therapy, 28, 141–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., and Morrow, L.D. (1984). Temporal dynamics of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response in serial compound conditioned stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 205–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., and Napier, R.M. (1991). In the blink of an eye: real-time stimulus factors in delay and trace conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 43B, 257–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., and Schreurs, B.G. (1986). Compound-component differentiation as a function of CS-US interval and CS duration in the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Animal Learning & behavior, 14, 144–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., and Weidemann, G. (1999a). Time course of conditioned responses in stimulus compounding in the rabbit nictitating membrane preparation. Sydney, Australia: University of New South Wales.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., and Weidemann, G. (1999b). Within-stimulus competition in trace conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Psychobiology, 27, 72–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., Gibbs, CM., Garcia, E., and Gormezano, I. (1979). Associative transfer and stimulus selection in classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response to serial compound CSs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 5, 1–18.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., Feyer, A., and Moses, J.L. (1981a). Second-order conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response as a function of the CS2-CS1 and CS 1-US intervals. Animal Learning & behavior, 9, 304–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., Schreurs, B.G., and Amodei, N. (1981b). Blocking acquisition of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response to serial conditioned stimuli. Learning and Motivation, 72, 92–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., Morrow, L.D., and Holt, P.E. (1984). General transfer across sensory modalities survives reductions in the original conditioned reflex in the rabbit. Animal Learning & behavior, 12, 129–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., Marshall-Goodell, B., and Gormezano, I. (1987a). Differential conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response to serial compound stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 13, 17–30.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., Schreurs, B.G., and Graham, P. (1987b). Temporal primacy overrides prior training in serial compound conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Animal Learning & behavior, 15, 455–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., Graham-Clarke, P., and Schreurs, B.G. (1989). Temporal patterns of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response to compound and component stimuli under mixed CS-US intervals. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 283–295.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., Cool, V., and Gormezano, I. (1991). Trace conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response as a function of CS-US interstimulus interval and trials per session. Learning and Motivation, 22, 269–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., Home, P.S., and Home, A.J. (1993). Discrimination learning using different CS-US intervals in classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Psychobiology, 21, 277–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., Home, A.J., Home, P.S., and Macrae, M. (1994). Summation and configuration between and within sensory modalities in classical conditioning of the rabbit. Animal Learning & behavior, 22, 19–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., Home, A.J., and Macrae, M. (1995a). Learning to learn: real-time features and a connectionist model. Adaptive behavior, 3, 235–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., Schreurs, B.G., Macrae, M., and Gormezano, I. (1995b). duration on the classically conditioned rabbit nictitating membrane response. Psychobiology, 23, 103–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J., Palmer, N., Weidemann, G., and Macrae, M. (2000). The effect of featuretarget intervals in conditional discriminations on acquisition and expression of conditioned nictitating membrane and heart-rate responses in the rabbit. Animal Learning & behavior, 28, 80–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kiernan, M.J., Westbrook, R.F., and Cranney, J. (1995). Immediate shock, passive avoidance, and potentiated startle: implications for the unconditioned response to shock. Animal Learning & behavior, 23, 22–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, K.-S. (1986). Effects of context manipulation on latent inhibition: a study on the nature of context in classical conditioning. Korean Journal of Psychology, 5, 75–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinkaide, P.S. (1974). Stimulus selection in eyelid conditioning in the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 86, 1132–1140.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Klopf, A.H. (1988). A neuronal model of classical conditioning. Psychobiology, 16, 85–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konorski, J. (1967). Integrative activity of the brain: an interdisciplinary approach. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kremer, E.F. (1978). The Rescorla-Wagner model: losses in associative strength in compound conditioned stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 4, 22–36.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K.S. (1916). The human salivary reflex and its use in psychology. Psychological Review, 23, 446–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lennartz, R.C., and Weinberger, N.M. (1992). Analysis of response systems in Pavlovian conditioning reveals rapidly versus slowly acquired conditioned responses: support for two factors, implications for behavior and neurobiology. Psychobiology, 20, 93–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, D.W. (1975). Partial reinforcement effects in classical aversive conditioning in rabbits and human beings. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 596–608.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, D.W., and Theios, J. (1967). Classical conditioning in rabbits under prolonged single alternation conditions of reinforcement. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 64, 273–276.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, D.W., Fischbein, L.C., and Monteau, J.E. (1972). The effects of interpolated US alone (USa) presentations on classical nictitating membrane conditioning in rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Conditional Reflex, 7, 107–114.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Levinthal, C.F. (1973). The CS-US interval function in rabbit nictitating membrane response conditioning: single vs multiple trials per conditioning session. Learning and Motivation, 4, 259–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levinthal, C.F., and Papsdorf, J.P. (1970). The classically conditioned nictitating membrane response: the CS-US interval function with one trial per day. Psychonomic Science, 21, 296–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinthal, C.F., Tartell, R.H., Margolin, CM., and Fishman, H. (1985). The CS-US interval (ISI) function in rabbit nictitating membrane response conditioning with very long intertriai intervals. Animal Learning & behavior, 13, 228–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levitan, L. (1975). Tests of the Rescorla-Wagner model of Pavlovian conditioning. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 6, 265–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, S., and Moore, J.W. (1969). Auditory differential conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response: IV. Training based on stimulus offset and the effect of an intertriai tone. Psychonomic Science, 15, 128–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lubow, R.E., and Moore, A.U. (1959). Latent inhibition: the effect of non-reinforced preexposure to the conditioned stimulus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52, 415–419.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N.J. (1975). A theory of attention: variation in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 82, 276–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macrae, M., and Kehoe, E.J. (1995). Transfer between conditional and discrete discriminations in conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response. Learning and Motivation, 26, 380–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macrae, M., and Kehoe, E.J. (1999). Savings after extinction in conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Psychobiology, 27, 85–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maleske, R.T., and Frey, P.W. (1979). Blocking in eyelid conditioning: effect of changing the CS-US interval and introducing an intertriai stimulus. Animal Learning & Behavior, 7, 452–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manning, A.A., Schneiderman, N., and Lordahl, D.S. (1969). Delay vs. trace heart rate classical discrimination conditioning in rabbits as a function of ISI. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80, 225–230.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marchant, H.G., III, and Moore, J.W. (1973). Blocking of the rabbit’s conditioned nictitating membrane response in Kamin’s two-stage paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 101, 155–158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marchant, H.G., Mis, F.W., and Moore, J.W. (1972). Conditioned inhibition of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 95, 408–411.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall Goodell, B., Kehoe, E.J., and Gormezano, I. (1992). Laws of the unconditioned reflex in the rabbit nictitating membrane preparation. Psychobiology, 20, 229–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mauk, M.D., and Ruiz, B.P. (1992). Learning-dependent timing of Pavlovian eyelid responses: differential conditioning using multiple interstimulus intervals. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, 666–681.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McGeoch, J.A. (1952). The psychology of human learning, 2nd ed. New York: Longmans, Green, & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNish, K.A., Betts, S.L., Brandon, S.E., and Wagner, A.R. (1997). Divergence of conditioned eyeblink and conditioned fear in backward Pavlovian training. Animal Learning & behavior, 25, 43–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, D.R., and Miles, R.C. (1953). Intralist-interlist relations in verbal learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45, 109–115.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Millenson, J.R., Kehoe, E.J., and Gormezano, I. (1977). Classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response under fixed and mixed CS-US intervals. Learning and Motivation., 8, 351–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, R.R., and Matzel, L.D. (1989). Contingency and relative associative strength. In S.B. Klein and R.R. Mowrer (Eds.), Contemporary learning theories: Pavlovian conditioning and the status of traditional learning theory (pp. 61–84). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minsky, M.L., and Papert, S. (1969). Perceptrons: an introduction to computational geometry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mis, F., and Moore, J.W. (1973). Effect of preacquisition UCS exposure on classical conditioning of the rabbit’s NMR response. Learning and Motivation, 4, 108–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mis, F.W., Andrews, J.G., and Salafia, W.R. (1970). Conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response: ISI by ITI interaction. Psychonomic Science, 20, 57–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, D.S. (1974). Conditional responding and intertrial-interval variability in classical conditioning in the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 87, 73–79.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, J.W. (1972). Stimulus control: studies of auditory generalization in rabbits. In A.H. Black and W.F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning. II: Current research and theory (pp. 206–230). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, J.W., and Stickney, K.J. (1980). Formation of attentional-associative networks in real time: role of the hippocampus and implications for conditioning. Physiological Psychology, 8, 207–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, J.W., Newman, F.L., and Glasgow, B. (1969). Intertriai cues as discriminative stimuli in human eyelid conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 79, 319–326.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, J.W., Choi, J.-S. and Barto, A.G. (1996). TD Model of classical conditioning: response timing and cerebellar implementation. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 22, 1643.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moscovitch, A., and Lolordo, V.M. (1968). role of safety in the Pavlovian backward fear conditioning procedure. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66, 673–678.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Napier, R.M., Macrae, M., and Kehoe, E.J. (1992). Rapid reacquisition in conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Behavior Processes, 18, 182–192.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nordholm, A.F., Lavond, D.G., and Thompson, R.F. (1991). Are eyeblink responses to tone in the decerebrate, decerebellate rabbit conditioned responses? Behavioral Brain Research, 44, 27–34.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M.M. (1970). Classical conditioning of the rabbit’s (Oryctolagus cuniculus) nictitating membrane response with fluctuating ISI and intracranial CS. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 72, 193–202.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I.P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes: an investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex (trans.by G.V. Anrep). London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearce, J.M. (1987). A model for stimulus generalization in Pavlovian conditioning. Psychological Review, 94, 61–73.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pearce, J.M. (1994). Similarity and discrimination: a selective review and a connectionist model. Psychological Review, 101, 587–607.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pearce, J.M., and Hall, G. (1980). A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review, 87, 532–552.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pfautz, P.L., Donegan, N.H., and Wagner, A.R. (1978). Sensory preconditioning versus protection from habituation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 4, 286–295.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Plotkin, H.C., and Oakley, D.A. (1975). Backward conditioning in the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 586–590.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Port, R.L., Beggs, A.L., and Patterson, M.M. (1987). Hippocampal substrate of sensory associations. Physiology and behavior, 39, 643–647.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Prokasy, W.F. (1965). Classical eyelid conditioning: experimenter operations, task demands, and response shaping. In W.F. Prokasy (Ed.), Classical conditioning: a symposium (pp. 208–225). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prokasy, W.F., and Gormezano, I. (1979). The effect of US omission in classical aversive and appetitive conditioning of rabbits. Animal Learning & behavior, 7, 80–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Randich, A., and LoLordo, V.M. (1979). Associative and nonassociative theories of the UCS preexposure phenomenon: implications for Pavlovian conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 523–548.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. (1965). Empirical codifications and specific theoretical implications of compound-stimulus conditioning: perception. In W.F. Prokasy (Ed.), Classical conditioning (pp. 226–248). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiss, S., and Wagner, A.R. (1972). CS habituation produces a “latent inhibition effect” but no active “conditioned inhibition.” Learning and Motivation, 3, 237–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R.A. (1967). Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. Psychological Review, 74, 71–80.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R.A. (1969). Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 72, 77–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R.A. (1970). Reduction in the effectiveness of reinforcement after prior excitatory conditioning. Learning and Motivation, 1, 372–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R.A. (1973). Evidence for “unique stimulus” account of configurai conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 85, 331–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R.A. (1982). Effect of a stimulus intervening between CS and US in autoshaping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 8, 131–141.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R.A. (1997). Summation assessment of a configurai theory. Animal Learning & behavior, 25, 200–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R.A., & Wagner, A.R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A.H. Black and W.F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning. II (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, G.B., Port, R.L., and Stillwell, E. (1993). Latent inhibition of the classically conditioned rabbit nictitating membrane response is unaffected by NMDA antagonist MK801. Psychobiology, 21, 120–124.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R.F., and Steinmetz, J.E. (1998). Contextually based conditional discrimination of the rabbit eyeblink response. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 69, 307–319.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, R.T., Scavio, M.J., Jr., and Erikson, K. (1979). Performance of the nictitating membrane CR following CS-US interval shifts. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 14, 189–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudy, J.W., and Sutherland, R.J. (1995). Configurai association theory and the hippocampal formation: an appraisal and reconfiguration. Hippocampus, 5, 375–389.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D.E., Hinton, G.E., and Williams, R.J. (1986). Learning internal representations by error propagation. In D.E. Rumelhart, J.L. McClelland, and the PDP Research Group (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructures of cognition (pp. 318–362). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saladin, M.E., and Tait, R.W. (1986). US preexposures retard excitatory and facilitate inhibitory conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Animal Learning & behavior, 4, 121–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saladin, M.E., Ten Have, W.N., Saper, Z.L., Labinsky, J.S., and Tait, R.W. (1989). Retardation of rabbit nictitating membrane conditioning following US preexposures depends on the distribution and number of US presentations. Animal Learning & behavior, 17, 179–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salafia, W.R., and Allan, A.M. (1982). Augmentation of latent inhibition by electrical stimulation of hippocampus. Physiology & behavior, 29, 1125–1130.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Salafia, W.R., Mis, F.W., Terry, W.S., Bartosiak, R.S., and Daston, A.P. (1973). Conditioning of the nictitating membrane response of the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) as a function of length and degree of variation of intertriai interval. Animal Learning & Behavior, 1, 109–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salafia, W.R., Daston, A.P., Bartosiak, R.S., Hurley, J., and Martino, L.J. (1974). Classical nictitating membrane conditioning in the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) as a function of unconditioned stimulus locus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 86, 628–636.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Salafia, W.R., Terry, W., and Daston, A.P. (1975). ITI. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 6, 505–508.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salafia, W.R., Martino, L.J., Cloutman, K., and Romano, A.G. (1979). Unconditional-stimulus locus and interstimulus-interval shift in rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) nictitating membrane conditioning. Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, 14, 64–71.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Salafia, W.R., Lambert, R.W., Host, K.C., Chiaia, L.N., and Ramirez, J.J. (1980). Rabbit nictitating membrane conditioning: lower limit of the effective interstimulus interval. Animal Learning & behavior, 8, 85–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scavio, M.J., Jr. (1974). Classical-classical transfer: effects of prior aversive conditioning upon appetitive conditioning in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 86, 107–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scavio, M.J., and Gormezano, I. (1974). generalization. Pavlovian Journal of Biological Sciences, 9, 25–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scavio, M.J., and Gormezano, I. (1980). Classical-classical transfer: effects of prior appetitive conditioning upon aversive conditioning in rabbits. Animal Learning & Behavior, 8, 218–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schafe, G.E., Sollars, S.I., and Bernstein, I.L. (1995). The CS-US interval and taste aversion learning: a brief look. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, 799–802.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schmajuk, N.A., and DiCarlo, J.J. (1992). Stimulus configuration, classical conditioning and hippocampal formation. Psychological Review, 99, 268–305.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schmajuk, N.A., and Holland, P.C. (Eds.) (1998). Occasion setting: associative learning and cognition in animals. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmajuk, N.A., and Moore, J.W. (1988). The hippocampus and the classically conditioned nictitating membrane response: a real-time attentional-associative model. Psychobiology, 16, 20–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmajuk, N.A., Lam, Y.-W., and Gray, J.A. (1996). Latent inhibition: a neural network approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 22, 321–349.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schmajuk, N.A., Lamoureux, J.A., and Holland, P.C. (1998). Occasion setting: a neural network approach. Psychological Review, 105, 3–32.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schneiderman, N. (1966). Interstimulus interval function of the nictitating membrane response underlying trace versus delay conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 62, 397–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneiderman, N. (1972). Response system divergencies in aversive classical conditioning. In A.H. Black & W.F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning: current theory and research (pp. 341–376). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneiderman, N., McCabe, P.M., Haselton, J.R., Ellenberger, H.H., Jarrell, T.W., and Gentile, C.G. (1987). Neurobiological bases of conditioned bradycardia in rabbits. In I. Gormezano, W.F. Prokasy, and R.F. Thompson (Eds.), Classical conditioning (pp. 37–63) Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreurs, B.G. (1989). Classical conditioning of model systems: a behavioral review. Psychobiology, 17, 145–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreurs, B.G. (1993). Long-term memory and extinction of the classically conditioned rabbit NM response. Learning and Motivation, 24, 293–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schreurs, B.G. (1998). Long-term memory and extinction of rabbit nictitating membrane conditioning. Learning and Motivation, 29, 68–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schreurs, B.G., and Alkon, D.L. (1990). US-US conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response: emergence of a conditioned response without alpha conditioning. Psychobiology, 18, 312–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreurs, B.G., and Gormezano, I. (1982). Classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response to CS compounds: effects of prior single stimulus conditioning. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 19, 365–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreurs, B.G., and Kehoe, E.J. (1987). Cross-modal transfer as a function of initial training level in classical conditioning with the rabbit. Animal Learning & behavior, 15, 47–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schreurs, B.G., Kehoe, E.J., and Gormezano, I. (1993). Concurrent associative transfer and competition in serial conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Learning and Motivation, 24, 395–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schreurs, B.G., Oh, M.M., Hirashima, C., and Alkon, D.L. (1995). Conditioning-specific modification of the rabbit’s unconditioned nictitating membrane response. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, 24–33.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sechenov, I.M. (1863). Reflexes of the brain. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheafor, P.J. (1975). “Pseudoconditioned” jaw movements of the rabbit reflect associations conditioned to contextual background cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 104, 245–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, S. (1969a). Effect of CS habituation on eyelid conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 68, 245–248.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, S. (1969b). Generalization of latent inhibition. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 69, 157–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, S., and Domjan, M. (1971). Backward conditioning as an inhibitory procedure. Learning and Motivation, 2, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skelton, R.W., Mauk, M.D., and Thompson, R.F. (1988). Cerebellar nucleus lesions dissociate alpha conditioning from alpha responses in rabbits. Psychobiology, 16, 126–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, M.C. (1968). CS-US interval and US intensity in classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66, 679–687.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, M.C., Coleman, S.R., and Gormezano, I. (1969). forward CS-US intervals. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 69, 226–231.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, P.R. (1977). Role of hippocampus in blocking and conditioned inhibition of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 91, 407–417.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, P.R., and Moore, J.W. (1975). Latent inhibition and stimulus generalization of the classically conditioned nictitating membrane response in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) following dorsal hippocampal ablation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 89, 1192–1203.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, P.R., Brennan, G., and Moore, J.W. (1974). Latent inhibition of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response as a function of CS intensity. Bulletin of Psychonomic Society, 4, 445–448.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutherland, N.S., and Mackintosh, N.J. (1971). Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutherland, R.J., and Rudy, J.W. (1989). amnesia. Psychobiology, 17, 129–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, R.S., and Barto, A.G. (1981). Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: expectation and prediction. Psychological Review, 88, 135–171.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, R.S., and Barto, A.G. (1990). Time-derivative models of Pavlovian reinforcement. In M. Gabriel & J.W. Moore (Eds.), Learning and computational neuroscience (pp. 497–537). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tait, R.W., and Saladin, M.E. (1986). Concurrent development of excitatory and inhibitory associations during backward conditioning. Animal Learning & behavior, 14, 133–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tait, R.W., Kehoe, E.J., and Gormezano, I. (1983). Effects of US duration on classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 91–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tait, R.W., Quesnel, L.J., and ten-Have, W.N. (1986). Classical-classical transfer: excitatory associations between “competing” motivational stimuli during classical conditioning of the rabbit. Animal Learning & behavior, 14, 138–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, E., and Wagner, A.R. (1964). Partial reinforcement of the classically conditioned eyelid response in the rabbit. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 58, 157–158.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, R.F., Donegan, N.H., Clark, G.A., Lavond, D.G., Lincoln, J.S., Madden, J., Mamounas, L.A., Mauk, M.D., and McCormick, D.A. (1987). their interactions in the rabbit. In I. Gormezano, W.F. Prokasy, and R.F. Thompson (Eds.), Classical conditioning (pp. 371–399). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E.C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York: Century.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, A.R., and Brandon, S.E. (1989). Evolution of a structured connectionist model of Pavlovian conditioning (AESOP). In S.B. Klein and R.R. Mowrer (Eds.), Contemporary learning theories: Pavlovian conditioning and the status of traditional learning theory (pp. 149–190). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weidemann, G., and Kehoe, E.J. (1997). Transfer and counterconditioning of conditional control in the rabbit nictitating membrane response. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50B, 295–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weidemann, G., Georgilas, A., and Kehoe, E.J. (1999). Temporal specificity in patterning of the rabbit’s conditioned nictitating membrane response. Animal Learning & Behavior, 27, 99–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weisz, D., and Walts, C. (1990). Reflex facilitation of the rabbit nictitating membrane response by an auditory stimulus as a function of interstimulus interval. Behavioral Neuroscience, 104, 11–20.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weisz, D.J., and LoTurco, J.J. (1988). Reflex facilitation of the nictitating membrane response remains after cerebellar lesions. Behavioral Neuroscience, 102, 203–209.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weisz, D.J., and Mclnerny, J. (1990). An associative process maintains reflex facilitation of the unconditioned nictitating membrane response during the early stages of training. Behavioral Neuroscience, 104, 21–27.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weisz, D.J., Harden, D.G., and Xiang, Z. (1992). Effects of amygdala lesions on reflex facilitation and conditioned response acquisition during nictitating membrane response conditioning in rabbit. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, 262–273.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • White, N. (1998). Effects of reductions in US intensity on CR elimination in the rabbit nictitating membrane preparation. Honours thesis. Sydney: University of New South Wales.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zackheim, J., Myers, C., and Gluck, M. (1998). A temporally sensitive recurrent network model of occasion setting. In N. Schmajuk and P. Holland (Eds.), Occasion setting: associative learning and cognition in animals (pp. 319–342). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2002 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Kehoe, E.J., Macrae, M. (2002). Fundamental Behavioral Methods and Findings in Classical Conditioning. In: Moore, J.W. (eds) A Neuroscientist’s Guide to Classical Conditioning. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-8558-3_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-8558-3_6

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-0-387-98805-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4419-8558-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics