Keywords

1 Introduction

Alcohol use and abuse is continuously causing disease, suffering as well as enormous costs for societies world-wide. If we are to prevent and treat alcohol use disorders, we need to understand the mechanisms underlying the development of addiction. One of the few common denominators among addictive substances is that they increase dopamine in the nucleus accumbens (nAc) (Di Chiara and Imperato 1988). This dopamine elevation has been linked to the rewarding and reinforcing properties (Koob 1992; Wise and Rompre 1989; Spanagel 2009), which is also why this neurotransmitter often is in focus in addiction research. Thus, understanding the mechanisms underlying ethanol’s ability to increase dopamine is of high importance.

Apart from dopamine ethanol is also known to increase extracellular levels of the amino acid taurine (De Witte et al. 1994; Dahchour et al. 1996). In addition, local administration of taurine in the nAc increases dopamine via the same neuronal circuitry as ethanol (Ericson et al. 2006) involving glycine receptors in the nAc as well as nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in the ventral tegmental area (Söderpalm and Ericson 2013). Interestingly, we also found that in order for ethanol to produce an elevation of dopamine in the nAc an increase of extracellular taurine is required (Ericson et al. 2011). This would then implicate taurine as a major participant in the reinforcing properties of ethanol. Further studies are needed to validate or discard this hypothesis.

Several studies have linked chronic drug/alcohol intake to decreased endogenous levels of dopamine in the mesolimbic dopamine system (Diana et al. 1993; Volkow et al. 2007; Feltmann et al. 2016). This adaptation appears also to be a common denominator for addictive drugs but the underlying mechanism to this event is not known. Whether chronic intake of ethanol also produces altered extracellular levels of taurine is not well studied. Thus, in the present study we aimed to measure basal and ethanol-induced extracellular levels of taurine and dopamine in naïve rats and in rats with a history of high voluntary ethanol intake.

2 Methods

2.1 Voluntary Ethanol Intake

A total of 48 male Wistar rats (Taconic, Ejeby, Denmark) weighing 160–180 g at arrival were housed with regular 12 h light cycle and had ad lib access to tap water and standard rodent chow. Following 1 week of acclimatization to the animal facilities the rats were placed in single cages with continuous access to a bottle of water and a bottle of 6% (v/v) ethanol solution. After 3 weeks the rats with the highest ethanol consumption were selected (n = 8) and were administered 12% ethanol solution in the bottle in addition to the water bottle for another 8 weeks. The bottles were weighted twice a week and body weight was measured once a week (Fig. 1). All experiments were approved by the Ethics Committee for Animal Experiments, Gothenburg, Sweden.

Fig. 1
figure 1figure 1

Ethanol intake calculated as g/kg/day (a) and weight gain (b) for the eight selected ethanol high preferring rats placed in a voluntary ethanol consumption paradigm. The first 3 weeks the rats received 6% ethanol solution in addition to the regular water bottle and 12% ethanol solution for the remainder of the experiment

2.2 In Vivo Microdialysis

In vivo microdialysis was carried out in freely moving rats with or without a history of ethanol consumption (naïve). Dialysis probes were surgically implanted in the nAc as previously described (Clarke et al. 2014) using the coordinates (A/P: +1.85, M/L: −1.2 mm to bregma, V/D: −7.8 mm relative to dura; Paxinos and Watson 2007). Following 2 days of recovery the microdialysis experiment was initiated. The dialysis probe was connected to a microperfusion pump via a swivel and the probe was perfused with Ringer’s solution at a rate of 2 μL/min. Dialysate samples were collected every 15 min and analyzed for dopamine and taurine using two separate high-performance liquid chromatography systems as previously described (Lido et al. 2009). After a stable baseline had been obtained the rats received ethanol (300 mM) in the perfusion medium for 45 min or were maintained on Ringer’s solution (control animals). After the experiment the rats were sacrificed and the probe placement was verified.

2.3 Statistic Analysis

Statistical significance was determined by Student’s t-test. Each value was expressed as the mean ± SEM. Differences were considered statistically significant when the calculated P value was less than 0.05.

3 Results

3.1 Ethanol-Induced Elevation of nAc Taurine Is Modest in Ethanol-Experienced Rats

In the present study we wanted to further explore the ethanol-induced elevation of extracellular taurine in the nAc. As we previously demonstrated an ethanol-induced elevation of dopamine needs to be preceded by an increase of extracellular taurine (Ericson et al. 2011) possible adaptations following chronic ethanol intake may be of importance. Here we found that voluntary consumption of rather high amounts of ethanol (6.22 ± 0.59 g/kg/day in average calculated from the last three recordings) did not have a major impact on ethanol’s ability to increase extracellular levels of taurine (Fig. 2a).

Fig. 2
figure 2figure 2

Extracellular levels of taurine in the nAc as measured by in vivo microdialysis in naïve rats or rats with a high ethanol intake (n = 5–7). Ethanol (300 mM) was perfused via the dialysis probe as indicated by the black line in (a). Baseline levels of taurine is shown in (b), and the histogram in (c) highlights extracellular taurine levels at time-point 30. All data are expressed as mean ± SEM, * = p < 0.05 as calculated against the Ringer group; # = p < 0.05 as compared to the ethanol naïve group

Rats with a previous history of ethanol consumption responded with increased extracellular levels of taurine following acute ethanol administration (Ringer vs. ethanol exposed rats, p < 0.001). Only at time-point 30, i.e. 30 min following the initiation of ethanol perfusion, a blunted response was revealed (Fig. 2c; ethanol naïve vs. ethanol exposed, p = 0.006). When comparing baseline levels of extracellular taurine, we found that the two groups of naïve rats (Ringer and ethanol naïve groups) had significantly different extracellular levels of taurine (p = 0.004). However, when pooling the two groups with ethanol naïve rats their basal levels of taurine did not differ from rats with a previous history of voluntary ethanol consumption.

3.2 Ethanol-Induced Elevation of nAc Dopamine Is Lower in Ethanol-Experienced Rats

When analyzing dopamine, we found that the ethanol-induced elevation of dopamine was less prominent in rats chronically exposed to ethanol (Fig. 3a). Although the ethanol high preferring rats did respond with a significant elevation of dopamine following acute exposure (Ringer vs. ethanol exposed, p = 0.042) ethanol was able to produce a larger increase in the ethanol naïve rats (Fig. 3c; ethanol naïve vs. ethanol exposed, p = 0.004). When comparing basal levels of dopamine, no differences were found between the groups (Ringer vs. ethanol naive rats, p = 0.427, Ringer vs. ethanol exposed rats, p = 0.210).

Fig. 3
figure 3figure 3

Extracellular levels of dopamine in the nAc as measured by in vivo microdialysis in naïve rats or rats with a high ethanol intake (n = 6–7). Ethanol (300 mM) was perfused via the dialysis probe as indicated by the black line in (a). Baseline levels of dopamine is shown in (b), and the histogram in (c) highlights extracellular dopamine levels at time-point 30. All data are expressed as mean ± SEM, * = p < 0.05 as calculated against the Ringer group; # = p < 0.05 as compared to the ethanol naïve group

4 Discussion

In the present study using rats with almost 3 months of high voluntary ethanol consumption, we found only very modest alterations of taurine in comparison to ethanol naïve rats. Ethanol has the ability to increase extracellular taurine both in naïve rats and rats with 3 months of high ethanol intake. This is in line with a study from Lallemand and co-workers (2011) where they only found a minor reduction in ethanol’s ability to increase taurine in rats following 3 weeks of forced ethanol intake (gavage). This would then indicate that the ethanol-induced increase of extracellular taurine is a rather robust phenomenon or that a longer period of alcohol exposure is needed to induce adaptations. When dopamine was analyzed in the same samples we found a more pronounced difference in response to acute ethanol administration between naïve rats and rats that had consumed ethanol for 12 weeks. The ethanol-experienced rats displayed a significant but modest dopamine elevation. It is not possible to determine whether the diminished dopamine elevation is due to adaptations following ethanol consumption or if the animals voluntarily choose to consume large amounts of ethanol due to an unresponsive endogenous dopamine system. It has also been suggested that the time point for measuring dopamine following ethanol exposure is delicate. Immediately after withdrawal dopamine levels are low whereas after a few weeks of abstinence the dopamine levels are higher than normal (Hirth et al. 2016). Further studies addressing this question is needed.

Low levels of endogenous striatal dopamine were suggested to both be a cause for, as well as consequence of, excessive ethanol intake (Kashem et al. 2012). When monitoring the basal dopamine levels in the present study we found no differences between naïve animals and those that hade been drinking large amounts of ethanol. Since the naïve rats did not have the opportunity to demonstrate whether they were ethanol preferring rats or not, a spontaneously low dopaminergic tone could have masked any ethanol-induced adaptations in dopamine transmission. However, including more animals and utilizing another method (such as no-net-flux microdialysis) could in future studies shed light on these issues.

Analyzing basal levels of taurine and dopamine in the same samples produced an interesting result. The two groups with naïve rats differed substantially in basal levels of extracellular taurine. This would indicate that the basal extracellular levels of taurine can be very different between individuals, an interesting phenomenon that should be investigated more in depth. In the present study, although statistically significant findings, we are unable to make any conclusions regarding possible adaptations of a taurinergic tone following ethanol consumption. We will continue to investigate differences in extracellular taurine levels in relation to ethanol consumption and ethanol-induced dopamine elevation.

5 Conclusion

In the present study we found that ethanol high preferring rats, consuming ethanol for 12 weeks, displayed a modestly attenuated ethanol-induced elevation of taurine and a diminished increase of extracellular dopamine. Whether this is due to ethanol-induced adaptations or if this is an underlying factor as to why some rats choose to voluntarily consume large amounts of ethanol remains to be established.