Skip to main content
  • 217 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter introduces the concept of motion-event description as a case of implicit learning by adult Anglophone learners of Spanish in a traditional language programme. The concept of motion, despite its frequent appearance in the second-language (L2) Spanish classroom, is not traditionally the focus of explicit presentation. Like other subtly acquired patterns of speech, it is not integrated into the table of contents of language textbooks. As a result, learners assimilate these aspects of the target language largely by hearing or reading (receptive skills) or producing the new language (speaking or writing) over time. This chapter also presents an overview of the entire book; the reader will easily be able to discern the main information and its exact location in the book.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Asher, J. J. (1969). The total physical response approach to second language learning. The Modern Language Journal, 53(1), 3–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asher, J. J. (2000). Learning another language through actions: The complete teacher’s guidebook (6th ed.). Los Gatos, CA: Sky Oaks Productions.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biber, D., Conrad, S., & Rappen, R. (1998). Corpus linguistics: Investigating language structure and use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cadierno, T. (2004). Expressing motion events in a second language: A cognitive typological perspective. In M. Achard & S. Niemeier (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics, second language acquisition, and foreign language teaching (pp. 13–49). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cadierno, T. (2008). Learning to talk about motion in a foreign language. In P. Robinson & N. C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition (pp. 239–275). New York and London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cadierno, T., & Lund, K. (2004). Cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition: Motion events in typological framework. In B. Van Patten, J. Williams, S. Rott, & M. Overstreet (Eds.), Form-meaning connections in second language acquisition (pp. 139–154). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cadierno, T., & Ruiz, L. (2006). Motion events in Spanish L2 acquisition. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 4(1), 183–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chaudron, C. (1985). Comprehension, comprehensibility, and learning in the second language classroom. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 7, 216–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chaudron, C. (1988). Second language classrooms: Research on teaching and learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, V. (1997). The consequences of bilingualism for cognitive processing. In A. M. B. de Groot & J. F. Kroll (Eds.), Tutorials in bilingualism (pp. 279–299). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeKeyser, R. (2003). Implicit and explicit learning. In C. J. Doughty & M. H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 313–348). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doughty, C. (2003). Instructed SLA: Constraints, compensation, and enhancement. In C. Doughty & M. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 256–310). Cornwall, UK: Blackwell Publishing.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Duffelmeyer, F. A. (1980). The influence of experience-based vocabulary instruction on learning word meanings. Journal of Reading, 24(1), 35–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. (1994a). Implicit and explicit language learning—An overview. In N. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 1–31). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. (1994b). Vocabulary acquisition: The implicit ins and outs of explicit cognitive mediation. In N. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 211–282). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. (2002a). Frequency effects in language processing. A review with implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24(2), 143–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. (2002b). Reflections on frequency effects in language processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24(2), 297–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. (2004). The processes of second language acquisition. In B. Van Patten, J. Williams, S. Rott, & M. Overstreet (Eds.), Form-meaning connections in second language acquisition (pp. 49–76). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. (2008). Usage-based and form-focused language acquisition: The associative learning of constructions, learned attention, and the limited L2 endstate. In P. Robinson & N. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition (pp. 372–405). New York, London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R. (1981). The role of input in language acquisition: Some implications for second language teaching. Applied Linguistics, 2(1), 70–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R. (1990). Instructed second language acquisition. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R. (1994a). The study of second language acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R. (1994b). A theory of instructed second language acquisition. In N. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 79–114). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R. (1995). Interpretation tasks for grammar teaching. TESOL Quarterly, 29(1), 87–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foster, P., Bolibaugh, C., & Kotula, A. (2014). Knowledge of nativelike selections in a L2. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 36, 101–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gass, S. M., & Selinker, L. (2008). Second language acquisition: An introductory course. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonglewski, M. (1999). Linking the internet to the national standards for foreign language learning. Foreign Language Annals, 32(3), 348–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hulstijn, J. (2005). Theoretical and empirical issues in the study of implicit and explicit second language learning: Introduction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 129–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kita, S., & Özyürek, A. (2003). What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(1), 16–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krashen, S. (1982). Principles and practices in second language acquisition. London: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krashen, S. (1985). The input hypothesis: Issues and implications. New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krashen, S. (1994). The input hypothesis and its rivals. In N. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 45–77). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krashen, S., & Seliger, H. (1976). The role of formal and informal linguistic environment in adult second language learning. International Journal of Psycholinguistics, 3, 15–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar, Vol. 1: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. (1991). Foundations of cognitive grammar. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lightbown, P., & Spada, N. (2015). How languages are learned. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Littlemore, J. (2009). Applying cognitive linguistics to second language learning and teaching. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, R. (2000). The sixth language: Learning a living in the internet age. Toronto: Stoddart.

    Google Scholar 

  • Long, M. (1983). Does second language instruction make a difference? TESOL Quarterly, 17(3), 359–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Long, M. (1988). Instructed interlanguage development. In L. Beebe (Ed.), Issues in second language acquisition: Multiple perspectives (pp. 115–141). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Long, M. (1996). The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition. In W. C. Ritchie & T. K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 413–468). San Diego, AC: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maxwell, W. (2001). Evaluating the effectiveness of the accelerative integrated method for teaching French as a second language. Master’s thesis, Retrieved from National Library of Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J., & Rumelhart, D. (1985). Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 114, 159–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D., & Duncan, S. (2000). Growth points in thinking for speaking. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 141–161). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., & Johnson-Laird, P. (1976). Verbs of motion. InLanguage and perception (pp. 526–558). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Navarro, S. (2013). Teletandem Canada-Chile: Enhancing communication in Spanish and English beyond the Classroom. In Proceedings of the 7th International Technology, Education and Development Conference (pp. 2256–2262). Valencia, Spain: IATED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Navarro, S. (2015). Thinking outside the classroom for practicing Spanish. In E. Guntly, Z. Lam, A. Anghelescu, & N. Weber (Eds.), Proceedings of 1st Okanagan Workshop on Innovation in Language Teaching (OWILT) (pp. 21–34). University of British Columbia Working Papers in Linguistics 41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Navarro, S., & Contreras, N. (Submitted). Telecollaboration, to what extent is this a valuable addition to a Spanish Language Program? Submitted to The UW Working Papers in Linguistics. NWLC32 Special Issue Conference Proceedings.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norris, J. M., & Ortega, L. (2000). Effectiveness of L2 instruction: A research synthesis and quantitative meta-analysis. Language Learning, 50, 417–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, K., & Chaudron, C. (1987). The effects of linguistic simplification and elaborative modifications on L2 comprehension. University of Hawaii Working Papers in ESL, 6, 107–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlenko, A. (2000). L2 influence on L1 in late bilingualism. Issues in Applied Linguistics, 11, 175–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pawley, A., & Syder, F. H. (1983). Two puzzles for linguistic theory: Nativelike selection and nativelike fluency. In J. C. Richards & R. W. Schmidt (Eds.), Language and communication (pp. 191–225). London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pienemann, M. (1989). Is language teachable? Psycholinguistic experiments and hypotheses. Applied Linguistics, 10(1), 52–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, P. (1995). Attention, memory, and the “noticing” hypothesis. Language Learning, 45, 283–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saltz, E., & Donnenwerth-Nolan, S. (1981). Does motoric imagery facilitate memory for sentences? A selective interference test. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 322–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. (1994a). Deconstructing consciousness in search of useful definitions for applied linguistics. AILA Review, 11, 11–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. (1994b). Implicit learning and the cognitive unconscious: Of artificial grammars and SLA. In N. C. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 165–209). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. (2001). Attention. In R. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 3–32). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Seliger, H. (1983). The language learner as linguist: Of metaphors and realities. Applied Linguistics, 4, 179–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharwood Smith, M. (1994). The unruly world of language. In N. C. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 33–43). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonson, M., Smaldino, S., Albright, M., & Zvacek, S. (2000). Teaching and learning at a distance: Foundations of distance education. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill/Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (1996). Two ways to travel: Verbs of motion in English and Spanish. In M. Shibatani & S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Grammatical constructions: Their form and meaning (pp. 195–220). Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (1997). Mind, code, and text. In J. Bybee, J. Haiman, & S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Essays on language function and language type: Dedicated to T. Givón (pp. 437–467). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (2000). Verbalized events: A dynamic approach to linguistic relativity and determinism. In S. Niemeier & R. Dirven (Eds.), Evidence for linguistic relativity (pp. 107–138). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, H. (1983). Fundamental concepts of language teaching. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (1985). Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In T. Shoepen (Ed.), Language typology and lexical description: Vol. 3. Grammatical categories and the lexicon (pp. 36–149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (1988). The relation of grammar to cognition. In B. Rudzka-Ostyn (Ed.), Topics in cognitive linguistics (pp. 165–205). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (1991). Path to realization: A typology of event conflation. Proceedings of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 17, 480–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (2000). Toward a cognitive semantics: Vol. II: Typology and process in concept structuring. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Telles, J. A., & Vasallos, M. L. (2006). Foreign language learning in-tandem: Teletandem as an alternative proposal in CALLT. The ESPecialist, 27(2), 189–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • VanPatten, B. (1993). Grammar teaching for the acquisition-rich classroom. Foreign Language Annals, 26(4), 435–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vasallos, M. L., & Telles, J. A. (2006). Foreign language learning in-tandem: Theoretical principles and research perspectives. The ESPecialist, 27(1), 83–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warschauer, M. (1995). E-mail for English teachers: Bringing the internet and computer learning networks into the language classroom. Alexandria, VA: Teachers of English to Speakers of Other Languages.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wigdorsky, L. (2005). El enredado ovillo de la lengua. Santiago: Editorial Universidad de Santiago de Chile.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. N. (1999). Memory, attention, and inductive learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21, 1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. N. (2004). Implicit learning of form-meaning connections. In B. VanPatten, J. Williams, S. Rott, & M. Overstreet (Eds.), Form-meaning connections in second language acquisition (pp. 203–218). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wong-Fillmore, L. (1985). When does teacher work as input? In S. M. Gass & C. G. Madden (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp. 17–50). Rowley, MA: Newbury House Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Ortega, S.A.N. (2017). Introduction. In: Comprehending and Speaking about Motion in L2 Spanish. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-49307-7_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-49307-7_1

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Palgrave Macmillan, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-49306-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-49307-7

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics