Keywords

Child maltreatment is a serious public health concern and the consequences of maltreatment are pervasive, spanning multiple domains of functioning. Although the experience of child maltreatment has many known developmental consequences that can manifest across the lifespan (Trickett & McBride-Chang, 1995; Trickett, Negriff, Ji, & Peckins, 2011), not all children who experience maltreatment go on to develop mental health problems. Protective factors such as familial support and self-esteem are thought to impact a child’s resilience in the face of adversity (Masten, 2001) yet few studies are able to longitudinally test for lasting positive or negative effects on development. Furthermore, less is known about how the perception of maltreatment experiences are associated with risk and resilience across development. Thus, the purpose of this study was to test whether the perception of maltreatment experiences as upsetting was associated with the development of internalizing and externalizing behavior problems during adolescence, a developmental period when internalizing symptoms and externalizing behaviors may increase in prevalence and severity (Moffitt, 1993; Petersen et al., 1993).

3.1 Perception of Maltreatment

Perception of an experience is a multifaceted construct as it is influenced by numerous psychological and contextual circumstances, such as an individual’s emotional or psychological state at the time of occurrence or recollection as well as all of the individual’s previous experiences and memories (Wilson & Ross, 2003). As the time since the event in question increases, perception of the event may change based on new experiences (Wilson & Ross, 2003), making the assessment of perception of a childhood experience a difficult task. Few studies have considered maltreated children’s perception of maltreatment experiences as a protective or risk factor for mental health processes, yet other studies of children and adults suggest perception of a traumatic event may be an important predictor of resilience (Boals & Schuettler, 2009). For instance, in a retrospective study of adults recalling their most traumatic experience, the perceived feeling of fear or helplessness during the event rather than characteristics of the event itself predicted the presence of PTSD symptoms (Boals & Schuettler, 2009). Similarly, one bereavement study of children and adolescents suggested the perception of loss is important for predicting outcomes, finding that adolescents’ perceived impact of the loss was positively associated with symptoms of depression (Harrison & Harrington, 2001).

Studies of disclosure which capture a component of perception (i.e., maltreatment is wrong and should be reported) also lend support for the role of perception in predicting mental health problems. The decision to disclose and the mental health effects of disclosure for children and adults vary by characteristics of the child (e.g., age), maltreatment (e.g., type), and disclosure experience (e.g., social reaction) (Goodman-Brown, Edelstein, Goodman, Jones, & Gordon, 2003; Graham-Bermann, Kulkarni, & Kanukollu, 2011; Nagel, Putnam, Noll, & Trickett, 1997; Ullman, 2007). For example, in children exposed to physical threat in the home, disclosure was associated with greater improvement in internalizing problems and reductions in pro-violent cognitions (Graham-Bermann et al., 2011). However, studies of adult women survivors of child sexual abuse found voluntary disclosure to be associated with worse mental health outcomes (Nagel et al., 1997) and women who did not disclose and engaged in repressive coping reported fewer internalizing and externalizing behavior problems (Bonanno, Noll, Putnam, O'Neill, & Trickett, 2003). The findings from previous studies on disclosure suggest aspects of the maltreatment or disclosure experience such as threat (e.g., sexual abuse) and deprivation (e.g., lack of social support) may play a role in the emergence and progression of internalizing and externalizing behavior problems across adolescence (McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Lambert, 2014). Despite evidence from studies of perceived impact and disclosure suggesting perception of maltreatment is important for understanding the development of mental health problems, no studies have directly tested this hypothesis in maltreated youth. The present study aims to address this gap in the literature by testing whether adolescents’ perception of maltreatment as their most upsetting experience is associated with the development of internalizing and externalizing behavior problems across adolescence and whether this association is conditional on maltreatment type.

3.2 Gender Differences

Boys and girls differ in a variety of biological and behavioral domains and in some situations, experience and respond to the environment differently. For example, boys and girls differ in their biological reactivity to different types of challenges (e.g., social stressor vs. cognitive stressor) (Stroud, Salovey, & Epel, 2002) and in response to psychological distress girls are more likely than boys to use emotion-focused coping strategies and seek comfort from a social support network (Brougham, Zail, Mendoza, & Miller, 2009; Eschenbeck, Kohlmann, & Lohaus, 2007). Gender differences in the biopsychosocial response to threat are considered to be adaptive from an evolutionary standpoint (Crick & Zahn–Waxler, 2003; Klein & Corwin, 2002; Taylor et al., 2000). Therefore, although studies suggest boys and girls experience similar rates of maltreatment (Finkelhor, Turner, Shattuck, & Hamby, 2015; Prevoo, Stoltenborgh, Alink, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van Ijzendoorn, 2017), the mental health consequences of maltreatment are expected to differ. To account for gender differences in the response to maltreatment as well as mental health outcomes, the association between perception of maltreatment, internalizing and externalizing behavior problems, and maltreatment type was examined separately for boys and girls .

3.3 Present Study

The following study tested whether perception of maltreatment as the most upsetting experience was associated with internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in maltreated youth separately for boys and girls, and whether maltreatment type moderated these associations. We were not able to directly measure adolescents’ perceptions of maltreatment. Instead, we asked adolescents to describe their most upsetting experience. The nature of these analyses were primarily exploratory due to the lack of research available on perception of maltreatment experiences in youth. The closest evidence that we can draw from to formulate hypotheses is the disclosure literature that suggests disclosure is associated with greater mental health problems in individuals with sexual abuse experiences (Bonanno et al., 2003; Nagel et al., 1997; Ullman, 2007). Therefore, we hypothesized that maltreated girls who perceived maltreatment as being their most upsetting experience would report experiencing greater internalizing problems than girls who did not perceive maltreatment as their most upsetting experience. Similarly, we hypothesized that maltreated boys who perceived maltreatment as being their most upsetting experience would report greater externalizing behavior problems than boys who did not perceive maltreatment as being their most upsetting experience. We also acknowledge that the association between perception of maltreatment as upsetting and mental health may differ by maltreatment type. Therefore, we also hypothesized maltreatment type would moderate these associations such that boys and girls with a history of emotional abuse and/or neglect (without sexual or physical abuse experiences) would report fewer internalizing and externalizing behavior problems when identifying maltreatment as their most upsetting experience compared to those reporting a nonmaltreatment most upsetting experience.

3.4 Methods

3.4.1 Sample Characteristics

Data were from four waves of an ongoing longitudinal study examining the effects of maltreatment on adolescent development. At Time 1 (T1), the sample was composed of 454 adolescents aged 9–13 years (n = 241 boys, n = 213 girls). Time 2 (T2), Time 3 (T3), and Time 4 (T4) occurred on average 1, 2.7, and 7.2 years after baseline. The present study only used data from the maltreated sample of 303 maltreated (n = 151 girls) youth (T1 M age = 10.87 years, SD = 1.13). The maltreated sample was diverse, consisting of youth identifying as Black or African American (n = 123, 41%), Latino (n = 106, 35%), White/non-Latino (n = 35, 12%), or Biracial (n = 39, 13%). Descriptive characteristics of the sample can be found in Table 3.1. Percentages for ethnicity, maltreatment type, and maltreatment type grouping are calculated by gender and most upsetting experience.

Table 3.1 Sample characteristics

3.4.2 Sample Recruitment

Maltreated youth were recruited from active cases in the Children and Family Services (CFS) of a large west coast city. The inclusion criteria were: (1) a new substantiated referral to CFS in the preceding month for any type of maltreatment (e.g., neglect, physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional abuse); (2) child age of 9–12 years; (3) child identified as Latino, African-American, or White (non-Latino); and (4) child residing in one of 10 zip codes in a designated county at the time of referral to CFS. With the approval of CFS and the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the affiliated university, potential participants were contacted via postcard and asked to indicate their willingness to participate. Contact via mail was followed by a phone call. A Certificate of Confidentiality was obtained from the Federal Government to protect each participant’s confidentiality. Disclosures of maltreatment that were not already reported to a CFS agency were not protected under this agreement. Retention strategies (e.g., contact through back-up contact information and social media) were effective as 81%, 64%, and 73% of the maltreated sample was retained at waves 2, 3, and 4, respectively. Information on recruitment and retention for the comparison sample not included in the present study is available in an earlier publication (Negriff, Saxbe, & Trickett, 2015).

3.4.3 Procedure

Assessments were conducted at an urban research university and all procedures were reviewed and approved by the IRB of the affiliated university. Following assent and consent from the adolescent and parent/guardian, the adolescent completed an array of questionnaires and tasks during a four-hour protocol, including hormonal, cognitive, behavioral, and health measures. Only a subset of the measures were used in the following analyses. Child and parent/guardian participants were compensated according to the National Institutes of Health Normal Volunteer Program guidelines.

3.4.4 Measures

Perception of Maltreatment as Most Upsetting Experience

Adolescents completed the Comprehensive Trauma Interview (CTI) (Horowitz, 1999; Noll, Horowitz, Bonanno, Trickett, & Putnam, 2003) at T3 and T4. The CTI includes a screen for 34 potentially traumatic experiences and a follow-up portion to capture relevant information regarding each event (e.g., age at occurrence, identity of perpetrator). During the follow-up, adolescents were asked to describe their most upsetting experience. Responses were coded according to whether or not the experience was a form of child maltreatment as defined by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) (Leeb, Paulozzzi, Melanson, Simon, & Arias, 2008). Examples of nonmaltreatment most upsetting experiences included the death of a relative or friend, witnessing or knowing the victim of neighborhood violence, and having a sick relative or friend. Coded responses were used to categorize adolescents into one of two groups: adolescents who perceived maltreatment to be their most upsetting experience (34% boys, 57% girls) and adolescents who did not perceive maltreatment to be their most upsetting experience (66% boys, 43% girls).

Internalizing Problems

Symptoms of depression were assessed at all four waves of measurement by self-report using the Child Depression Inventory (CDI) (Kovacs, 1981, 1992). Adolescents rated symptoms of depression (k = 27) on a scale of 0–2 with higher scores indicating more severe symptoms (α = .86–.89). Responses were summed to create a composite score of depressive symptoms. Symptoms of anxiety were assessed at all four waves of measurement by self-report with the Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC) (March, Parker, Sullivan, Stallings, & Conners, 1997). The MASC consists of 30 items on a scale from 0 to 3 with higher scores representing greater symptom severity. Only MASC items administered at all four waves (k = 21) were summed to create a composite score of anxiety symptoms (α = .87–.90). MASC items not administered at T4 (k = 9) due to the age appropriateness of the questions were not included in the anxiety sum scores at T1-T3 .

Externalizing Behavior Problems

Subscales from the Youth Self Report (YSR) (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) assessed aggressive behavior (k = 14 items) and rule breaking behavior (k = 10 items) at all four waves of measurement by self-report. For each item, adolescents reported whether the statement was “never true,” “sometimes true,” or “always true.” Items that were not administered at T3 and T4 for aggressive behavior (k = 3 items) and rule breaking behavior (k = 1 item) due to the age inappropriateness of the questions, and rule breaking behavior items that improved Cronbach’s α when deleted (k = 1 item) were not included in the aggressive behavior and rule breaking behavior sum scores (aggressive behavior α = .80–.84; rule breaking behavior α = .75–.79).

Maltreatment Type

Records from the CFS agency were carefully reviewed and documented at T1 for each child’s history and type of maltreatment using the Maltreatment Case Record Abstraction Instrument (MCRAI) (Barnett, Manly, & Cicchetti, 1993; Mennen, Kim, Sang, & Trickett, 2010). In this sample, 232 youth (74% girls, 80% boys) experienced neglect, 156 youth (55% girls, 48% boys) experienced emotional abuse, 156 youth (44% girls, 59% boys) experienced physical abuse, and 60 youth (27% girls, 13% boys) experienced sexual abuse. In order to more meaningfully test for effects of maltreatment type, maltreated youth were classified into two mutually exclusive groups according to their sexual and physical abuse history. One group consisted of sexually and/or physically abused adolescents who may have also experienced emotional abuse and/or neglect (61% girls, 67% boys). Sexually and physically abused adolescents were grouped together because victims of sexual and/or physical abuse are more likely to have experienced poly-victimization (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2012) and psychological trauma (Leeb et al., 2008) compared to victims of emotional abuse and neglect. A second group consisted of adolescents who experienced emotional abuse and/or neglect without a history of sexual or physical abuse (39% girls, 33% boys).

Covariates

The time-varying covariate age was included as a covariate to account for the increase in prevalence and severity of internalizing and externalizing behavior problems that occurs during adolescence (Petersen et al., 1993). Age was also considered to be a proxy for pubertal development as the two are highly correlated and the majority of adolescents age out of pubertal staging (i.e., Tanner Stage 5) by age 15 (Slyper, 2006). The time-invariant covariate ethnicity was reported by the adolescent’s parent or guardian at T1 as being Black/African-American (35%, 52%), Latino (42%, 25%), White (non-Latino) (8%, 11%), or Biracial (15%, 12%) for adolescents reporting maltreatment and nonmaltreatment as their most upsetting experience, respectively. Ethnicity was included as a covariate to account for ethnic differences in risk for internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in adolescents (López et al., 2017). In the US, the largest percentage of victimized youth identified as being White/non-Latino (44%) (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2012); therefore, in subsequent analyses adolescents’ ethnicity was regrouped as either being White (non-Latino) or an ethnic minority (Black/African American, Latino, or Biracial) .

3.4.5 Analytic Plan

Descriptive analyses were performed for all continuous variables by gender. Hypotheses were tested using linear and quadratic growth models in a multilevel model framework using SAS ® (Version 9.4) Proc Mixed. Missing data were accounted for with restricted maximum likelihood (REML) estimation. Growth models were fit separately for depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, rule breaking behaviors, and aggressive behaviors, stratified by gender. All model predictors were grand mean centered so that the intercept represents the average mental health outcome for the prototypical adolescent at T1. Models were built by fitting an unconditional (i.e., no growth) model, a linear growth model with intercept at T1, a quadratic growth model with intercept at T1, a linear or quadratic growth model with most upsetting experience as a time-invariant covariate, and then testing the moderating effect of maltreatment type on the association between the mental health outcome and most upsetting experience. For each of the outcomes, the best fitting model for depressive symptoms, aggressive behavior, and rule breaking behavior in boys and girls, and anxiety symptoms in boys included the linear term and covariates. The best fitting model for symptoms of anxiety in girls included the quadratic term and covariates. The results did not differ when covariates were removed from models.

3.5 Results

3.5.1 Descriptive Analyses

Descriptive statistics of study measures are presented in Table 3.2. Results from independent t-tests show mean differences in internalizing and externalizing behavior problems between boys and girls. On average, compared to boys, girls: reported more symptoms of depression at T4 (t222 = −2.59, p < .05); reported more symptoms of anxiety at T1 (t299 = −2.03, p < .05), T3(t191 = −2.88, p < .01), and T4 (t219 = −3.98, p < .01); reported fewer aggressive behaviors at T1 (t299 = 2.59, p < .05); and reported fewer rule breaking behaviors at T1 (t299 = 3.04, p < .01).

Table 3.2 Descriptive statistics for boys and girls

In girls, those reporting maltreatment (vs. nonmaltreatment) as their most upsetting experience were older at T3 (t102 = −2.30, p < .05). In boys, those reporting maltreatment (vs. nonmaltreatment) as their most upsetting experience: were younger at T4 (t101 = 2.19, p < .05); reported fewer aggressive behaviors at T1 (t108.36 = 2.07, p < .05) and T2 (t100 = 2.19, p < .05); and reported fewer rule breaking behaviors at T1 (t114.63 = 3.22, p < .01).

3.5.2 Hypothesis Tests

Intraclass Correlation Coefficients (ICC)

Unconditional, no-growth models (i.e., intercept only) were fit for both boys and girls for symptoms of depression, symptoms of anxiety, aggressive behavior, and rule breaking behavior. The ICC for boys and girls was .48 and .43 (symptoms of depression), .29 and .23 (symptoms of anxiety), .35 and .42 (aggressive behavior), and .30 and .24 (rule breaking behavior), respectively.

Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Girls

Model estimates for girls are presented in Table 3.3. In girls, the trajectory of depressive symptoms differed according to most upsetting experience; girls reporting maltreatment as their most upsetting experience increased in symptoms of depression across adolescence whereas girls reporting nonmaltreatment as their most upsetting experience decreased in symptoms of depression across adolescence (Fig. 3.1). Most upsetting experience was also associated with change in anxiety symptoms over time. Girls reporting a nonmaltreatment most upsetting experience decreased in symptoms of anxiety at a steeper rate from T1 to T2, and increased in anxiety symptoms at a steeper rate from T3 to T4 compared to girls reporting a maltreatment most upsetting experience (Fig. 3.2). Maltreatment type did not moderate the association between most upsetting experience and depressive or anxiety symptoms, or in the change in depressive or anxiety symptoms over time.

Table 3.3 Growth models for internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in girls
Fig. 3.1
figure 1

Depression symptom trajectories differ according to most upsetting experience in girls

Fig. 3.2
figure 2

Anxiety symptom trajectories differ according to most upsetting experience in girls

In girls, most upsetting experience was not associated with the trajectory of aggressive behavior. However, most upsetting experience was associated with rule breaking behavior at T1 of the study and this association was conditional on maltreatment type. Of girls who experienced emotional abuse and/or neglect (without sexual and/or physical abuse experiences), those reporting maltreatment as their most upsetting experience reported lower levels of rule breaking behavior than girls with a nonmaltreatment most upsetting experience (Fig. 3.3).

Fig. 3.3
figure 3

Rule breaking behavior at Time 1 differs according to most upsetting experience and maltreatment type in girls

Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Boys

Model estimates for boys are presented in Table 3.4. In boys , most upsetting experience was not associated with the trajectories of depressive or anxiety symptoms, nor was there a moderating effect of maltreatment type on any of the associations. However, in boys, those reporting maltreatment as their most upsetting experience had lower levels of aggressive behavior and rule breaking behavior at T1 than boys reporting nonmaltreatment as their most upsetting experience.

Table 3.4 Growth models for internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in boys

3.6 Discussion

Our study found that girls’ and boys’ reports of most upsetting experience were associated with internalizing and externalizing behavior problems during adolescence. Regardless of maltreatment type, reporting maltreatment as their most upsetting experience was associated with increased depressive symptoms and less steep change in anxiety symptoms over time in girls, and lower levels of aggressive and rule breaking behavior in boys. Further, in girls with a history of emotional abuse and/or neglect, rule breaking behavior was lower at study onset in girls reporting a maltreatment most upsetting experience. Our results for depression and rule breaking behavior in girls are consistent with the disclosure literature (Bonanno et al., 2003; Graham-Bermann et al., 2011; Nagel et al., 1997). For boys, the results are consistent with disclosure of non-sexual forms of maltreatment (Graham-Bermann et al., 2011) however the association was present for both emotionally abused/neglected and physically/sexually abused boys. Our finding of gender differences may be attributed to differences in coping responses to adversity between boys and girls (for example, see Eschenbeck et al., 2007) and the effectiveness of those coping strategies (Littleton, Horsley, John, & Nelson, 2007; Schnider, Elhai, & Gray, 2007), contributing to differences in what type of experience boys and girls find most upsetting and how this perception relates to mental health. However, we are not able to test these important questions with data from the current study but these are avenues of research future studies can explore.

3.6.1 Clinical Implications

Findings from the present study offer a broader understanding of how perceptions of maltreatment as an adolescent’s most upsetting experience relate to mental health outcomes across adolescence for clinicians, social workers, and other advocates for the health and wellbeing of children. Our findings suggest clinicians can gauge maltreated children’s risk for depression, anxiety, aggression, and rule breaking behavior by asking children to reflect on their most upsetting experience. Treatment plans can then be tailored to address each child’s individual risk. Tools for discussing most upsetting experiences have already been developed and shown to be feasible in treatment and intervention settings. For example, clinicians administering PTSD assessments prompt children to describe their most significant trauma and clinicians administering Trauma-Focused Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (Cohen, Mannarino, & Deblinger, 2016) ask children to create their trauma narrative based on their most upsetting or scary experience. However, care should be taken when asking a child to describe their most upsetting experience as this line of questioning could cause emotional distress to the child or clinician, require a report to be made to CFS agencies, or influence an ongoing investigation (Kuehnle & Connell, 2010).

Findings from the present study also highlight the importance of developing interventions and treatment plans that account for gender differences in risk for mental health problems. While maltreatment as a most upsetting experience has not been widely studied across genders, our findings are consistent with other studies showing gender differences in risk factors for psychopathology. For example, previous research has found gender differences in the psychological response to adversity (Eschenbeck et al., 2007) and in the developmental pathway that links child maltreatment to risk for internalizing and externalizing behavior problems (Maschi, Morgen, Bradley, & Hatcher, 2008). Thus, our findings suggest that both a child’s gender and most upsetting experience are critically important for clinicians and child advocates when determining risk for and treatment plans to prevent mental health problems following maltreatment experiences.

3.6.2 Program Evaluation Implications

Our findings have important implications for evaluating interventions and treatments that target depressive disorders in adolescent girls with a history of child maltreatment. Specifically, we suggest that evaluators test short- and long-term intervention and treatment effectiveness for depressive symptoms in girls according to their most upsetting experience. Given that girls reporting a maltreatment most upsetting experience reported increased depressive symptoms over time, the effectiveness of an intervention may not be apparent in girls until years following the maltreatment experience. While most upsetting experience was not associated with change in aggressive or rule breaking behavior over time, most upsetting experience may be a useful metric for evaluating baseline differences in boys and girls following assignment to an intervention, standard care, or no treatment group.

3.6.3 Policy Implications

Our study demonstrated that adolescents’ most upsetting experience was associated with risk for mental health problems. While many aspects of the maltreatment experience (e.g., relationship to the perpetrator) likely contribute to the traumatic nature of maltreatment and whether or not it is perceived as most upsetting, the aftermath of reporting maltreatment can also be traumatizing for youth. For example, the child’s experience with CFS agencies and the judicial process and familial support following a report have been shown to increase the traumatizing effects of child maltreatment (Goodman et al., 1992). Thus, while legislative efforts to prevent child maltreatment should continue to be supported, it is also crucial to support legislation that protects children during and after the initial report of child maltreatment.

One way legislators can reduce the traumatic aftereffects of maltreatment is through increased financial support of children’s advocacy centers (CAC). CAC’s are community-based centers that allow children to report child maltreatment to representatives from the criminal justice system, health care system, and other professionals involved in investigating maltreatment allegations and providing treatment (Boeskin, Edwards, Laird, & Lounsbury, 2016). The implementation of CAC’s and enacting laws to protect child witnesses has likely improved mental health outcomes for maltreated children (Smith, Witte, & Fricker-Elhai, 2006) by reducing the psychological harm associated with reporting maltreatment. Currently, the majority of funding for CAC’s comes from the government, primarily through the Victims of Child Abuse Act (VOCAA). However, public and private funds gained through VOCAA have not been able to keep up with the rapid development and utilization of CAC’s across the US (Boeskin et al., 2016). Given CAC’s reduce the traumatic nature of maltreatment which can contribute to children’s perception of maltreatment, it is imperative that VOCAA and other laws that provide support for CAC’s (e.g., Victims of Crime Act) continue to be reauthorized with annual increases in funding (for review, see Boeskin et al., 2016).

3.6.4 Limitations

The findings from the present study contribute to our understanding of how perception of maltreatment as the most upsetting experience is associated with internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in boys and girls, and how our findings can be used to inform clinical risk, program evaluation, and support for policies that prioritize children and their families. However, the findings and their implications should be considered within the context of the study’s limitations. First, perception of maltreatment was indirectly assessed by asking adolescents to describe their most upsetting experience and not by directly asking adolescents about their feelings toward maltreatment. Therefore, we were unable to ascertain whether adolescents reporting maltreatment as their most upsetting experience was a proxy for psychological adjustment to maltreatment (e.g., the ability to describe a maltreatment experience to a stranger) or willingness to voluntarily disclose maltreatment. We were also unable to establish whether adolescents reporting nonmaltreatment as their most upsetting experience was reflective of something comparatively more traumatic happening (e.g., death of a loved one) rather than poor psychological adjustment, or the refusal to voluntarily disclose maltreatment. Although we were unable to directly test these questions, the latter scenario (i.e., something more traumatic happening) is less methodologically problematic as it would have a suppressive effect on our findings.

Second, perception of maltreatment was assessed retrospectively under the assumption that adolescents’ most upsetting experience at T3 or T4 would be indicative of adolescents’ psychological adjustment or willingness to disclose to the interviewer at earlier time points, regardless of traumatic or disturbing experiences occurring from T1-T3. Perception of maltreatment was not assessed at earlier time points due to ethical and methodological concerns. The CTI assesses a wide variety of traumatic experiences (e.g., rape) and was not administered at T1 and T2 to prevent young adolescents from experiencing emotional distress. Although perception of maltreatment was assessed retrospectively, the elapsed time between the maltreatment and T3 is still less than the elapsed time between childhood and adulthood, when most studies of perception and disclosure take place.

Despite these limitations, this study has many strengths. The present study is valuable in that data were collected longitudinally from a high-risk population of maltreated youth, with high retention rates at each wave of measurement. Similarly, few studies are able to test developmental hypotheses about maltreated youth while considering differences between boys and girls. As such, the findings from the present study contribute to the literature on how perception of maltreatment as upsetting is associated with the development of mental health problems for adolescent boys and girls.

3.6.5 Conclusions and Future Directions

In summary, the findings from the present study have clinical, program evaluation, and policy implications. In the clinical setting, most upsetting experience can be used by clinicians and social workers to assess risk for internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in boys and girls, which are consistently associated with poor developmental outcomes across the lifespan (Fergusson & Woodward, 2002; Lewinsohn, Rohde, Seeley, Klein, & Gotlib, 2003; Whiteford et al., 2013). Most upsetting experience can be used as a metric to test for baseline differences in intervention groups and short- and long-term intervention effectiveness. Further, both child maltreatment and the events that follow can contribute to the traumatic nature of maltreatment (Goodman et al., 1992). Legislators can minimize the trauma associated with the aftermath of reporting maltreatment by supporting programs that provide resources to maltreated children and their families, such as CAC’s (Smith et al., 2006).

Future studies on perception of maltreatment and the development of internalizing and externalizing behavior problems should consider ethically and methodologically sound ways to measure perception of maltreatment during childhood and early adolescence when the maltreatment experience is more proximal. For example, researchers can consult with caseworkers, psychologists, and other licensed professionals on how to ask children about their thoughts and feelings toward their maltreatment experiences in a valid way without causing psychological distress. Future studies should also consider the role programs and services resulting from legislation and intervention by CFS agencies play in shaping children’s perceptions of maltreatment, and their association with internalizing and externalizing behavior problems from childhood into adulthood.