Introduction

Dave and Judy just moved to a new apartment. A delivery guy haphazardly places the parts of their new sectional sofa across the living room. Both Dave and Judy think about how to best arrange the sectional. Although they independently decide on the same location and configuration, Dave makes his decision more quickly. Talking about how they decided where it should go, they realize they used different approaches. While Dave re-arranged the sectional by thinking about it as a whole, Judy mentally manipulated the pieces one by one, potentially explaining why she contemplated the solution longer. Why did they adopt different strategies? Is it because Dave is a man and Judy is a woman? Or do other factors such as the walls of the room, or the way they are accustomed to think about space contribute to their cognitive strategy differences? The current research examined factors affecting cognitive strategies used for mental rotation.

We constantly process spatial information, e.g., moving without bumping into the furniture, knowing the direction between home and work, or fitting suitcases into a car trunk. Although we all engage in spatial processing, some people do so better than others. Individual differences in spatial ability have long captured researchers’ attention, particularly sex differences (Collins & Kimura, 1997; Kail, Carter, & Pellegrino, 1979; Lippa, Collaer, & Peters, 2010; Miller & Halpern, 2014; Reilly, Neumann, & Andrews, 2016), yet conclusions have not been consistent and questions remain (Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995; Uecker & Obrzut, 1993). However, sex is not the only factor that has shown a relationship to mental rotation. Spatial cognitive style (Pazzaglia & DeBeni, 2001, 2006), stimuli characteristics, such as complexity (e.g., Bethell-Fox & Shepard, 1988; Doyle & Voyer, 2013; Doyle, Voyer, & Lesmana, 2015; Folk & Luce, 1987), dimensionality (e.g., Bauer & Jolicoeur, 1996; Hampson, Levy-Cooperman, & Korman, 2014; Jansen-Osmann & Heil, 2007), or occlusion (Alexander & Evardone, 2008; Doyle & Voyer, 2013; Voyer & Doyle, 2010; Voyer & Hou, 2006), and instructions (e.g., Sharps, Price, & Williams, 1994) can also affect mental rotation. The present work explores the sole and combined effects of stimuli characteristics, such as framing and figure consistency, and individual differences, such as habitual spatial thinking, on mental rotation performance. We begin by explaining the Mental Rotation task and how possible cognitive strategies might affect its performance. We go on to discuss factors, such as stimulus characteristics and habitual spatial thinking that could affect rotation performance.

Mental rotation task

In classic mental rotation experiments (e.g., Shepard & Metzler, 1971), participants decide whether two figures are the same or mirror images of one another. The time to do so shows a linear relationship between the two figures’ initial angular disparity and both response time (e.g., Shepard & Metzler, 1971) and accuracy (e.g., Butler et al., 2006), an effect known as the Angular Disparity Effect (ADE). The ADE has been shown with psychometric multiple-choice tests (Alexander & Evardone, 2008; Doyle & Voyer, 2013; Vandenberg & Kuse, 1978; Voyer & Hou, 2006) and computerized Mental Rotation tests (Bryden, George, & Inch, 1990; Doyle, Voyer, & Lesmana, 2015). Based on this finding, researchers hypothesize that participants mentally rotate one figure to align with the other. Note, however, that recent work from Tufts’ Spatial Cognition Lab (Gardony, Taylor, & Brunye, 2014) suggests that rotation may not be to alignment, but to an angular disparity affording successful comparison. In actuality, this mental rotation process involves several steps (Cooper & Shepard, 1973; Halpern & Collaer, 2005; Just & Carpenter, 1976). A person first encodes both stimuli then mentally rotates one to align with the other. Once aligned, the person compares the figures, deciding whether they are the same and then responds. Regression data showing the relationship between angular disparity and response accounts for the different mental rotation steps (Kail et al., 1979; Lohman, 1986); the slope reflects mental rotation rate, and the intercept includes encoding, comparison, and response processes. Eye movement data also suggest these steps (Freedman & Rovegno, 1981; Just & Carpenter, 1976).

Sex differences in mental rotation have received significant research attention. Not uncommonly, men mentally rotate faster than women (Linn & Petersen, 1985; Lippa, Collaer, & Peters, 2010; Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974; Masters & Sanders, 1993; Peters, 2005; Resnick, 1993; Richardson, 1994; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995) although accuracy is often comparable (Goldstein, Haldane, & Mitchell, 1990; Kail et al., 1979; Voyer & Bryden, 1990). In reality this male advantage may be more ethereal. Some studies have found no sex difference (Butler et al., 2006; Hugdahl, Thomsen, & Ersland, 2006; Jansen-Osmann & Heil, 2007; Jordan, Schadow, Wuestenberg, Heinz, Peters, & Jancke, 2002; Peters, 2005; Voyer & Bryden, 1990), especially when instructions emphasized general rather than spatial thinking (Scali, Brownlow, & Hicks, 2000) or when the stimuli were haptically or visually experienced replicas of 3-D figures rather than drawings (Robert & Chevrier, 2003). In other studies women were faster than men (Moè, 2009; Thomsen, Hugdahl, Ersland, Barndon, Lundervold, & Smievoll, 2000; Uecker & Obrzut, 1993). This variability in mental rotation sex differences suggests that other factors may contribute to strategy use, including aspects of the Mental Rotation task (e.g., stimuli characteristics) and/or other individual differences (e.g., habitual spatial thinking).

Possible factors leading to differential mental rotation performance

Strategies in mental rotation

Different data patterns have been used to explain sex-linked cognitive strategies for mental rotation: response times slowed down as a function of angular disparity suggesting complexity of the stimuli affected rotation, and further made the difference between sexes more observable (Gluck & Fitting, 2003; Halpern, Benbow, Geary, Gur, Hyde, & Gernsbacher, 2007; Kail et al., 1979). Women also displayed a positively skewed response distribution, compared to men’s normal distribution (Kail et al., 1979; Jordan et al., 2002; Lohman, 1986). Both the distribution and the slope difference suggest strategy differences.

Two mental rotation strategies have been proposed in the literature: holistic vs. piecemeal (Kail et al., 1979; Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974; Strasser, Koller, Strauß, Csisinko, Kaufmann, & Glück, 2010) or imagistic versus analytic (Heil & Jansen-Osmann, 2008; Lawton, 2010; Pezaris & Casey, 1991). We will adhere to the use of holistic and piecemeal strategies henceforth since the terms fit the conditions used in the current study. With holistic processing, the figure is rotated as a whole; with piecemeal, it is broken down into parts that are rotated separately (Geiser, Lehmann, & Eid, 2006; Robertson & Palmer, 1983; Yuille & Steiger, 1982). Specifically, holistic strategies involve analog spatial processing and piecemeal strategies rely on feature-by-feature non-spatial comparison. Hemispheric laterality observed in mental rotation further supports these two strategies (e.g., Hugdahl et al., 2006).

Stimuli characteristics

Naylor, Taylor, and Cross (1998) created stimuli to test holistic versus piecemeal strategies. Participants viewed 3-D cube figures that were either whole objects, akin to Shepard and Metzler’s (1971) original figures, or the same figures cut in half to facilitate a piecemeal strategy. They found no sex difference, overall, but an interaction between sex and whole/cut objects that suggested strategy differences. As predicted, women responded similarly to cut and whole objects. Men’s data for cut objects matched that of women, but men differed in their whole object rotation. This pattern suggested that women consistently took a piecemeal approach whereas men sought an efficient strategy for the stimuli at hand (Naylor, Taylor & Cross, 1998). So, if mental rotation performance differences relate to strategy choices, stimuli promoting either piecemeal or holistic processing should interact with the strategy used.

Frame of reference is vital in spatial information processing (e.g., Levinson, 1996) and research showed that surrounding geometry affects the spatial representation (Kelly, McNamara, Bodenheimer, Carr, & Rieser, 2008; Kelly & McNamara, 2008; Mou, Zhang, & McNamara, 2009; Mou, Zhao, & McNamara, 2007; Wang & Spelke, 2000). Gestalt perception, and perceptual grouping (Rock, 1990), specifically, suggests that a frame would emphasize an object’s unitary structure, thus predicting more effective rotation with a frame. In contrast, the Multiple Systems Framework (Zacks & Michelon, 2005) suggests that mental rotation engages object-based transformations. An object’s reference frame moves relative to the observer. This theory predicts more effective rotation without a frame, because the frame imposes an environment-based reference on an object-based transformation. Thus, a frame should impede performance.

How a frame affects mental rotation may also interact with stimulus consistency (whole vs. cut). With perceptual grouping theory (Rock, 1990), frame might force unity more so for whole figures and holistic processing, but might impede cut figure rotation. With the Multiple Systems Framework a piecemeal strategy, promoted by cut stimuli, might engage environment-based references to track piece-by-piece rotation (Zacks & Michelon, 2005). This would suggest that a frame should hinder whole object rotation and would either not impact or might even help cut figure rotation.

Habits and preferences in spatial processing

Just as with mental rotation, people adopt habitual preferences and strategies for processing spatial information at a greater scale. For example, when learning a new environment, one person may seek out a map while another may prefer to explore or get a verbal description. When thinking about a known environment, one person may view it as if from above (survey representation), someone else may imagine taking a route through the environment (route representation), and a third person may think about the various landmarks in the environment (landmark representation). The root of these habits and preferences has not been systematically explored, but they seem to affect other spatial processing.

Pazzaglia and De Beni (2001, 2006) developed and used a Questionnaire on Spatial Representation, which includes questions designed to understand whether an individual habitually uses a survey, route or landmark-centered approach when thinking about known environments or learning new places. They then related one’s habitual approach to mental rotation. Overall, individuals, both men and women, who had survey-based representational preferences scored better on mental rotation compared to individuals who habitually took a landmark or route-centered approach (Pazzaglia & De Beni, 2001). Thus, one’s habitual spatial processing; i.e., the way they habitually represent spatial information, appears to impact spatial processing more generally. One’s preference for how he/she learns a new environment (maps, exploration, verbal description) may have a similar effect.

Current research

The current research, in three experiments, investigated various influences on mental rotation performance and strategies. Overall, we expected to replicate the standard ADE in mental rotation research (e.g., Shepard & Metzler, 1971). Experiment 1 employed a standard 3-D rotation task, but with modified stimuli. The first modification (whole vs. cut objects) encouraged a holistic or piecemeal approach. The second modification (unframed vs. framed) allowed for standard reference frame use or introduced a salient alternative reference frame. In Experiment 2, we further investigated the rotation performance on reference frame use, introducing a circular frame in addition to no frame and the rectangular frame. A circular frame provides a salient basis for comparison, but without the axes that define a reference frame. Experiment 3 explored how the same stimulus modifications impacted the rotation of a 3-D figure within a 3-D frame, again using whole and cut figures. Overall, rotation performance for strategy-consistent stimuli should reflect the pattern indicative of that strategy. All experiments also explored sex differences and habitual spatial processing differences in the form of spatial representation (Pazzaglia & De Beni, 2001) and preference for learning spatial information.

Experiment 1

In mental rotation tasks, stimuli generally consist of whole objects. People imagine these rotating as a solid unit (holistic strategy) or cognitively sub-divide them (piecemeal strategy). These different strategies are evident in response time (RT) patterns, such as overall faster performance with a holistic strategy, and greater increase in RT as a function of the angular difference with a piecemeal strategy. Experiment 1 explored how stimulus characteristics that create affordances for either a holistic or piecemeal strategy affect rotation performance.

We modified the standard Shepard and Metzler (1971) type figures to encourage a piecemeal strategy (cut objects) and test strategy use with frames surrounding each object. While stimulus differences, such as those manipulated in Experiment 1, might encourage a particular mental rotation strategy, people have habitual cognitive approaches to spatial tasks. To examine how these habitual spatial approaches affect rotation, we included individual difference measures that illuminate them.

Method

Participants

Ninety-two Tufts undergraduates, 40 women and 52 men, participated for partial course credit.

Materials

Mental rotation stimuli

Stimuli consisted of object pairs defined by three factors; Angular Disparity (00, 300, 600, 900, 1200, 1500, and 1800), Figure Consistency (whole, cut), and Framing (framed, unframed). Stimuli consisted of three 3-D Shepard and Metzler (1971) type figures created with CAD software. Each figure included ten or eleven cubes with 90-degree angular projections into 3-D space. These 3-D block figures comprised the whole figures. We created cut figures by dividing the whole figures into two parts (one half with five cubes and the other with five or six) and separating the halves by 0.75 inches (Naylor et al., 1998). In total, there were 63 whole and 63 cut figures, totaling to 126 trials per experiment. We also created a mirror-version of each figure, totaling to 63 same and 63 mirror images. For all figures, we created framed stimuli by adding a rectangular frame, approximately 6 × 4 in., surrounding each whole figure and both halves of each cut figure. In total, there were 126 framed and 126 unframed figures. This way, participants were tested on each angular disparity 14 times; seven in whole, seven in cut blocks; and seven in unframed and seven in framed conditions.

Stimulus pairs consisted of two whole or two cut figures (Fig. 1) that were either the same or different (mirror image) from one another. A vertical line separated the two figures in a pair. Pairs of objects (whole and cut) differed in angular rotation by 30-degree-increments, yielding seven rotations: 00, 300, 60, 900, 1200, 1500, and 1800, and included rotations along X-, Y-, and Z-axes.

Fig. 1
figure 1

(a) (top left) A pair of unframed whole figures, while (b) (top right) shows a pair of framed cut figures. (c) (above left) An unframed cut pair and (d) (above right) shows framed cut stimuli

Questionnaires

Questionnaires gathered information on factors implicated in differential habitual spatial thinking. The Individual Differences questionnaire asks participants about gender, familial sinistrality, self-rated sense of direction, and preference for spatial information format (map or verbal directions). The Mental Representation questionnaire (Pazzaglia & De Beni, 2001) assesses spatial processing habits, including sense of direction and cognitive approaches to spatial processing. There are 17 questions in total and an examplary question would be “When you are in your home-town or city, do you naturally know cardinal points? That is, do you know where North, South, East, and West are?” Most of the questions ask participants to rate their answers on a Likert-type scale, from 1 to 5 where 1 indicates “not at all” and 5 indicates “very much.” Through coding the questions in a certain way (Pazzaglia & De Beni, 2001, 2006), a person can be identified as habitually using a survey-centered versus landmark-centered representation.

Design

In Experiment 1 characteristics of stimuli were manipulated: (1) Participants (46 participants) either received framed stimuli or they (46 participants) received unframed stimuli). (2) Within the framing condition, participants either received whole figures or their cut versions.

The questionnaires that participants completed allowed us to categorize them by: (1) sex, (2) preference when learning new environments (maps or verbal directions), and (3) mental representations tendency (survey or landmark). Thus, the present study used a 2 (framed vs. unframed, between participant) × 2 (whole vs. cut, within participant) × 2 (individual factors: sex, preference, or mental representation, between participant) mixed design. The dependent variable was performance (rotation rate and accuracy) on the Mental Rotation test.

Procedure

The experiment took place in Tufts University’s Spatial Cognition Lab. After consenting to take part, participants sat in front of a computer running SuperlabTM software.

For the Mental Rotation trials, participants read instructions describing our two-part response paradigm. With this paradigm, participants, on each trial, first view a fixation cross and then see the mental rotation pair. They are to hit the space bar when they know whether the figures are the same or mirror images. This will be referred to as the know response. After hitting the space bar the same pair appears and participants respond same or different, using designated keys. This will be referred to as the answer response. We employed this two-part response paradigm to separate RT for mental rotation from RT for other aspects of choice reaction time tasks (Donders, 1969). Participants progressed through trials at their own pace. The experiment was divided into two blocks (whole figures and cut figures) that were randomly counterbalanced across participants. Each block contained 63 trials, yielding 126 total trials.

After completing both experimental blocks, all participants completed the Individual Differences questionnaire. Participants in the framed group completed the study later and they additionally completed the Mental Representation questionnaire along with the Individual Differences questionnaire.

Results

Coding and analysis

Data from seven people were eliminated for not following instructions properly. Moreover, one participant had lower than 50 % overall accuracy rate, and two participants were outliers and their data were not included in the analysis. Analyzed data included RT to the know response and accuracy of answer responses. Given the high response variability, data were trimmed by eliminating each participant’s highest and lowest know RT responses (Bush, Hess, & Wolford, 1993).

Statistical assessment used mixed-factor ANOVA with a 7 (Angular Disparity: 00, 300, 600, 900, 1200, 1500, 1800) × 2 (Figure Consistency: whole, cut) × 2 (Framing: framed, unframed). Angular Disparity and Figure Consistency served as within-participant factors and Framing as a between-participant factor. For brevity’s sake, we first report results of this omnibus ANOVA, which is the main focus of interest in this study. We ran another mixed-factor ANOVA with a 7 (Angular Disparity) × 2 (Figure Consistency) × 2 (Sex: men, women) design. We then replaced factors of habitual spatial thinking (Preference and Mental Representation) with Sex. We did not run all these factors concurrently in order to conserve power. Considering that the other main focus of interest is the interaction between habitual spatial thinking on strategy-consistent stimuli in mental rotation, we included Preference and Mental Representation into the analysis, one by one. Sections reporting sex differences and habitual spatial thinking results only relate additional reports beyond the omnibus ANOVA.

Data were analyzed using a mixed design ANOVA and Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of sphericity has been violated for Angular Disparity for RT data, X 2 (20) = 130, p < .001, and for the accuracy data, X 2 (20) = 51.4, p < .001. Therefore, degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser estimates of sphericity (ε = .65) for RT, and Huynh-Feldt correction (ε = .87) for accuracy. Assumption of sphericity has also been violated for Angular Disparity* Figure Consistency interaction; RT data, X 2 (20) = 58.3, p < .001, and accuracy data, X 2 (20) = 41.1, p = .004. On both RT and accuracy, degrees of freedom were corrected using Huynh-Feldt estimates of sphericity; ε = .92, ε = .94, respectively.

Mental rotation performance

The mean RT for the experiment is 6,240 ms and overall accuracy is 77.9 %. Results showed the classic ADE with increased RT, F(3.89, 350) = 134, p < .001, ηP 2 = .60, (Fig. 2a), and decreased accuracy, F(5.19, 467) = 56.23, p < .001, ηP 2 = .39 (Fig. 2b) as a function of Angular Disparity. The error bars represent standard errors for all the figures in the manuscript.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Both Know RT (a; left) and accuracy (b; right) as a function of Angular Disparity

Do strategy-consistent-stimuli affect mental rotation performance?

The analysis showed an effect of Figure Consistency. Participants responded faster and more accurately to whole (MRT = 5794 ms, Macc = 82.5 % accuracy) than to cut objects (MRT = 6,685 ms, Macc = 73.3 %), F(1, 90) = 14.4, p < .001, ηP 2 = .14 for mean RT and F (1, 90) = 53.9, p < .001, ηP 2 = .38 for accuracy.

Figure Consistency interacted with Angular Disparity with both mean RT, F(5.49, 494) = 7.88, p < .001, ηP 2 = .080, and mean accuracy, F(5.66, 509) = 9.84, p < .001, ηP 2 = .099. For small (30°, 60°) angular differences, participants rotated whole and cut figures equally fast. At greater angular disparities, they rotated whole figures faster (see Fig. 3a) and more accurately (Fig. 3b).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Response times (a: top) and correct percentages (b: bottom) to whole and cut figures on degrees of rotation. Note: Paired t-test analyses were done to compare whole and cut figures at each degree of rotation. Results are depicted with asterisks. * denotes significance criterion of .05 (p < .05) for the corresponding degree of rotation and ** denotes the significance criterion of .001 (p < .001)

Does framing stimuli affect mental rotation performance?

Framing did not affect mental rotation performance by itself in rotation rate, F(1, 90) = 2.24, p = .14, or rotation accuracy, F(1, 90) = .022, p = .88. However, it interacted with Figure Consistency for mean RT data, F(1, 90) = 4.22, p = .038, ηP 2 = .045. When the figures were unframed, participants responded equally quickly to whole and cut figures, whereas when there was a frame around, cut figures generated slower responses (see Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Response rates to whole and cut figures when they are unframed or framed

There was also a three-way interaction between Frame × Figure Consistency × Angular Disparity for mean RT data, F(5.49, 494) = 2.87, p = .012, ηP 2 = .031. This 3-way interaction is a further depiction of distinct slower response time to both framed and cut stimuli. For unframed stimuli (see Fig. 5a), participants responded similarly to whole and cut figures for 300, 600, 1500, and 1800. However, for framed stimuli (see Fig. 5b), they responded equally fast to whole and cut objects at smaller angular differences (30°, 60°), but the difference was more evident for greater angular differences (>60°).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Performance to whole and cut figures over degrees of rotation is shown as a function of frame. (a) (top) Mean response times for un-framed stimuli and (b) (bottom) shows mean response times for framed stimuli. Paired t-test analyses were done to compare whole (darker line) and cut (lighter line) figures at each angular disparity, for both unframed and framed conditions. * significance at .05 level (p < .05), ** significance at .001 level (p < .001)

Does sex of the participant affect mental rotation performance?

Sex did not show a main effect for RT, F(1, 90) = 2.96, p = .09. However, women responded to Mental Rotation figures less accurately (Macc = 73.1 %), F (1, 90) = 17.57, p < .001, ηP 2 = .16 than men (Macc = 81.6 %). Sex did not show any further interactions with Angular Disparity or Figure Consistency on either RT or accuracy data, all Fs < 1.0.

Does habitual spatial thinking affect mental rotation performance?

Spatial information preference (henceforth referred to as Preference) was categorized on participants’ self-reports on learning spatial information—maps or verbal descriptions. Sixty-three people preferred maps, 26 people preferred verbal directions, and three preferred both when given spatial information. Those selecting “both” were not included in the analyses. In Preference, it is important to know the extent to which it overlaps with Sex (Table 1). Men (80.4 %) and women (57.9 %) preferred maps more than verbal descriptions, and men preferred map descriptions over verbal directions more so than women, c2(1, N = 89) = 5.33, p < .05. Importantly both sexes have representation in both preference groups.

Table 1 Sex ratios in reference to preference and representation

Map preference participants responded more accurately (M = 81.4 %) than verbal preference participants (M = 70.1 %), F(1, 87) = 27.8, p < .001, ηP 2 = .24. The data also showed a Preference × Angular Disparity interaction for accuracy, F(5.56, 484) = 5.57, p < .001, ηP 2 = .060. Participants who preferred verbal directions responded less accurately than those with map preference over degrees of rotation (Fig. 6), and their responses varied across different angles while map preference showed a steady decline as a function of angular disparity.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Accuracy rates for people preferring maps and verbal directions over degrees of rotation. The dark line shows map preference while light line shows verbal preference. Paired t-test analyses depict the significant comparison for each degree of rotation. Both * and ** show significant results. * presents alpha level at .05 (p < .05) and ** marks a more stringent significance criteria (p < .001)

Mental representation (henceforth referred to as Representation). The Representation questionnaire was only administered to participants later in the study and those people were the ones who were exposed to Framed stimuli. As with Preference, it is important to know the extent to which Sex and Representation overlap (see Table 1). Men were categorized as having more survey-centered representations than women, who had more landmark-representations (c2(1, N = 89) = 7.39, p < .05). Women showed habitual tendencies toward primarily using landmark-centered representations (77.3 %) and men primarily used survey-centered representations (62.5 %) and both sexes were represented in both landmark and survey groups.

Overall, survey-centered individuals responded faster (survey: 5,543 ms; landmark: 7,374 ms), F(1, 44) = 7.20, p = .01, ηP 2 = .14, and more accurately (survey: 81.8 %; landmark: 75.2 %), F(1, 44) = 4.87, p = .033, ηP 2 = .10. However, Representation data did not result in any interactions with Angular Disparity or Figure Consistency (all Fs < 1.0). especially after corrections according to sphericity assumptions.

Discussion

Consistent with findings within the literature, we showed the classic Angular Disparity Effect (ADE) in Mental Rotation task: participants decided whether a pair of 3-D figures were the same or different more slowly and less accurately as the angular disparity between the figures increased (Blough & Slavin, 1987; Collins & Kimura, 1997; Cooper, 1975; Heil & Jansen-Osmann, 2008; Kail et al., 1979; Shepard & Metzler, 1971). Experiment 1 results did not replicate the commonly reported sex finding with men outperforming women in rotation rate while men were still more accurate than women. Thus, our findings relating to performance on mental rotation did show a sex difference, but this difference was not as robust as the literature suggests (Collins & Kimura, 1997; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Masters & Sanders, 1993; Voyer et al., 1995). The idea of other individual differences contributing to the difference in mental rotation performance becomes more important.

Current research extended previous mental rotation studies by introducing strategy-consistent stimuli (namely, cut versions of whole figures). To our knowledge, the combination of factors examined here is new to the literature. Stimulus characteristics, particularly whether the stimuli themselves are presented as whole or cut, contribute to response strategies. We predicted that having figures in pieces would promote piecemeal processing while whole figures would enable more holistic processing. Consistent with our prediction, participants rotated whole figures faster and more accurately. Moreover, they rotated cut figures more slowly and less accurately as angular disparity increased, a pattern suggested to account for strategy differences (Kail et al., 1979; Strasser, et al., 2010). Thus, although with caution, we can state that Experiment 1 was successful in using both whole and cut figures to reflect holistic and piecemeal strategies used in mental rotation. Whole figures seemed to promote a holistic strategy and cut figures promoted a piecemeal strategy.

Experiment 1 also introduced frames around the figures. Framing the stimuli, overall, did not affect rotation performance. However, framing the cut figures seemed to hurt their processing speed. Responses to cut figures slowed down more as angular disparity increased. Frame seemed to heighten performance differences between whole and cut figures, especially for greater angular disparities, mimicking the cognitive strategy pattern. With framed stimuli, whole figure rotation mirrored holistic processing while cut figures reflected piecemeal approach. When there was no frame around the figures, however, no consistent pattern between whole and cut rotations was observed. These findings support the motivation behind stimulus manipulations showing that framing emphasized the unitary configuration of an object, especially for whole figures.

Whether a person prefers to receive spatial information via maps or verbal directions, and the way she represents that information (survey vs. landmark; Pazzaglia & DeBeni, 2001) was hypothesized to reflect strategy differences in mental rotation. Rotation performance differed for both preference and representation factors; map preference was more accurate and became more accurate as angular disparity between the rotated pairs increased, mimicking the holistic strategy pattern suggested for men. Also, participants who relied more on survey-centered representations were more accurate and faster in mental rotation performance than people who used landmark-representations. Thus, habitual spatial thinking seems to affect rotation performance, and showing signs of possible strategy differences overriding sex differences. However, all these findings need to be evaluated with great caution. Participant numbers, especially after breaking down sexes into preference or representation styles, reduce the power to report the findings in confidence.

Experiment 2

In Experiment 1, surrounding 3-D figures with 2-D rectangular frames affected mental rotation performance. Experiment 2 used frames of different shapes (circular and rectangular), still 2-D, to explore the frame’s role, either as emphasizing the unity of the object (circular and rectangular) or providing a reference frame for comparison (rectangular only). Different versions of the typical mental rotation paradigm use different shaped frames (Borst, Kievit, Thompson, & Kosslyn, 2011; Hoyek, Collet, Fargier, & Guillot, 2012; Kooshabeh & Hegarty, 2010; Miller & Halpern, 2014; Neuburger, Jansen, Heil, & Quasier-Pohl, 2012; Vandenberg & Kuse, 1978). In Experiment 2, we compared rotation performance of two frame types to un-framed figures in order to extensively test framing effects. And since the main focus was on frame types, we did not include cut versions of the stimuli.

Method

Participants

Fifty-three Tufts University students (35 women, 18 men) participated for partial course credit. None had participated in the previous experiments.

Materials

Mental rotation stimuli

Experiment 1’s 3-D stimuli (only whole ones) were again used, combined with three frame types: no-frame, rectangular (Experiment 1) and circular. When present, frames surrounded each figure (Fig. 7). Stimuli were paired to have same (yet rotated) or mirror-reflection combinations with a vertical line dividing the pair. In total there were 126 trials for each frame type, yielding 378 trials. Of the 126 trials, 63 were the same and 63 were mirror reflections. Each angular difference was represented 18 times within a frame type.

Fig. 7
figure 7

Stimuli with different frame types: (a) unframed, (b) rectangular frame, and (c) circular frame

Questionnaires

The individual differences and spatial mental representation questionnaires used in Experiment 1 were again used here.

Design

In Experiment 2 stimuli were manipulated. In addition to the Framing conditions of Experiment 1, Experiment 2 also included circular frames.

Participants were categorized on sex, preference, and representation based on their self-reports. Thus, the present study was based on a 3 (circular-framed, rectangular-framed, unframed, within participant) × 2 (individual factors: sex, preference, or mental representation, between participant) mixed design. The dependent variable was the performance on the Mental Rotation test.

Procedure

The procedure followed that of previous experiments. Participants sat in front of computers after consenting to partake in the experiment. Instructions preceded each of the three experimental blocks, defined by the frame types. Block order was counterbalanced across participants. On each trial participants first saw a fixation cross, followed by a pair of figures. Participants responded “same” or “different” by pressing designated keys. After the task participants completed both questionnaires.

Results

Coding and analysis

Dependent measures included accuracy and RT. Data were trimmed by eliminating each participant’s highest and lowest RT at each angular disparity (Bush et al., 1993). Three participants’ data were eliminated because they either did not follow instructions (2) or their accuracy was lower than 50 % (1).

Repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) on mean percentage of correct response and RT data was conducted with 3 (Frame Type: unframed, rectangular, circular) × 7 (Angular Disparity: 00, 300, 600, 900, 1200, 1500, and 1800) × 2 (Sex: men, women; or Preference: map, verbal directions; or Representation: survey landmark). Angular Disparity, and Frame Type were within-participant factors. Individual difference measures, including Sex, Preference, and Representation were between-participant factors of primary interest. Sex and habitual thinking (preference, representation) were analyzed separately adding each to the omnibus ANOVA in order conserve power and effect sizes, as were done in Experiment 1. According to Mauchly’s test, the assumption of sphericity was violated for Angular Disparity for RT data, X 2 (20) = 220, p < .001, and for the accuracy data, X 2 (20) = 50.8, p < .001. And degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser estimate for RT data of (ε = .30) and Huynh-Feldt for accuracy data, (ε = .89) for accuracy. Angular Disparity* Frame Type interaction also violated the sphericity assumption, for RT data: X 2 (77) = 219, p < .001, ε = .60 (Greenhouse-Geisser correction); and for accuracy data: X 2 (77) = 103, p < .05, ε = .89 (Huynh-Feldt correction).

Mental rotation performance

The mean RT for the experiment was 4,024 ms and the overall accuracy is 73.2 %. Results showed the ADE with increasing RT, F(2.05, 105) = 91.9, p < .001, ηP 2 = .64, and decreasing accuracy, F(5.31, 271) = 57.1, p < .001, ηP 2 = .53, with increasing angular disparity (see Fig. 8a and b).

Fig. 8
figure 8

(a) (left) shows mean reaction times and (b) (right) shows mean accuracy at each degree of rotation

Does framing stimuli affect mental rotation performance?

Frame Type also affected mental rotation performance only in rotation speed, F(2, 102) = 3.62, p < .05, ηP 2 = .066. Simple contrasts comparing frame types showed similar rotation performance with circular and rectangular framed stimuli (Mcir = 4,601.7 ms; Mrec = 4,362.9 ms), however participants rotated unframed stimuli faster (Munfr = 3,785.9 ms) than either frame type (p’s < .05).

Does sex of the participant affect mental rotation performance?

Sex did not show any main effect for RT, F(1, 51) = .003, p = .96, or accuracy data, F(1, 51) = 2.99, p = .089. However, there was a marginal Sex × Angular Disparity interaction with the accuracy data, F(5.31, 271) = 2.17, p = .054, ηP 2 = .041. With increasing angular disparity, the performance gap between men and women slightly increased (Fig. 9).

Fig. 9
figure 9

Shows mean correct response for men and women at each degree of rotation. Note that * shows the significance level at .05 level (p < .05)

Does habitual spatial thinking affect mental rotation performance?

Preference did not affect performance alone (F = 3.29). The distribution of Preference among sexes shows a tendency for both women (74 %) and men (78 %) to prefer maps (Table 2). Preference did not differ by Sex, c2(1, N = 53) = .078, p = .78.

Table 2 Sex ratios in reference to habitual thinking styles (preference and representation)

Rotation rate data showed a Preference × Angular Disparity interaction, F(2.21, 113) = 5.59, p < .05, ηP 2 = .099. The ADE was more pronounced for participants preferring verbal directions (Fig. 10).

Fig. 10
figure 10

Levels of preference over degrees of rotation for mean reaction time. Post-hoc analyses revealed significant results between map and verbal preference at each degree of rotation; * showing p < .05 and ** marking p < .01

The Preference × Angular Disparity interaction can be further qualified by marginally significant three-way interaction between Preference, Angular Disparity, and Frame Type, F(7.36, 375) = 1.96, p = .057, ηP 2 = .037. Mental rotation speed for participants preferring verbal directions differed as a function of both angular disparity and frame, showing the greatest ADE for circular frames followed by rectangular frames and least for no frame (see Fig. 11b). However, with respect to participants who preferred maps, they showed an ADE, but no effect of frame (see Fig. 11a). Accuracy data did not show any interactions (all Fs < 1.0).

Fig. 11
figure 11

Mental rotation performance of stimuli with different frame types over degrees of rotation as a function of preference. (a) (on the left) shows performance of map preference, and (b) (on the right) performance of verbal preference with mean reaction times

Representation

Both men and women showed both habitual spatial representation approaches (landmark, and survey). Women were more inclined to use a landmark approach (57 %) and men relied more on a survey approach (56 %). However, each category had representation from both sexes (see Table 2). Women and men did not show a difference in categorized representation style, (c2(1, N = 53) = .769, p = .380). Representation did not affect rotation performance either for mean RT data, F(1, 51) = 1.74, p = .19, or for accuracy data, F(1, 51) = .85, p = .36.

Discussion

Overall, results showed the standard ADE (e.g., Collins & Kimura, 1997; Shepard & Metzler, 1971) with participants rotating 3-D figures more slowly and less accurately as the angular disparity between figures increased, consistent with Experiment 1 results. There was no overall sex difference, but mental rotation performance differed with greater AD with men’s slight advantage in accuracy. Again, consistent with Experiment 1, we did not find robust sex differences.

Framing affected mental rotation performance; having a frame around the stimuli, overall, slowed rotation rate, regardless of the Frame Type. The Multiple Systems Framework (Zacks & Michelon, 2005) may explain this effect of framing. Mental rotation involves object-based transformations. Adding a frame introduces a reference frame external to the object, potentially leading to conflicting mental rotation approaches. Another explanation involves a potential conflict in dimensionality between the frame (2-D) and the 3-D figures. The 2-D frame may serve as a flatness cue (Eby & Braunstein, 1995), suggesting that people also think of the figures as 2-D (Reinhardt-Rutland, 2003). Inconsistencies interpreting represented dimensionality may hinder overall performance. Implications of the findings will be further discussed in the general discussion section.

Experiment 2 further explored individual differences in habitual spatial processing (Preference and Representation). Participants preferring verbal directions (compared to maps) had a stronger ADE with response time. This difference was further emphasized by frame type. The ADE separated for the different frame types (unframed, circular-, and rectangular) within participants with a verbal preference. This finding further supports the idea that how people habitually process spatial information reliably affects rotation performance, especially when introducing various types of stimuli.

Experiment 3

Three main modifications were done in Experiment 3. (1) We changed the dimensions of the surrounding box to further investigate the effects of framing. Since Experiment 2 results led us to suspect a possible frame-figure dimensionality mismatch, we replaced the structure around the stimuli with a 3-Dimensional box. (2) Whole and cut versions of mental rotation figures were again used to replicate Experiment 1 results to account for the strategy use. (3) There was an increase in the number of participants in order to ensure that habitual thinking categories (Representation and Sense of Direction) would be similarly represented across sexes.

Method

Participants

One hundred and ten WPI students (60 women and 50 men) participated for partial course credit. Participants were taking Introductory to Psychology as an elective course.

Materials

Mental rotation stimuli

Three-dimensional block figures from Experiment 1 were again used. The Framed condition in Experiment 1 had 2-D rectangular shapes surrounding each figure. Here, 3-D prism boxes were created to replace 2-D boxes to frame the stimuli. Thus, the Framed condition in Experiment 2 had 3-D shapes surrounding each figure. Each box was approximately 189 (height) × 152 (length) × 47 (width) in size (see Fig. 12). The Unframed condition had the same stimuli as in Experiment 1 (see Fig. 12a, further top left and Fig. 12b, top left). Stimuli pairs were presented as whole (Fig. 12a) and and cut (Fig. 12b) and paired to have the same (yet rotated) or different (mirror-reflections) combinations with a vertical line dividing the pair.

Fig. 12
figure 12

(a) (left) A pair of whole-framed figures while (b) (right) shows a pair of cut-framed stimuli

There were four blocks of trials: whole-unframed, whole-framed, cut-unframed, cut-framed. Each block started with 12 practice trials that were followed by 63 test trials. Within each block, participants were exposed to each rotation angle nine times, totaling to 36 times throughout the experiment. Overall, participants completed 280 trials excluding practice sessions. Of these test trials, 140 of them showed whole versions and 140 of them showed cut versions of these; 140 of them were unframed and 140 of them were framed; and 140 were the same pairs and 140 were different pairs.

Questionnaires

Two questionnaires were used in Experiments 1 and 2: (1) the Individual Differences Questionnaire, which was the basis for Preference, and (2) the Mental Representation Questionnaire (Pazzaglia & De Beni, 2001), which was used to categorize participants according to their Representation styles. In Experiment 3, the Individual Differences Questionnaire was replaced with a more comprehensive and widely used self-report on preference and sense-of-direction; the Sense of Direction (SOD). The Santa Barbara Sense-of-Direction Scale was used to assess environmental spatial ability and has been reported to correlate with objective spatial tasks (Hegarty, Richardson, Montello, Lovelace, & Subbiah, 2002). It consists of two demographic questions on sex and age of the participant, followed by 15 positive and negative statements such as “I am very good at giving directions” and “I very easily get lost in a new city,” respectively. Participants report their assessments for each statement on a Likert-type scale where 1 indicates strongly agree, and 7 indicates stongly disagree. As a result, each participant receives a computed score and a higher the score indicates a higher sense of direction (Hegarty et al., 2002).

Design

The present study had two independent variables: frame and figure consistency. For the frame variable participants saw block figures without a frame, and within a 3-D frame. For the figure consistency variable, they were exposed to whole stimuli and cut versions of them.

Participants were categorized on sex, representation, and SOD according to their self-reports. Thus, the present study was based on a 2 (un-framed vs. framed, within participant) × 2 (whole vs. cut, within participant) × 2 (individual factors: sex, representation, or SOD, between participant) mixed design. The dependent variable was the performance on the Mental Rotation test.

Procedure

The procedure was very similar to Experiments 1 and 2. The main distinction was the pre-screening. Participants completed the Representation Questionnaire prior to the experiment to ensure recruitment of roughly equal number of participants per category. When the participant came to the lab, he/she signed the consent form and completed the computerized Mental Rotation task via Inquisit. Instructions preceded each of the four experimental blocks, defined by stimuli conditions. They either saw a block of whole figures first, or a block of cut figures, and the order was counterbalanced across participants. A trial consisted of a fixation cross, presented for 500 ms, followed by a pair of figures. Participants responded “same” or “different” by pressing designated keys, the placement of which was also counterbalanced. After completing the task, participants filled out the SOD scale.

Results

Coding and analysis

Dependent measures included accuracy and reaction time (RT). Data from five participants were eliminated because they were outliers (two people) or had accuracy rates below 50 % (three people). Participant’s highest and lowest RTs at each angular disparity were trimmed because of high variability (Bush et al., 1993).

A mixed-design ANOVA was used with within-subject factors of Angular Disparity (00, 300, 600, 900, 1200, 1500, and 1800), Figure Consistency (whole, cut) and Framing (unframed, framed) and between-participant factors: individual difference measures: Sex (men, women), Representation (survey, landmark), and SOD (high, low). Between-subject factors were analyzed separately by adding each, one by one, to the omnibus ANOVA.

Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of sphericity was violated for Angular Disparity for RT data, X 2 (20) = 182, p < .001, and for the accuracy data, X 2 (20) = 68.5, p < .001. Therefore, degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser estimates of sphericity (ε = .51) for RT, and Huynh-Feldt correction (ε = .84) for accuracy. The assumption of sphericity has also been violated for Angular Disparity* Figure Consistency interaction; RT data, X 2 (20) = 35.2, p = .019, and accuracy data, X 2 (20) = 49.9. On both RT and accuracy, degrees of freedom were corrected using Huynh-Feldt estimates of sphericity; ε = .95, and ε = .91, respectively.

Mental rotation performance

The mean RT for the experiment was 4,932 ms and the overall accuracy was 72.8 %. ADE with increased RT, F(3.05, 253) = 235, p < .001, ηP 2 = .74, (Fig. 13a), and decreased accuracy, F(5.05, 551) = 98.05, p < .001, ηP 2 = .47 (Fig. 13b) as a function of Angular Disparity.

Fig. 13
figure 13

Both reaction time (RT) (a; left) and accuracy (b; right) as a function of Angular Disparity. Note that the mean RTs are depicted on a 1,000–7,000 ms scale and mean correct response displays 50–100 % scale to show the effect more clearly

Do strategy-consistent-stimuli affect mental rotation performance?

The analysis showed a main effect of Figure Consistency. Participants responded faster and more accurately to whole objects (MRT = 4763 ms, Macc = 77.1 % accuracy) than to cut ones (MRT = 5101 ms, Macc = 68.6 %), F (1, 83) = 4.49, p = .037, ηP 2 = .051 for RT and F (1, 109) = 116, p < .001, ηP 2 = .52 for accuracy.

Figure Consistency interacted with Angular Disparity with both RT, F(5.70, 473) = 7.26, p < .001, ηP 2 = .080, and accuracy, F(5.48, 597) = 6.15, p < .001, ηP 2 = .053. Participants consistently responded less accurately to cut figures for angular disparities and the performance difference for whole and cut figures was more evident at most degrees of rotation (Fig. 14b). For RT data, participants rotated to whole and cut figures equally fast (Fig. 14a).

Fig. 14
figure 14

Mean response times (a: top) and mean correct percentages (b: bottom) to whole and cut figures over degrees of rotation are depicted. Note the y axes for reaction time (RT) (1,000–7,000 ms) and accuracy (50–100 %) for a closer display. Post-hoc t-tests for both RT and accuracy data show significant results with asterisks. * alpha level of .05 (p < .05); ** significance at the .01 level (p < .01)

Does framing stimuli affect mental rotation performance?

Framing did not affect rotation performance by itself, F(1, 83) = 1.49, p = .23, and it did not show any interactions with the other factors (all Fs < 1.0).

Does sex of the participant affect mental rotation performance?

Sex did not show a main effect for RT, F(1, 107) = 1.63, p = .20 but men responded to 3-D figures more accurately (M = 75.8 %), F(1, 106) = 7.41, p = .008, ηP 2 = .067 than women (Macc = 70.5 %). Sex did not show any further interactions with Angular Disparity or Figure Consistency, for either RT or accuracy data, (all Fs < 1.0).

Does habitual spatial thinking affect mental rotation performance?

Mental representation (henceforth referred to as Representation) was categorized in similar numbers for each sex (see Table 3). Both women (57 %) and men (56 %) used slightly more landmark-centered representations, c2(1, N = 110) = .005, p = .944. It is important to note that both sexes were categorized in similar ratios in landmark and survey groups. Both sexes preferred map descriptions over verbal directions, c2(1, N = 110) = .535, p = .464. Representation was not found to be significant as a main effect; either for RT data, F(1, 107) = .30, p = .58 or for accuracy data, F(1, 108) = .31, p = .58.

Table 3 Sex ratios in reference to representation and sense of direction

Sense of Direction (SOD) was categorized on participants’ self-reports on rating their spatial information processing. The SOD score for each participant was computed and the ones above the mean were categorized as having high sense of direction and the ones scored below the mean were grouped as having low sense of direction. Fifty-one people scored as high SOD, and 59 people were categorized as low SOD (Table 3). Of those with high SOD, 58.8 % were women and among the ones with low SOD, 50.8 % were women. Both men and women were close in SOD distribution. There was no main effect of SOD, F(1, 107) = .050, p = .82, among high and low SOD groups.

Discussion

Consistent with Experiments 1 and 2, the ADE effect was observed for both mental rotation rate and accuracy. As was found in Experiment 1, modifying 3-D stimuli to rotate whole and cut versions seemed to enforce the suggested strategy use. Participants were faster and more accurate in rotating whole figures as opposed to cut figures. More interestingly, the difference between whole and cut figure rotation was more evident for greater degrees of rotation, especially for accuracy data.

Unlike Experiments 1 and 2, however, framing with 3-D boxes did not affect the rotation performance. It might be because of the congruency between the dimensions of the frame and the figure. Consistent with previous research findings, participants did not have 2-D frames to serve as flatness cues (Eby & Braunstein, 1995) or were forced to think the figures in 2-D to match the frame (Reinhardt-Rutland, 2003).

Replicating results found in Experiments 1 and 2, the only sex difference was found with men being more accurate than women. However, there was no difference in terms of rotation rate. Again, we can conclude that we did not find a robust sex difference, considering the modifications with the stimuli. Habitual spatial thinking styles (representation, and SOD) did not suggest any performance or strategy differences either and these will be further addressed in the general discussion.

General discussion

Many everyday activities engage mental rotation. Going back to Dave and Judy trying to place their sectional in their new living room, Dave might be rotating the couch as a whole, leading to a more effective decision about its placement. In contrast, Judy may have been mentally moving the individual parts. Having the walls of the room around the furniture, or interpreting the furniture as 3-D may have been another factor that affected the difference in their performance. The present research explored the contributions of stimulus differences and individual differences in habitual spatial processing to mental rotation performance.

It is important to note that our findings replicated the classic angular disparity effect (ADE; e.g., Blough & Slavin, 1987; Collins & Kimura, 1997; Cooper, 1975; Heil & Jansen-Osmann, 2008; Kail et al., 1979; Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974; Shepard & Metzler, 1971). ADE shows that rotation performance is directly affected by the angular difference between objects.

Do stimuli characteristics affect mental rotation performance?

Between Dave and Judy, would their rotation process be influenced by how sectional was brought in: one piece or within pieces? Would incorporating the walls of the room into this process help the task or disrupt it? In terms of stimulus differences, the holistic and piecemeal strategies suggest that people conceptualize the classic Shepard-Metzler figures differently when engaging in mental rotation. The present research explored two different stimulus manipulations. First, we had whole and cut versions of the stimuli, predicting that cut figures would promote a piecemeal strategy. Second, we placed frames around individual stimuli within a pair. We had two different predictions for this manipulation (Experiments 1 and 3). The frame could emphasize the unity of the figure (Rock, 1990), leading to a more holistic approach or the frame could disrupt object-based reference frame (Zacks & Michelon, 2005) use during rotation, thus impeding a holistic strategy.

Cutting the whole stimuli

Results suggested that participants used mental rotation strategies in line with the stimulus consistency for 3-D figures. The rotation of cut figures was more consistent with a piecemeal strategy and rotation of whole figures with a holistic strategy. Participants more rapidly and accurately rotated whole compared to cut 3-D figures and this difference widened with increasing angular difference, as was shown in Experiment 1. This finding suggests participants used holistic rotation with whole figures and a piecemeal approach with cut figures. However, this finding was only evident when the frames were 2-D structures (Experiment 1) and did not replicate with 3-D frames (Experiment 3). In other words, 2-D structure around the figures emphasized the unitary configuration of the figures (Rock, 1990) engaged a more holistic strategy, whereas a 3-D frame did not affect or interfere with the use of strategies.

Framing the stimuli

Our predictions for how framing would affect mental rotation were based on previous results within the literature. Framing did indeed interact with stimulus consistency and showed somewhat different findings across our studies. In Experiment 1, rotating 3-D figures within 2-D frames slowed cut figure rotation rate and more so with increased angular disparity, while no difference based on figure consistency emerged for unframed stimuli. In other words, results showed rotation strategies consistent with stimulus consistency (holistic for whole objects; piecemeal for cut objects) with framed 3-D stimuli. In Experiment 2, rotating 3-D figures within circular or rectangular 2-D frames negatively affected rotation rate in comparison to unframed figures. Participants rotated figures within some kind of 2-D structure more slowly. In Experiment 3, however, there was no effect of frame. Taken all the findings into consideration, we can suggest that having a frame around the stimuli affects rotation, only if the frame dimension is incongruent with the target. When participants are faced with an incongruent mental rotation problem, they might need to change their perception of the figure itself because a 2-D frame suggests flatness (Eby & Braunstein, 1995) and/or decreased depth (Reinhardt-Rutland, 2003).

How do individual differences affect mental rotation performance?

Sex has been the most studied individual difference with respect to mental rotation. We suggest that habitual spatial processing may help explain possible sex differences. Going back to the initial example, does Dave prefer maps and Judy prefer verbal directions? Does Dave usually form a more global, configural representation of larger environments while Judy thinks about the larger scale layouts in terms of its landmarks? If Dave and Judy differ in these ways, would they use different strategies for mental rotation? With a series of experiments, this study investigated how one’s habitual thinking relates to his/her rotation strategy (Jordan et al., 2002; Kail et al., 1979; Thomsen et al., 2000). The habitual approaches we examined included preference for spatial information format (maps vs. verbal directions) and spatial representation approach (survey vs. landmark). Preference for maps and survey representation should both predict a more holistic, configural mental rotation strategy.

Sex

Sex differences in the current study were not as robust as has been suggested in the literature (Collins & Kimura, 1997; Kimura, 1999; Kail et al., 1979; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Voyer et al., 1995). In all 3 experiments, men were more accurate in rotation than women. However, there were no additional effects. As hypothesized, sex may not be the only explanatory factor in assessing and understanding the difference in mental rotation performance.

Habitual spatial thinking

Across the experiments, more women and men preferred maps over verbal directions while categorization across mental representations varied more. Still, those distributions show that sex is not the primary factor which determines one’s preference regarding or mental representation of spatial information, or general sense of direction. We hypothesized that a Map Preference and a habitual Survey Representation should align with a holistic rotation strategy. People preferring maps rotated more accurately (Experiment 1) than those preferring verbal directions, and the difference was more pronounced with greater angular disparity (Experiments 1 and 2). Also in line with our hypothesis, people who habitually use a survey representation made more accurate and faster rotations (Experiment 1) than the ones taking a landmark approach (see also Pazzaglia and De Beni, 2006). This suggests that habitual spatial processing may help explain sex differences in mental rotation. To our knowledge this is the first finding linking habitual spatial thinking to rotation strategy use. However, these findings should be evaluated very carefully, because the results did not carry over to Experiment 3. One possible explanation was the difference in sample populations across experiments. Experiments 1 and 2 collected undergraduates from Tufts University. For Experiment 3 we recruited undergraduates at Worcester Polytechnic Institute (WPI), an institution attracting students with technical skills, preference, or inclinations. Research suggests that having superior spatial ability is correlated with a tendency towards Science-Technology-Engineering-Math (STEM) fields (Uttal & Cohen, 2012) as well as success in these areas (Kozhevnikov, Motes, & Hegarty, 2007; Vasilyeva & Lourenco, 2012). Since we tested participants already studying in STEM fields, and since the students were roughly equally distributed across representation and SOD categories, possible effects due to habitual spatial processing might have been washed out.

Conclusions

Taken together the present studies extend extant findings on factors contributing to mental rotation strategy use in several ways. First, different features of the stimuli themselves play a role. In the current work this included figure consistency (whole vs. cut), presence of a frame around a stimulus, and, looking across studies, frame dimensionality. Second, how people habitually think about spatial information predicts mental rotation strategy use. Third, stimulus characteristics and individual differences in habitual spatial processing interact. In all cases, factors predicting or promoting a holistic strategy push strategy use in that direction and vice versa for the piecemeal strategy. This also suggests some cognitive flexibility in mental rotation strategy use.

The current research has implications for everyday life. We interact with maps or walk through rooms, buildings, or streets trying to create mental maps. Yet, we may reach a detour sign on a known route or have our global positioning (GPS) device fail. These situations would require us to mentally manipulate spatial information. Going back to the couple Dave and Judy, there might have been a difference between their rotation performances not merely because of their sex, but possibly because of some other factor that is perceived in the surrounding environment, the nature of the object they were rotating, or the way they have come to learn to process spatial information.