Behavior analysis is both “alive and well” (Baum 2000, p. 263) despite what some students of mainstream psychology might think. As a new graduate student in behavior analysis, I find that my colleagues and I are troubled when our field is misunderstood, and even more so when it is misunderstood by prominent scholars from other fields. Of course, people who misunderstand the field are not entirely to blame. As Skinner (1977) stated, “I long ago adopted the basic rule in animal research that the organism is always right. It does what it is induced to do by its genetic endowment or the prevailing conditions” (p. 1007). People, too, have idiosyncratic ontogenic behavioral histories (Skinner 1981). Thus, critics of behavior analysis are not to blame for misunderstanding behavior analysis. To understand why others perpetuate misunderstandings of behavior analysis, we would need to critically examine the prevailing conditions that give rise to those misunderstandings, which is outside the scope of the present paper. For our part, we behavior analysts bear the burden of adequately understanding our own field before speaking for the field. This puts me and my colleagues—a new generation of students in behavior analysis—in a predicament: How can one champion the field if one does not fully understand the field?

Like any other science behavior analysis has evolved. Its philosophical precursor, behaviorism, can be attributed to John Watson’s (1878–1958) epistemic advancement from science infused with mentalism (Fredericks 2006). For example, Watson (1913) criticized the psychology of consciousness studied through introspective methods and advanced the psychology of behavior studied with objective methods of experimentation, and advocated for the goals of prediction and control of behavior. Unfortunately, many of the anti-behavioral views can be attributed to mischaracterized representations of Watson’s own work (Lattal & Rutherford 2013; Todd & Morris 1992). At best, even Skinner’s publications of works such as Walden Two in 1948 and Beyond Freedom and Dignity in 1971 spurred controversies; at worst, these controversies sparked criticism and misunderstanding.

Just as Watson wrote over a century ago, “I do not wish unduly to criticize psychology” (1913, p. 163), neither do I wish for behavior analysis to be criticized. As Brave New World Revisited reminds us:

Everything that is done within a society is done by individuals. These individuals are, of course, profoundly influenced by the local culture, the taboos and moralities, the information and misinformation handed down from the past and preserved in a body of spoken traditions or written literature; but whatever each individual takes from society…will be used by him in his own unique way. (Huxley 1958, p. 105)

Although not a behavior analyst, Huxley’s quote resonates with Skinner’s statements about the importance of prevailing conditions. As other behavior analysts have suggested (Poling 2010; Schlinger 2010), I too urge students of behavior analysis to broaden their perspectives by reading and publishing beyond the boundaries of our field and to challenge the tensions produced by the psychology of the Zeitgeist!

Behavior analysts have written extensively about the most common misunderstandings and strategies for correcting them (e.g., Kestner & Flora, 1995; Todd & Morris, 1992). To build on these discussions, I attempted to investigate the popular opinion of behavior analysts when it comes to misunderstandings of the field. I distributed an electronic survey through behavior analytic listservs (Teaching Behavior Analysis [TBA-listserv]) and social media accounts (personal tweets and Facebook groups [Association for Behavior Analysis International, The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, Behavior Analysis in Practice, Disseminating Behavior Analysis, Students of Applied Behavior Analysis] wall posts). The rationale for recruiting via the internet was to survey the perspectives of the widest possible range of behavior analysts. The purpose of the survey was to provide information about how, in the opinion of respondents, graduate students should respond when they hear other people voice misunderstandings of behavior analysis.

Respondents were 167 individuals from behavior analytic listservs and social media outlets (M age = 36.6 years, SD age = 11.9 years; 57 % female). The respondents selected from a list the positions that most resembled their current ones. The reported positions were clinicians (44 %), researchers and professors (27 %), students (16 %), and others (13 %). With respect to reported time spent working, 29, 28, 23, 15, and 5 % reported working 6–10, 2–5, 11–20, >20, and about 1 year(s), respectively. The survey described five situations in which someone misunderstood, and in that sense misrepresented, behavior analysis and was overheard by a graduate student in behavior analysis (Table 1). Respondents were asked to select from a list of eight responses how the student should respond. The options ranged from “Yes, any graduate student in this context should attempt to correct this misconception” to “No, graduate students should never attempt to correct this misconception in this context (Table 2).”

Table 1 Hypothetical scenarios with misunderstandings of behavior analysis
Table 2 The eight responses provided in each scenario

Table 3 shows the percentage of respondents who selected each response option for each of the five situations. In every case, the most selected response, with 46–67 % of respondents selecting it across scenarios, was “Yes, any graduate student in this context should attempt to correct this misconception” (response 1). Overall, the second-most-often selected response, chosen by 14–17 % of respondents across scenarios, was “Yes, but only once they’ve completed at least one course in behavior analysis” (response 2). Response 8, “No graduate students should ever attempt to correct this misconception in this context,” was selected slightly less often than response 2, with 6–20 % of respondents selecting response 8 across scenarios.

Table 3 Percentage of responses for each scenario

Based on my initial assumption that graduate students should have a good understanding of behavior analysis before speaking for the field, the findings of the survey were surprising. The majority of respondents indicated that any graduate student should serve as an ambassador for behavior analysis, even a first-year student, and attempt to correct misunderstandings. A limitation of this survey, however, was that it provided limited response options. A few emails from respondents indicated a desire for conditional responses, such as “if the student did well in a course” or “if the student had direct behavior-analytic experience.” These comments seem to suggest that there are limitations on the conditions under which students should serve as ambassadors for the field and that the actual opinions of respondents are not as clear cut as the reported results suggest.

For example, one respondent indicated that he or she wished for response options that were individualized to the scenario being presented. Another stressed that competency in a particular area, a dimension not directly reflected in the response options provided, is more important that degrees held or current position in determining how a person should respond to the scenarios provided. For example, perhaps an individual who specializes in the area of feeding disorders should not defend behavior analysis against misrepresentations pertaining to substance abuse. Giving experts in behavior analysis an unconstrained opportunity to address the issue of how graduate students should respond when they hear their field misrepresented will extend the findings reported here, provide practical guidance for graduate students, and undoubtedly prove interesting. I invite them to address the following questions:

Questions for the Experts

  1. 1.

    Given that the majority of respondents indicated that any graduate student should attempt to correct misconceptions, what possible steps can graduate students take to defend the discipline from misunderstandings by people outside of the field?

  2. 2.

    Speaking on behalf of the behavior-analytic community obviously occurs during any point of professional development. At what stage of professional development should behavior analysts reach out to the broader scientific community to disseminate behavior analysis?