Introduction

With 13% of the world adult population classified as obese, the prevalence of obesity is an important global health concern [1]. This concern has grown as the worldwide prevalence of obesity has increased nearly threefold since 2016 [1]. While adult obesity rates vary among the global regions, the rates are particularly high in the U.S. where 37% of adults are classified as obese [2]. Although the reported rate of obesity among military service members is lower than that of the overall U.S. adult population, obesity is nevertheless a concern for the military. Between 1995 and 2008, combined rates for overweight and obesity among active duty personnel rose from 50.6 to 60.8%, with most of this change due to an increase in the rates of obesity from 5.0 to 12.7% [3]. More recently, data from the 2015 Health Related Behaviors Survey suggest a continuation of this trend with an obesity rate of 14.7% among U.S. active duty personnel [4]. According to this survey, the Army, with an obesity rate of 18.0%, has the highest obesity rate among the branches of the armed services.

The primary concern about obesity in military service members is reduced force readiness. Military service is innately demanding, and body composition standards are thought to play an integral role with regard to physical fitness, overall health, and maintaining a military appearance [5]. A body mass index (BMI) of 30 kg/m2 or greater increases the risk of premature discharge from the Army [6]. In 2006, an estimated 1,200 first-term military service enlistees were prematurely discharged as a result of their failure to meet weight standards, and the cost associated with the early separation of these individuals was an estimated $61 million [7]. Compared to those with a normal BMI, soldiers who are obese have a 33% greater risk of lower extremity musculoskeletal injury or disorder (e.g., sprains, stress fractures, joint derangements, osteoarthritis) [8]. Service member with excessive body fat face an increased risk of heat injury [9], and those who are obese are twice as likely to fail their fitness readiness tests when compared to non-obese service members [10]. National security concerns have even been raised [11], as the U.S. armed services appear to be struggling to recruit eligible individuals who meet weight stands amidst the obesity epidemic [12, 13].

In addition to military-specific concerns, there are also concerns for the overall health of service members. An analysis of data from the Millennium Cohort Study, a large prospective study of service members and former service members, found that obesity was associated with hypertension, diabetes, sleep apnea, and coronary heart disease [14]. Within this study, those with obesity were also more likely to screen positive for depression and PTSD compared to participants with a normal body weight. A more recent study found the prevalence of prediabetes and diabetes to be increased among U.S. Air Force members with a BMI ≥ 30 [15].

There is a growing body of evidence suggesting an important relationship between body weight status and cognitive functioning [16]. A review by Smith et al. [17] found a clear association between obesity and neurocognitive deficits, particularly in executive functioning. Executive functions are a family of higher-order cognitive processes used in overriding instinct and in taking goal-directed action [18, 19]. Although there are three primary executive functions, namely cognitive inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility [19], there are various approaches to measure each. Obesity is associated with deficits in numerous executive function outcomes, including inhibition, cognitive flexibility, working memory, decision-making, verbal fluency, and planning [16]. Among girls, poor executive function predicts greater increases in BMI from age 10 to 16 [20]; whereas, better working memory was found to predict a greater weight loss among individuals participating in a weight loss program [21]. Further, cognitive remediation therapy which targets executive functions and thinking style is linked with greater decreases in binge eating and weight loss [22]. Overall, executive functions appear to play a crucial role in regulating behaviors related to one’s BMI.

Relative to what is known regarding the relationship between neurocognitive function and body weight status within the general population, the topic remains essentially uncharted within the military. To better understand the relationship between obesity and neurocognitive function among service members, an analysis of the variables related to these constructs was performed using data collected as part of a cross-sectional study designed to investigate the relationship between lifestyle factors and psychological resilience among deploying U.S. soldiers. We hypothesized that obese soldiers will have significantly lower scores in executive functioning compared to non-obese soldiers.

Method

Subjects

Data from a previous study to investigate the relationship between lifestyle factors and psychological resilience [23] were used for the analyses in the present study. Because of operational time constraints, not all participants in the original study were able to complete the neurocognitive assessment necessary for inclusion in the present study. Of the 260 Soldiers included in the original study, only 109 provided the data necessary for the present study. For two independent samples, based on a modest obesity prevalence rate in the U.S. of 31.8%, a total sample size of 65 would have provided sufficient power (95%) for detecting superiority in mean scores (two tailed) between groups with an alpha Type I error rate of 5% (0.05) and beta Type II error rate of 20% (0.20) [24]. Based on this sample size calculation, our sample of 109 participants (non-obese, n = 79 vs obese, n = 29) would have been sufficiently powered to reject the null hypothesis.

Participants were from two different brigade combat teams scheduled for deployment to Iraq between 2009 and 2010. Participants provide written informed consent. Compensation was not offered. Data collection occurred at the brigade combat teams’ Soldier Readiness Processing sites. The protocol was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects, Institutional Review Board, at Kansas State University.

Materials

Demographic characteristics, health-related behaviors

Height, weight, demographic characteristics and health-related behaviors were self-reported using a questionnaire. A “fill-in-the-black” format was used to record height, weight, and age in years. Education level, sex, and race/ethnicity were measured using response option categories as reported in an earlier study describing the neuropsychological performance of an active duty military sample [25]. Smoking status was measured by asking, “Which of the following best describes your smoking habits?” and providing the following response options: “never smoked”, “current smoker”, or “ex-smoker”. Alcohol use was measured by asking, “Which of the following best describes your usual alcohol consumption during the last week?” and providing the following response options: “none”, “less than one drink per day”, “1–2 drinks per day”, “3–4 drinks per day”, and “more than 4 drinks per day”. Physical activity was measured using a question developed to take into account regular soldier physical training (i.e., PT). Participants were asked, “During the last 6 months, how many days per week have you done moderate or intense physical activities (including PT) for at least 10 minutes at a time?” with response options including “0”, “1”, “2–3”, and “4 or more.”

Neurocognitive battery

Neurocognitive functioning was measured using the Combat Stress Assessment [26], which was administered on a handheld computer. The cognitive tasks included, simple reaction time, code substitution, procedural reaction time, go–no–go, matching to sample, code substitution recall, and several versions of the Stroop test. Performance was quantified using throughput scores [27].

  1. a.

    For simple reaction time, a symbol appears on the screen and participants respond by tapping the symbol as quickly as possible.

  2. b.

    For code substitution, a key of symbols and numbers appear on the screen. A symbol and number pair also appears on the screen below the key. Participants respond by indicating whether or not the symbol and number pair matched the key.

  3. c.

    For procedural reaction time, numbers appear on the screen and participants respond by tapping the box that corresponded to each of the numbers.

  4. d.

    For go–no–go, an asterisk or a plus sign appears on the screen and participants respond by tapping the screen when the asterisk appears or not tapping the screen when the plus sign appears.

  5. e.

    For matching to sample, a pattern appears on the screen briefly. Participants are then presented with two patterns and respond by tapping the pattern that that matches the initial pattern.

  6. f.

    For code substitution recall, a number and symbol pair appear on the screen and participants indicate whether or not the pair match the key they were provided in the code substitution test.

  7. g.

    The Stroop test consist of a series of words of various colors appearing on the screen. Participants are instructed to respond to the words by tapping the color rather than the meaning of each word. Four different versions of the Stroop test were administered including the congruent version (color and meaning of word agreed), the incongruent version (color and meaning of word differed), the neutral version (nonsense words appeared), and the emotional version (combat-related words such as “I.E.D” and “convoy” appeared).

Statistical analysis

The data were analyzed using SPSS version 25.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY). Using the standard mathematical formula, weight (kg) / [height (m)]2 [28], BMI was calculated using the height and weight as recorded on the self-administered questionnaire. Body weight status was categorized as either obese (BMI ≥ 30 kg/m2) or non-obese (BMI ≤ 29.99 kg/m2) [29].

Descriptive analyses were performed to characterize the sample. Demographic and health-related behavior responses were collapsed into meaningful categories to allow for comparison of body weight status (i.e., obese vs non-obese) using Chi-square tests (Χ2). Data were inspected for the appropriate assumptions for the statistical test used. Fisher’s exact test values were examined in instances where the Chi-square expected cell counts were less than five. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) with Bonferroni adjusted pairwise comparisons was used to analyze body weight status differences across neurocognitive variables. Binary logistic regression analysis with backwards elimination (Wald) was used to ascertain the contribution of the frequency of weekly moderate-to-intense exercise (dummy coded) and scores on neurocognitive measures for which there were significant differences based on body weight category. Removal of variables from the regression analysis was based on a significance of p < 0.10; while, two-tailed tests and a significance level of p < 0.05 were used for all analyses.

Results

Sample characteristics

The majority of the participants were male (92.7%; n = 101) and married (56.9%; n = 62). Roughly, two-thirds (68.8%; n = 75) identified themselves as non-Hispanic Caucasian. The majority had not attained a college degree (72.5%; n = 79). Age in years ranged from 19 to 50 with a mean age of 29.0 (95% CI [27.6–30.5]). Few were current smokers (27.5%; n = 30), and few reported regularly consuming more than two alcoholic drinks per day (11.0%; n = 12). Sixty percent (59.6%; n = 65) reported completing moderate-to-intense physical activity four or more days per week. Additional sample characteristics are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of sample

Body weight status

The BMIs ranged from 18.9 to 37.7 kg/m2 with a mean BMI of 27.4 kg/m2 (95% CI [26.7–28.2]). The prevalence of obesity was 26.6% (n = 29). Participants who reported moderate-to-intense physical activity two or more times per week were less likely to be classified as obese compared to those reporting less frequent physical activity, p = 0.004 Fisher's exact test. Body weight status did not differ significantly based on sex (p = 1.0 Fisher’s exact test), age (≤ 24, 25–32, or ≥ 33 years of age, p = 0.131), ethnic/racial category (non-Hispanic Caucasian or all others, p = 0.625), education level (college degree or less, p = 0.993), smoking status (current smoker, former smoker, or non-smoker, p = 0.882), marital status (married or not, p = 0.125), or alcohol use (one or more drinks per day or less than one drink per day p = 0.533).

Group differences in neurocognitive functioning

The results of the MANOVA for neurocognitive measure differences based on body weight status were significant, F(10) = 1.99, p = 0.043, ηp2 = 0.17 (Wilk’s Lambda), and there were no violations in homogeneity of variance, (Levene’s test, p > 0.05). There were significant group differences in throughput scores on the neutral, p = 0.023, and emotional, p = 0.013, versions of the Stroop, but not the congruent, p = 0.114, or incongruent versions, p = 0.176. In addition, there were significantly group differences in throughput scores on only one other subtest of the Combat Stress Assessment, the code substitution task, p = 0.020, but not simple reaction time, procedural reaction time, go–no–go, matching to sample, or code substitution recall. The results of pairwise comparisons (Bonferroni adjusted) revealed that scores on these subtests were significantly lower for the obese group compared to the non-obese group, p < 0.05 (Table 2).

Table 2 Group body weight differences on neurocognitive measures

Using a one-way ANOVA, there were no significant differences in cognitive scores between body weight categories in the frequency of weekly moderate-to-intense exercise (1 vs. 2–3 vs. 4 or more times per week), p > 0.05.

Associations between body weight status and measures of executive function

A binomial logistic regression analysis was performed with obesity as the dependent variable and physical activity and each of the neurocognitive tests for which there were significant differences based on weight status category. The results indicated that the model was significant; Χ2(5) = 15.18, p = 0.010, and accounted for approximately 19% of the variance (Nagelkerke R2 = 0.19) and correctly classified 73% of the participants. After extraction, only emotional Stroop scores, Wald Χ2(1) = 5.22, p = 0.022, B = − 0.04, and frequency of moderate-to-intense physical activity, Wald Χ2(1) = 6.85, p = 0.009, B = − 2.27, were significant predictors of weight status. The results suggest that when all other variables are held constant, with a unit increase in the frequency of weekly moderate-to-intense physical activity (greater than 1 × weekly), the odds of being obese is reduced by 0.10 times or 90% (OR = 0.10, 95% CI [0.02, 0.57]). The results also suggest, when all other variables are held constant, the odds of being obese is reduced by 0.97 times or 3% (OR = 0.97, 95% CI [0.94, 1.00]) with every unit increase on the emotional Stroop which for the full sample had a standard deviation of 16.14 points. Overall, the results suggest an inverse relationship between BMI status and both measures of executive functioning and frequency of exercise.

Discussion

In the current study, we found that U.S. soldiers who are categorized as obese compared to non-obese have poorer performance on tasks of executive functioning, namely both the neutral and emotional Stroop tests and the code substitution test, which supported our hypothesis. More specifically, those who were categorized as obese had lower scores in cognitive inhibition. The results also revealed that the odds of being obese was reduced by 3% with each unit increase on the emotional Stroop test. More impressively, increasing the frequency of moderate-to-intense exercise to greater than one time weekly reduced the odds of being obese by 90%.

Most certainly, obesity is linked to lifestyle factors, and the lifestyle changes concomitant with military service have been shown to be associated with a reduction in BMI and weight loss among overweight and obese individuals [30]. The results of the current study revealed that increasing the frequency of moderate-to-intense exercise weekly reduces the odds of being obese by 90%. This is consistent with earlier findings in which increased physical activity has been shown to be independently associated with lower fat mass [31]. As such, findings such as those presented in the present study support the importance of the Dietary Guidelines for Americans highlighting the need for regular physical activity in weight maintenance [32].

Measures of self-regulation of eating and physical activity have been found to be lower among obese individuals [33], and an increasing number of studies are finding an association between executive function and obesity-related behaviors. Cross-sectional surveys have shown impulsivity, measured by delay discounting, positively associated with frequent fast-food consumption [34] and symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder which are believed to arise from a deficit in executive functioning [35] positively associated with symptoms of binge eating [36]. Likewise, consumption of fruit and vegetables as well as saturated fat has been shown to be associated with various measures of executive function [37, 38].

Exercise has been shown to improve executive function in overweigh children [39] and children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [40]. In contrast, in the current study, we did not observe significant differences in cognitive scores between groups based on the frequency of weekly moderate-to-intense physical activity. While Guiney & Machado [41] outlined evidence supporting the cognitive benefits of regular exercise in healthy older adult, they point out the need for additional research to distinguish the cognitive benefits of exercise in young adults.

Although this study is unique in that it is the first to note this occurrence within a military population, the finding of an association between obesity and compromised executive functioning is consistent with a growing body of evidence [16]. A review by Smith et al. [17] found a clear association between obesity and deficits of executive function; however, the nature of the relationship remained unclear with the authors citing evidence suggesting several possibilities including: (a) obesity may be responsible for cognitive deficits, (b) cognitive deficits may contribute to the propensity for obesity, or (c) obesity and cognitive deficits may be linked by a shared genetic predisposition which can be triggered by lifestyle factors.

The cross-sectional design of the study is a limitation, as no causal or directional effect can be inferred. While the current study findings appear to build on what is known in general, research is needed to better understand the relationship between executive function and body weight among military personnel. Although body weight status classification estimates based on self-reported heights and weights were similar to those based on measured values for young adults [42], the use of self-reported anthropometric data to determine body weight status should be considered a potential limitation. In addition, while the cut-point used in this study is well established [29], the use of BMI to categorize service members as obese or non-obese is not without potential concerns [43]. Nonetheless, BMI has been shown to be an accurate discriminator for the classification of obesity among service members based on percent body fat [44].

The use of self-reported health behaviors, including a measure of physical activity that has not been validated in a military population is also a limitation of the study. Lastly, the use of data from a previous study is a limitation. While differences between samples is a potential concern, the demographic characteristics of the participants in the present study were similar to those of the original study [23] as well as in earlier studies involving soldiers. In the present study, 92.7% were male, 56.9% were married, and the mean age was 29.0 years. In the original study [23], 90.8% of the participants were male, 55.8% were married, and the mean age was 29.0 years. In an earlier study of 88,235 soldiers, Milliken et al. [45] reported a sample in which 90.8% of the participants were male, 58.2% were married, and the mean age was 30.4 years. Overall, a strength of the study is that it appears the sample of soldiers in the present study is characteristic of the parent study as well as the larger population of soldiers. Additional strengths of the study include the use of a validated battery of tests [26] to assess neurocognitive functioning and the inclusion of an array of demographic variables. Being the first study to explore executive function and obesity with a military population adds to the strengths of the study as well.

The findings of this study should not be interpreted without consideration of the main ideas that comprise the Healthy at Every Size (HAES) paradigm. Developed as a means to conceptualize overall health independent of body weight, HAES encourages body acceptance, an intuitive or non-diet approach to eating, and a reasonable outlook regarding physical activity [46]. Weight stigma is associated with eating pathologies [47], and the concepts of HAES have proven their worth especially among female populations [48], including within a randomized controlled trial in which HAES interventions were found to decrease eating disinhibition [49].

The military population is predominately male, and male service members report eating for physical rather than emotional reasons [50]. Nonetheless, efforts to meet military weight requirements include unhealthy and extreme weight loss strategies [51]; while, normal body weight status among service members has been found to be associated with greater reliance on internal hunger and satiety cues [50]. Yet, restrained eating may not necessarily be detrimental [52]. A recent review concluded that military weight management interventions can be effective [53]. Although a full HAES approach has not been explored within the military, research exploring the intuitive or non-diet approach appears promising. While there are clearly military-specific risks associated with obesity, the “weight loss at any cost” mindset can result in unfavorable results as well.

Given the rising prevalence of obesity in the military, the results of this study are important in and of themselves. However, executive functions can be improved [19] and recent clinical studies have found that executive function training can foster weight loss. For example, Allom et al. [54] showed that patients receiving Cognitive Remediation Therapy achieved higher executive function scores, reduced habitual sedentary behavior, and potentially reduced habitual unhealthy eating.

In all, the finding of an association between obesity and executive functions is consistent with a growing body of evidence. Future studies are needed and should include researcher-measured anthropometric values and validated measures of physical activity. Studies designed to better understand the direction of the variable associations are also needed to establish better insight and to inform research related to the treatment and prevention of obesity within military populations.

What is already known on this subject?

An important relationship exists between body weight and cognitive functioning. In particular, decrements related to executive function are associated with less favorable body weight measurements.

What this study adds?

Among U.S Soldiers, obesity was associated with less frequent moderate-to-intense physical activity and compromised executive functioning, namely decrements in response inhibition.