Introduction

Regular and sustained participation in physical activity and exercise (one domain of physical activity) has been shown to yield several positive health outcomes, including improvements in mental health conditions [1], cardio-vascular disease [2] and well-being [3]. There is evidence, however, suggesting that for a sub-set of people, exercise can become obsessive, or compulsive (or both), to a point where negative health outcomes are experienced, with physical outcomes including bone fractures, psychological outcomes including increased anxiety, and social outcomes including the detriment of social relationships and financial debt [4]. Several terms have been used to describe the phenomenon, including ‘exercise dependence’ and ‘compulsive exercise’ [5, 6]. Unfortunately, several authors have failed to provide definitions for these terms, making the interpretation of findings in the related literature challenging [7]. In this paper, the term exercise addiction (EA) will be used, as it has been suggested as the most appropriate as it includes aspects of dependence and compulsion [8, 9], and can be defined as ‘a morbid pattern of behaviour in which the habitually exercising individual loses control over his or her exercise habits and acts compulsively, exhibits dependence, and experiences negative consequences to health as well as in his or her social and professional life’ [7].

Early conceptualization in this area suggested a distinction between being highly committed to exercise and EA, with committed, non-EA, subjects exercising mainly for extrinsic rewards, not seeing exercise as central to their life, and suffering fewer and/or less severe symptoms of withdrawal upon cessation [10]. Concurrently, EA subjects exercise for intrinsic rewards, seeing exercise as central to their life, and suffer severe psychological and physiological withdrawal upon cessation [11]. Many theoretical models have been proposed to explain EA, including the Sympathetic Arousal Hypothesis [12], the Cognitive Appraisal Hypothesis [13], the IL-6 model [14], Four Phase model [15], Biopsychosocial model [16]. Furthermore, Egorov and Szabo [17] updated the Cognitive Appraisal Hypothesis with their Interactional Model of EA. Disparities between these models indicate a lack of etiologic consensus amongst researchers, which could be one reason why the phenomenon has not been officially classified as a behavioural disorder in The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5 [18] or the International Classification of Diseases 11th Revision [19].

Several screening measures have been developed for assessing the risk of EA, with earlier ones lacking underlying theoretical frameworks, including the Obligatory Exercise Questionnaire (OEQ) [20] and the Exercise Dependence Questionnaire (EDQ) [21]. More recent screening measures have used underlying theoretical models, such as the Exercise Dependence Scale (EDS) [22], which uses Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4 [23] criteria for substance abuse, and the Exercise Addiction Inventory (EAI) [24], based on Brown’s [25, 26] components of behavioural addictions. Currently, there is no consensus as to which questionnaire is the most appropriate; however, the most commonly used in recent studies are the EAI and the EDS [7].

EA has been shown to be highly prevalent in populations that have either indicated or clinically diagnosed eating disorders (EDs), with prevalence rates in these populations ranging from 29 to 80% [27, 28]. This is supported by Berczik et al.’s [9] classification of EA being sandwiched between ‘body-dysmorphic disorder’ and ‘anorexia nervosa’ on Hollander and Wong’s [29] compulsive–obsessive spectrum. Of the different types of EDs, in-patients with clinically diagnosed restricting-type anorexia nervosa (AN) have been shown to have higher prevalence rates of EA (80%) when compared with binge/purging-type AN (43.3%), purging-type bulimia nervosa (BN; 39.3%), and EDs not otherwise specified (EDNOS; 31.9%) [30]. High prevalence of EA in patients with AN is of particular concern as these subjects have been shown to have the highest mortality rates of all EDs [31,32,33], as well as having an increased risk of osteopenia, osteoporosis, and related fractures [34]. It has been reported that subjects with indicated EDs and EA often present with stress fractures and engage in excessive exercising despite injury, with some subjects reporting exercising because it feels like a compulsion rather than for enjoyment [35, 36]. Considering that weight gain is one of the primary aims of treatment of patients with EDs (particularly AN) [37, 38], excessive exercise can result in longer periods to achieve the desired weight gain, which can be costly from a service provision prospective.

EA has also been shown to be prevalent in populations that show no evidence of indicated EDs (failing to reach published cut-off in ED testing questionnaires). For instance, a recent meta-analysis found 11.4% of health club users had EA in the absence of indicated EDs [39] Some primary studies have also shown significant differences between prevalence rates of EA with and without indicated EDs, with EA in the absence of EDs showing consistently lower prevalence rates than subjects with indicated EDs [27, 28]. Furthermore, a recent meta-analysis explored the prevalence of EA in different populations; however, offers no information on the ED status of the populations [40], making it unclear if the prevalence of EA differs by ED status.

Despite the potential differences in the underlying prevalence and potential adverse events of EA between those without and without indicated EDs, it is unknown whether EA risk differs substantially in subjects with and without indicated ED. Moreover, pooled prevalence rates and differences between pooled prevalence rates are unknown. Therefore, the primary aim of this study was, using meta-analytic techniques, to calculate ORs for EA in subjects with and without ED symptomology, and systematically compare the prevalence of EA according to EA measurement tools. Understanding this is essential to understanding more about these conditions, and could inform the creation of new measurement tools, as well as informing the development of targeted interventions. Considering that primary studies have reported that the prevalence of EA with indicated EDs is higher than those without indicated EDs [27, 28], we hypothesised that when comparing studies systematically we would find that EA is more prevalent in subjects with indicated EDs than without.

Methods

This systematic review was conducted according to the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) criteria and the recommendations in the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement [41, 42].

Search strategy

Two investigators (MT, LS) searched PsycINFO, Medline, SportDiscuss and Open Grey from inception to 30/04/19 for articles written in English. The search terms (title of article) used were (exercise OR physical activity OR fitness OR sport OR sports) and (addition OR dependence OR dependency OR compulsion OR addict) or (maladaptive OR excessive OR compulsive OR obligatory OR obsessive) and (exercise OR physical activity OR fitness OR exerciser OR exercisers OR sport OR sports). The reference lists of the articles included in the analysis were hand-searched to identify additional literature, and conference abstracts were also considered.

Study selection

Titles and abstracts were independently assessed by two authors (MT, LS) for eligibility against the inclusion and exclusion criteria.

Inclusion criteria

Articles were included that met the following criteria:

  1. 1.

    Studies that reported an EA with and without indicated EDs OR or statistics sufficient to calculate such an OR.

  2. 2.

    Written in English.

  3. 3.

    In adults (≥ 18 years).

  4. 4.

    That measured the prevalence of EA in any population using any validated measuring tool of EA with established cut-offs (as per original authors’ guidelines) that define subjects as at risk of EA.

  5. 5.

    Tested for indicated EDs using a validated measure.

  6. 6.

    That used the same study population to determine EA prevalence rates in indicated and non-indicated ED populations (to eliminate population bias).

Exclusion criteria

  1. 1.

    Non-adults (< 18 years).

Data extraction

The following information was extracted by the lead author including demographic [age, sex, body mass index (BMI)] and prevalence (total EA with and without indicated EDs n, EA with and without indicated EDs events n, measuring instrument of EA, measuring instrument of ED) data, and missing information was obtained where possible by contacting lead authors (see Acknowledgments). If prevalence data were missing and the authors did not respond/have access to the data (two attempted contacts to authors over a 1-month period), these studies were excluded. Prevalence data were then converted into ORs. Studies with missing demographic data, but full EA with and without indicated ED prevalence data, were included. Subjects were then categorised into two groups: subjects that failed to meet published cut-offs (as defined by the original author article) for EDs in the non-indicated ED group, and subjects that scored over the published cut-offs for EDs in the indicated ED group. In both ED status sub-groups, subjects that met the published cut-offs for EA were, respectively, categorised as EA.

Meta-analysis

A random-effects model was conducted, weighting studies based on the inverse variance, and calculating odds ratios (ORs) and prevalence rates with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis version 3 [43]. The primary aim was to calculate ORs of EA in populations with and without indicated EDs. The meta-analysis was conducted in the following steps: 1. ORs of EA comparing those with and without indicated EDs were calculated with 95% CIs using a mixed effects analysis. 2. Heterogeneity was assessed with the Cochrane Q [44] and I2 [45] statistics for all analyses. 3. Sub-group analysis comparing ORs of EA in populations with and without indicated ED by EA measurement tool. Publication bias was assessed with a visual inspection of funnel plots and with the Begg–Mazumdar–Kendall’s tau [46] and Egger bias test [47]. As per recommendations from Fu et al. and Sterne et al. [48, 49], these tests were only conducted if the number of studies exceeded ten. If the Egger bias test was significant, to adjust for potential publication bias, the trim-and-fill adjusted analysis was used to remove the most extreme small studies from the positive side of the funnel plot and effect sizes re-calculated, until the funnel plot was symmetrical with the new effect size [50]. A sensitivity analyses was calculated around the primary analyses, using a one-study removed method. This was to detect whether the observed effect was overly influenced by any one study.

Results

The literature search yielded 1375 results, of which 369 were removed as duplicates, leaving 1006 studies screened using title and abstract. From the 1006 titles and abstracts screened, 223 studies were selected for full-text review. Of the 223 studies reviewed, 9 studies were eligible for inclusion. Reasons for exclusion are shown in Fig. 1 and descriptive statistics for included studies are shown in Table 1. From the 9 included studies, there were a total of 2140. 1732 subjects scored below published ED cut-offs and were categorised as the non-indicated ED group, of which 342 scored above EA cut-offs and were defined as exercise addicted. 408 subjects scored above the published ED cut-offs and were categorised as the indicated ED group, of which 225 scored above EA cut-off and were defined as EA. The methods of measuring EA were the Exercise Dependence Questionnaire [21], the Exercise Dependence Scale [22], the Obligatory Exercise Questionnaire [20], and the Exercise Addiction Inventory [24]. The methods of measuring for indicated EDs were the Eating Attitudes Test 40 [51], the Eating Attitudes Test 26 [52], the Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire [53], the Eating Disorder Inventory-2 [54], and the SCOFF Questionnaire [55].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Prisma flowchart of included studies

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of included articles

Meta-analysis results

ORs of EA in populations with and without indicated EDs

The pooled OR of EA in populations with indicated EDs compared to those without indicated EDs was 3.71 (95% CI 2.00-6.89; I2 = 81.159; p  ≤ 0.001; Egger bias = 2.054 p = 0.480; trim-and-fill adjustment not required). The meta-analysis forest plot is shown in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Forest plot showing odds ratios of exercise addiction in populations without vs with indicated eating disorders

Sensitivity analysis

The direction or significance of the ORs was not changed by the sensitivity analysis, with point estimates ranging from 3.019 to 4.755. One study [56] had a large effect of the magnitude of the result, with the removal of this study yielding an estimate of 4.755 (95% CI 2.875–7.863; p  ≤ 0.001)

Sub-group analysis of EA prevalence in populations with and without indicated ED by EA measurement type

As shown in Table 2, the OR of EA among subjects with indicated vs no-indicated EDs was highest when measured with the OEQ (6.9; 95% CI 2.2–21.8), followed by the EAI (4.2; 95% CI = 1.4–12.2), the EDS (3.9; 95% CI = 1.2–12.5), with the EDQ yielding the lowest OR (2.4; 95% CI 1.0–5.7).

Table 2 Odds ratios of risk of exercise addiction with and without indicated eating disorders by exercise addiction measurement type

Discussion

This meta-analysis of 9 studies demonstrated that the OR of EA in populations with vs without indicated EDs was 3.7. The sensitivity analysis showed that the direction and significance of the findings were unchanged when one study was removed. ORs also differed largely in both populations depending on the EA measurement tool being used.

Subjects who score above published cut-offs for EDs are over three times more likely to be at risk of EA, with observed prevalence rates in subjects with indicated EDs comparing well with EA studies conducted on clinical ED populations [35, 57]. One possible reason is that excessive exercise has been consistently shown to be an inherent part of several types of EDs, with patients demonstrating aversions to weight gain and showing obsessions towards not gaining weight [58]. Furthermore, ED subjects have been shown to score higher on addictive personality measures and obsessive–compulsive behaviours [59]. Considering this, future studies that use clinically diagnosed ED patients (of all types of EDs) would be useful. Given that EA can be secondary to an ED and with the results of this study suggesting that subjects who show ED symptomology have significantly higher prevalence of EA, this adds to the evidence suggesting that practitioners working with ED patients should consider monitoring exercise levels a priority, as ED patients have been shown to suffer from serious medical conditions as a result of excessive exercise, such as fractures, increased rates of cardiovascular disease in younger patients and increased overall mortality [34].

The large difference in EA prevalence observed between indicated and non-indicated ED groups adds to evidence suggesting that ED symptomology should be screened for in all studies that measure EA. The current meta-analysis excluded 93 studies that failed to measure ED symptomology, which agrees with recent reviews suggested that the EA literature has not readily distinguished between ED status as standard practice [60]. Currently, to measure EA and screen for EDs, two questionnaires are needed. Future work to create a new tool that screens for EDs and EA in one single tool would be beneficial. Not only would this benefit researchers by only having to use one tool to categorise EA with and without indicated EDs, it could also be beneficial in both clinical and public health settings by highlighting at-risk subjects earlier, which could inform (in ED subjects) specialised nursing observation and bathroom supervision to regulate EA behaviours be implemented earlier in treatment. Moreover, earlier categorisation of EA with an indicated ED has the potential to allow practitioners such as general practitioners, physiotherapists and health practitioners to therapeutically explore EA at an earlier point.

The large differences in prevalence rates by EA measurement tool are a further indication that differing EA measurement tools are measuring different aspects of the same phenomenon, with the two tools with no underlying theories (OEQ and EDQ) yielding higher ORs than the two tools that use underlying addiction theories (EAI and EDS). To date, no studies have been conducted comparing subjects who score above the published thresholds for different EA measurement tools and clinical interviews to determine the sensitivity of these tools. It is, therefore, difficult to recommend a specific measurement tool. It is the authors’ view that the EAI and EDS be used until such studies are conducted, as they both are based on underlying theories of addiction and have been described as comparable by several authors [8, 9].

Limited research has been conducted exploring possible treatments for EA. Much of the focus of treatment has been focussed around cognitive behaviour therapies, yet their effectiveness has been shown to be limited [61]. Adams, Miller and Kraus [62] suggested specific therapeutic guidelines for subjects with EA, although Lichtenstein et al. [63] comment that studies to test these guidelines are sparse. Although one individual pharmacological case study has been conducted with an EA patient showing lower EAI scores post-4 weeks of taking an anti-psychotic drug (Quetiapine) [64], no other pharmacological treatments have been explored. Due to the higher rates of serious injury and mortality amongst ED patients, treatment for ED should be prioritised over treatment specifically for EA, although further research is required to explore the relationships between the two so that effective treatments can be refined and/or developed.

While this meta-analysis is the first to measure EA prevalence rates in populations with and without indicated EDs, the findings should be considered within the limitations of this study. First, the heterogeneity of population groups and measurement tools (for EA and ED) and very small sample sizes means that this should only be considered a broad overview; further studies are needed to determine more accurate prevalence rates, using homogeneous tools. Second, the use of questionnaires for testing for EDs has limited applications to clinical diagnoses. Third, the use of the questionnaires in this study precluded the sub-categorisation of different types of EDs, which is relevant as previous research has shown prevalence rates to differ depending on the type of ED [30]. Moreover, athletic subjects who use ED testing questionnaires have been shown to under-report due to possible stigmatisation, with false-positives a possibility [53, 65]. Lastly, there was high heterogeneity which we could not fully explain.

What is already known?

It is known that exercise addiction exists both as a primary condition without indicated eating disorders, and as a secondary condition to an eating disorder. What is unknown is the magnitude of risk for EA with an indicated eating disorder vs no-indicated eating disorders.

What does this study add?

Subjects with indicated eating disorders are over 3.5 times more likely to suffer from addiction to exercise than their non-indicated eating disorder counterparts. Due to the higher risk of mortality in subjects with eating disorders, this study adds to the evidence that exercise levels should be closely monitored in these populations. Furthermore, the absence of eating disorders does not preclude the risk of being addicted to exercise. Further study in this area to explore treatments and the effect on quality of life is warranted.

Conclusion

The OR for EA in populations with vs without indicated EDs is 3.7, with EA being significantly more prevalent in subjects with indicated EDs than in subjects without indicated EDs, adding to the evidence that practitioners working with ED subjects should closely monitor exercise levels. However, even in those showing no indicated EDs, EA is of notable prevalence—EA should not be discounted entirely on the basis of no indicated ED behaviours. It is also recommended that all future research exploring the prevalence of EA test for EDs to determine accurate prevalence rates.