Introduction

Interpersonal functioning is considered as a key etiological factor explaining both the development and maintenance of eating disorder (ED) symptoms [1,2,3]. In line with the Cognitive-Interpersonal Maintenance Model [4], anorexia nervosa (AN) results from a combination of vulnerability and maintenance factors that include cognitive, emotional and social features such as lack of mental flexibility, compulsory behaviours, greater reactivity to stress and insecure attachment or social avoidance. According to the meta-analysis conducted by Caglar-Nazali et al. [5], individuals with AN experience significant impairments in core facets of social processing, especially perceived social inferiority, sensitivity to social dominance, and reduced self-agency. Supporting the observed social difficulties in AN patients, especially in AN patients with a restrictive form (AN-R), studies have shown that AN-R is associated with impaired recognition of emotional expressions and more facial avoidance [5], cold social responses [1], increased physical distance for comfortable social interactions [6] and higher social anhedonia [7]. According to Treasure and Schmidt [4], social impairments might deteriorate the social interactions of AN-R subjects, increase their social isolation and worsen their symptomatology.

In this project, we focused on social emotions that might have a deleterious impact on AN-R social interactions. Specifically, we examined whether AN-R patients are particularly prone to experiencing envy and Schadenfreude, which are emotions related to the fortune of others or evoked by social comparison [8]. Envy is a negative emotion that “occurs when a person lacks another’s superior quality, achievement, or possession and either desires it or wishes that the other lacked it” [9]. This definition thus suggests that there are two sub-types of envy: benign and malicious. Although they both induce negative feelings, the primary motivation in benign envy is to develop and evolve to reach the achievement of others or obtain the same possessions, whereas malicious envy is associated with the motivation to hurt or diminish others to make them lose their advantages [10, 11]. Schadenfreude is defined as a pleasant emotion elicited by the misfortunes of others [11], generally occurring when the others are “maliciously” envied or disliked [12, 13]. Importantly, experiencing these emotions increases the feeling of shame, inferiority, anger or resentment [10] and is negatively perceived by others [14]. Consequently, if AN-R patients are prone to experiencing these emotions, they may be subject to more negative affect and social isolation, thus risking maintaining or even worsening their symptomatology. Importantly, unlike other social deficits suffered by AN-R patients (e.g., decoding others’ mental states, cold attitude), experiencing these emotions may adversely impact both their intra- (e.g., self-esteem) and interpersonal functioning (e.g., being negatively perceived by others). Therefore, experiencing these emotions may be particularly deleterious for these patients, so this issue needs to be addressed.

The literature provides arguments in favor of this hypothesis. Indeed, higher experiences of Schadenfreude and malicious envy have been associated with narcissism, low self-esteem and high social comparison [10, 12, 15,16,17], which are frequently reported by AN-R patients and which predict low body satisfaction [18,19,20]. As a result, AN-R patients may experience higher Schadenfreude and malicious envy as a way to restore their low self-esteem. Specifically, individuals who experience a self-evaluation threat may experience more Schadenfreude due to their greater need to restore their self-worth [21, 22]. In support of our hypothesis, preliminary research has emphasized the role of envy in eating disorders [23,24,25]. For instance, a qualitative study showed that patients with AN report experiencing envy towards others [24]. It has also been shown that the rate of rejection of unfair offers in an ultimate game (i.e., suggested to reflect feelings of envy towards the other player) tended to be higher in AN patients (restrictive and binge-eating/purging subtypes combined) compared to healthy controls [25].

The main objective of the present study was to go beyond these preliminary findings and to test the hypothesis that AN-R patients experience greater malicious envy and greater Schadenfreude than healthy controls (HC). The investigation of the relationship between AN-R and benign envy was more explorative, so not associated with specific hypotheses. The second objective was to examine whether low reports of self-esteem and a strong tendency to compare oneself socially are related to the experience of these feelings in general and/or in the patient’s care (i.e., toward other inpatients). Finally, because the inclusion criteria for patients were based on DSM-5, which defines the severity of AN-R in terms of body mass index (BMI), we hypothesized that more severe AN-R (lower BMI) would be associated with higher reports of envy and Schadenfreude.

Method

Participants

Twenty-six female inpatients with restrictive anorexia nervosa (AN-R) were recruited in inpatient units at two psychiatric clinics (Clinique La Ramée, Brussels, Belgium; CHU Lille, France). The diagnoses were obtained from the patients’ medical records (derived from clinical interview) and were based on DSM-5 criteria [26]. Thirty-two healthy women were recruited in the social networks of the investigators and undergraduate students who collected the data. The inclusion criteria included normal weight (BMI 18.50–24.99 kg/m2) and no history of eating disorders, as assessed by the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview [27]. Exclusion criteria for both groups included any major medical or neurological disorders, including those requiring medications, visual impairment, and substance use disorders.

Material

Scenarios We created four scenarios that aimed to induce envy and Schadenfreude in participants (see supplementary data). They were based on previous vignettes, the first part of which was intended to induce benign and malicious envy whereas the second part was intended to induce Schadenfreude [11, 12, 28]. In the present study, the scenarios included these two parts and described a person of the same age and sex (i.e., young adult female) to facilitate the participants’ tendency to project themselves on the characters. For instance, one of the four scenarios described a young female student who obtained a job thanks to familial support. After reading this first part, participants had to rate their experience of benign (“I would like to be in the position of (…)”, I would like to be in the shoes of (…)”) and malicious envy (“I’m jealous of (…); “I would like (…) to fail”) on a 6-point Likert scale (ranging from 1 = totally disagree to 6 = totally agree) (adapted from [12]).Footnote 1 In the second part of each scenario that aimed at inducing Schadenfreude, the character had an unfortunate experience (e.g., being fired from her job). Participants were then instructed to rate the five usual Schadenfreude items on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 6 (totally agree) (“I’m satisfied with what happened to (…),” “I like what happened to (…),” “I couldn’t resist a little smile,” “I actually had to laugh a bit,” and “I felt Schadenfreude”; [29]). Internal consistency across the four scenarios was satisfactory (benign envy: α = .82; malicious envy: α = .68; Schadenfreude: α = .84).

Iowa-Netherlands Comparison Orientation Measure (INCOM French version, [30]) is an 11-item questionnaire that measures the tendency to compare oneself with others on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) (e.g., “I often compare myself with others with respect to what I have achieved in life”). Higher scores indicate higher social comparison. The English and Dutch versions present satisfactory internal consistency and test–retest reliability [31]. Its internal consistency in the present research was satisfactory (α = .81).

Rosenberg’s Self-Esteem Scale (French version, [32]). Participants have to assess on a 4-point Likert scale (from totally disagree to totally agree) the extent to which they agree with 10 items related to their self-esteem (e.g., “On the whole, I am satisfied with myself”). Higher scores indicate higher self-esteem. The French version of the measure presents satisfactory internal consistency and test–retest reliability [32]. Its internal consistency in the present research was satisfactory (α = .68).

The Benign and Malicious Envy Scale (BeMaS [33]). The French version produced by the authors using a back-translation procedure is a 10-item scale that assesses the tendency of participants to experience benign and malicious envy. Participants rate on a 6-point Likert scale (from 1 = totally disagree to 6 = totally agree) the extent to which they agree with each item (e.g., benign envy: “When I envy others, I focus on how I can become equally successful in the future”; malicious envy: “I wish that superior people would lose their advantage”). The Dutch version of the measure presents satisfactory internal consistency [33]. Its internal consistency in the present research was also satisfactory (benign envy: α = .81; malicious envy: α = .86).

Envy towards inpatients Finally, we developed a 6-item questionnaire for AN-R inpatients that assesses benign (3 items; i.e., “I wanted to look the same as other patients”; “I wanted to do as well as other patients;” “I wanted to have the same characteristics/qualities as other patients”) and malicious envy (3 items; i.e., “I feel bad when other patients reach their goals”; “I wanted other patients not to have some qualities”; “I have a negative feeling towards patients whom I consider better than me”) they experienced during the last 2 weeks of their stay towards the other Eating Disorders inpatients (1 = never, 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, 4 = often, 5 = usually; 6 = always). It aimed to test whether envy towards inpatients also occurs in AN-R clinical care and whether it is related to social comparison and self-esteem. Its internal consistency was satisfactory (benign envy: α = .85; malicious envy: α = .71).

Procedure

Participants were tested individually. Once they had signed the consent form, they read the scenarios and completed the questionnaires. At the end, they were fully debriefed about the study. The study lasted 30 min and was approved by the local ethical committee (Comité d’Ethique Hospitalo-Facultaire Saint-Luc-UCL; Belgium).

Data analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using the SPSS software package Version 24. Univariate analysis of variance was used to test the effect of the group on envy, Schadenfreude, self-esteem and social comparison. Pearson correlations investigated the association between variables.

Results

As presented in Table 1, AN-R patients did not significantly differ in age from HC but presented significantly lower BMI. Patients also reported lower self-esteem (medium effect size) and a greater tendency to compare themselves with others (large effect size).

Table 1 Descriptive data (mean, SD, min–max) of demographic information, benign and malicious envy, Schadenfreude, social comparison, and self-esteem in AN-R patients and healthy controls (HC)

Regarding our first objective, results revealed that the scenarios led AN-R patients to experience more benign envy than HC (medium effect size) with no significant difference regarding malicious envy and Schadenfreude. Furthermore, AN-R patients reported that they experienced benign envy “sometimes to often” (M = 3.54; SD = 1.14), but reported “no to rare” experience of malicious envy (M = 1.59 SD = .72) towards the other inpatients.

In respect to our second objective, Table 2 shows that social comparison was significantly correlated with two measures of benign envy (towards inpatients and BeMaS) and with malicious envy measured by the BeMaS. However, there was no significant correlation between self-esteem and envy or Schadenfreude.

Table 2 Pearson correlations between benign and malicious envy, Schadenfreude, social comparison, and self-esteem in all participants

Finally, regarding the third objective, Table 3 shows that lower BMI was associated with higher reports of benign envy, malicious envy and Schadenfreude in the AN-R group only. Compared to HC, the correlations in AN-R patients tended to be significantly different for malicious envy (Z-score = 1.34; p < .10) and Schadenfreude (Z-score = 1.33; p < .10).

Table 3 Pearson correlations between body mass index and all variables (benign and malicious envy, Schadenfreude, social comparison, and self-esteem) in AN-R patients and healthy controls (HC)

Discussion

The aim of the study was to test whether AN-R patients experience greater envy (malicious and benign), Schadenfreude, lower self-esteem and higher social comparison than healthy controls (HC). Moreover, we aimed to examine the associations between BMI and these variables in AN-R patients. Based on self-reported measures and scenarios, results showed that AN-R patients reported lower self-esteem, more social comparison, and experienced more benign envy than HC. Interestingly, lower BMI was associated with higher benign envy, malicious envy and Schadenfreude in AN-R only.

The significant finding about benign envy is in line with previous studies [23, 24] and shows that AN-R patients were more motivated to evolve and reach the status of others than HC. They also reported on average that they experienced benign envy towards the other patients ‘sometimes to often’, suggesting that their general tendency to experience this feeling is also present in the inpatient environment. Although benign envy refers to an unpleasant experience of frustration at not possessing what others have or having their quality, this may have positive effects for AN-R patients. It has been suggested that benign envy resolves this frustration by getting individuals to improve and surpass themselves by performing better [13, 33] and that it occurs when the outcome of the other person is perceived as positive and deserved [11]. Consequently, the presence of other inpatients may be beneficial if weaker symptomatology is perceived as positive and deserved, leading patients to wish to improve their own clinical status. On the contrary, an inpatient environment might lead to deleterious outcomes if AN-R patients perceive, compare and then acquire new problematic eating behaviours. Moreover, it has been shown that benign envy may have no impact if the goal is not reachable [34]. Specifically, higher reports of benign envy led participants to report more motivation to perform better only when they thought they could improve their performances as much as the others. Based on another study, one may even suggest the deleterious effect of benign envy if no self-affirmation opportunity is available [22]. In the latter study, participants with low self-esteem reported higher self-threat (e.g., feeling less good when comparing oneself with the other person) and subsequently higher Schadenfreude towards higher achievers (i.e., other students with high academic performances) when there was no opportunity for self-affirmation (i.e., no possibility to affirm one’s most important value) [22]. Based on these findings, one could thus recommend a balanced attitude in AN-R clinical care by encouraging the positive aspects of benign envy (e.g., surpassing oneself) while remaining particularly alert to the influence of other factors (e.g., opportunity for self-affirmation, reachable goals) and to the risk that benign envy turns into competition, which has been hypothesized to maintain AN-R disorders [35].

The non-significant group differences in malicious envy and Schadenfreude appear to be accounted for by the influence of a third variable, i.e. patients’ BMI, which significantly predicted envy and Schadenfreude. Indeed, despite the questionable role of BMI in defining AN-R severity [36, 37], higher BMI in AN-R patients was associated with feeling less envy (particularly malicious) and Schadenfreude. In other words, when faced with individuals who they perceived as having greater quality, patients with more severe AN-R (i.e., lower BMI) felt greater motivation to surpass themselves or to hurt them and experienced more pleasant emotion as a result of the misfortunes of those individuals. Because BMI was not correlated with self-esteem or social comparison, these psychological factors are likely not involved in this association, or at least not in a linear way. The association between self-esteem and BMI is thought to be moderated by AN recovery stage. For instance, self-esteem was negatively correlated with BMI in acute AN-R patients, whereas these variables were not associated with patients who had already recovered [38]. Therefore, weight gain in AN-R patients with low BMI may be experienced as a failure of the self or as a tendency to deviate from one’s objective, whereas it does not seem to have any deleterious effect in patients who have recovered. Therefore, self-esteem might not be associated with envy, Schadenfreude or BMI in all AN-R patients, due to the complex nature of self-esteem in AN-R. The association might be context-dependent, i.e. when there is no opportunity for self-affirmation, the misfortunes of others might be particularly pleasant for severe AN-R patients (i.e., low BMI) because they might protect or enhance their self-evaluation [21]. Of note, lower BMI may reduce AN patients’ cognitive ability to find new ways to face challenging situations (i.e., flexibility; problem-solving, [39, 40]). This may consequently lead patients to use Schadenfreude as a less cognitively demanding way to enhance their self-evaluation than strategies that require more cognitive resources such as reappraisal [41]. Therefore, although some studies have emphasized the social implications of BMI [42, 43], future studies should also consider other confounders when examining the links between AN-R and interpersonal functioning, i.e. AN-R current and lowest BMI, chronicity, stage of recovery and cognitive functioning.

Concerning the non-significant associations between malicious envy, Schadenfreude and social comparison in AN-R, future studies should focus on upward social comparison rather than the general tendency to compare oneself with others, as envy may result from upward social comparison [34]. This is particularly relevant in AN because upward appearance comparison has been shown to predict future eating disorders [44] and because ED patients reported higher levels of submissive behaviour and a more unfavourable social comparison (i.e., perceiving oneself as worse than someone else) than matched HC [45]. Therefore, unfavourable social comparison in ED patients may deteriorate social interactions perceived as potentially damaging in terms of self-devaluation [3]. If setting one’s own social and body standards higher (i.e., maladaptive perfectionism) were to be associated with difficulties in social comparison and hence social emotions, these high standards would translate into envy and Schadenfreude for those with lower BMI. Future studies could thus focus on the predictive role of upward social comparison and high standards for the self as factors accounting for impaired social interactions in AN-R. This may also account for the non-significant group differences in malicious envy and Schadenfreude that we found.

The study is not free from limitations. First, we cannot rule out the hypothesis of social desirability, especially for such socially unaccepted emotions. Future studies should thus add an implicit measure of Schadenfreude (e.g., recording facial expressions, see [46]) to counter this effect. A second limitation is that other comorbid diagnoses were not collected, so such confounders could not be excluded. Third, the AN-R group had greater benign envy than the control group for the scenarios measure but not for the BEMAS measure, probably due to low power. In addition, although the internal consistencies of all measures were satisfactory, the different measures of benign envy were not inter-correlated. Fourth, the sample size was small, preventing us from examining other research questions such as potential interactions between group (AN-R vs HC) and self-esteem in predicting the different forms of envy. Finally, other clinical data (i.e., frequency of restriction behaviours) should be collected in further studies. Nevertheless, despite these limitations, the study has several strengths including that it is the first to investigate whether AN-R patients are prone to experiencing negative emotions related to the misfortune of others based on three complementary measures. Finally, the study highlighted the significant role of BMI in AN-R patients’ social functioning.