Does cognitive avoidance mediate the relation of anxiety and binge eating

Binge eating is a prevalent and impairing condition marked by psychological distress [1]. Now recognized within the DSM-5 as a psychological disorder, binge eating disorder (BED) is defined as consuming a larger than average amount of food while experiencing a loss of control, along with three or more of the following associated features: eating until uncomfortably full, eating much more rapidly than normal, consuming large amounts of food in the absence of physical hunger, eating alone due to embarrassment over the amount eaten, feeling disgusted, depressed, or guilty afterward [2]. Although BED may be more common among women, binge eating is the most prevalent form of eating disturbance among men [1]. In addition to full BED, subclinical binge eating is also associated with a host of impairments, including psychological dysfunction [35], and may occur at higher rates among men than women [6]. Therefore, dimensional examinations of binge eating symptoms may be an important component to better understand factors that may impact the development and maintenance of binge eating across genders.

Theoretical models of binge eating help to contextualize the symptoms of this deleterious behavior and its correlates. Cognitive avoidance theory posits that distressing emotional states, such as anxiety, creates vulnerability for binge eating, and that binge eating serves as a process through which the salience of negative thoughts is mitigated. In turn, the avoidance of negative thoughts also limits the salience of negative emotions [7], with binge eating functioning as an anxiety reduction strategy [8]. Heatherton and Baumeister coined the phrase “escape from awareness,” [7] to describe the process through which binge eating reduces the salience of negative emotions. They described that cognitive narrowing, a state in which thinking and interaction with the environment occur on a more basic level, is achieved for individuals during in binge eating. For example, thoughts are limited in scope to the direct experiences of binge eating (e.g., sensory experiences) rather than more abstract concepts and beliefs (e.g., self-concept). As such, this theory suggests that because the scope of awareness is limited, the adverse consequences of binge eating are not salient during the act itself [7]. There is some support in the literature thus far for binge eating functioning, in part, as an attempt to reduce negative emotions through cognitive avoidance. Negative affect has been identified as a trigger of binge eating episodes [9, 10], and binge eating has been shown to serve the function of coping with negative affect in empirical evaluations of the “escape from awareness” model [11]. Additionally, there is evidence that reductions in binge eating may occur for individuals following participation in programs to reduce experiential avoidance [12], and that suppression of thoughts related to food (e.g., strong agreement with statements such as “there are foods that I try not to think about” and “I always try to put eating problems out of my mind”) is associated with higher levels of disordered eating pathology among individuals seeking treatment for BED [13]. Empirical evidence supports the notion that binge eating provides relief from negative affect, on at least a temporary, momentary level [14].

To better understand the process through which binge eating may occur, it is also important to consider the particular components of negative affect that could prompt the perceived need for avoidance. The literature has demonstrated an association between high levels of anxiety symptoms and binge eating symptoms [15, 16]. Additionally, previous research has found that anxiety is uniquely important, after considering other components of negative affect, in understanding the severity of binge eating symptoms [6]. This is supported by evidence that binge eating is associated with a reduction in anxiety [17], particularly among individuals with BED [18]. Despite the promising developments in the literature thus far, to our knowledge, there have not been empirical evaluations that have examined cognitive avoidance as a mediator in the relation between the anxiety and binge eating symptoms.

There is reason to suspect that anxiety and cognitive avoidance may have differential relations to binge eating symptoms between women and men. It is well established in the literature that anxiety is more prevalent among women [19], and as previously mentioned, BED rates are higher among women than men [1]; therefore, it may be the case that cognitive avoidance is more salient in the context of anxiety and BED for women as well. While there are few investigations of gender differences of cognitive avoidance in the weight and eating literature, at least one study has found that suppression of food-related thoughts mediates the relation between stress and weight cycling for women, but not for men [20]. In sum, examination of gender differences in the relation of cognitive avoidance, anxiety, and binge eating is a meaningful, yet underexplored area of the literature.

The primary aim of this study was to empirically examine the role of cognitive avoidance as the pathway through which anxiety relates to binge eating symptoms, consistent with the “escape from awareness” model [7]. This study is novel in that it was the first, to our knowledge, to examine the role of cognitive avoidance as a mediator of the relation between anxiety and binge eating symptoms for women and men. We hypothesized that (1) cognitive avoidance would be significantly positively related to binge eating symptoms, and (2) the relation between anxiety and binge eating symptoms would be mediated by cognitive avoidance. We also conducted analyses to determine whether the effect of cognitive avoidance would differ by gender in subsamples of men and women.

Methods

Participants and measures

Participants (N = 436; 62 % female) were recruited nation-wide across the United States, and ranged from 18 to 72 years old (M = 33.4, SD = 13.5). Individuals with psychosis (e.g., self-reported schizophrenia) were excluded. Responses from participants were excluded if they were determined to be invalid or random per validity screening questions.

Assessment included self-reported demographic variables and questionnaires pertaining to anxiety, cognitive avoidance, and binge eating. Anxiety was measured using the depression anxiety stress scales [21]. The DASS is a 21-item self-report questionnaire with a 7-item subscale measuring anxiety. The Likert-type scale is scored such that higher values indicate greater anxiety. Psychometrics for the anxiety subscale are strong [22], including test–retest reliability (r = 0.79 [21, 23]), concurrent validity with the Beck Anxiety Inventory (r = 0.84 [24]) and internal consistency (α = 0.82 in the current sample).

Cognitive avoidance was measured using the 15-item white bear suppression inventory (WBSI) [25]. Item-level scores range from 1 (strongly agree) to 5 (strongly disagree), with items including statements such as There are things I prefer not to think about. Good internal consistency (0.87–0.89) and test–retest reliability (0.69–0.92) have been shown in previous samples [25], and internal consistency (α = 0.95) was strong for the current sample.

Binge eating symptoms were measured using the 22-item eating disorder diagnostic scale (EDDS) [26]. This measure has demonstrated good psychometrics with both men and women [2628]. For this study, the symptom composite of the EDDS was used as it allows for dimensional interpretation of symptoms rather than dichotomous categorization of symptoms [29]. Higher symptom composite scores indicate greater pathology.

Test–retest reliability (r = 0.87), internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.89), and convergent validity have been established for the symptom composite [26]. Consistent with the previous literature [28], a binge eating-specific symptom composite was computed for this study. Internal consistency analyses from previous research [28] (α = 0.84) and the current sample consistency (α = 0.76) indicate adequate reliability for the binge eating-specific symptom composite.

Procedure

The Institutional Review Board at The University of Missouri–St. Louis approved this research. Because we were interested in examining binge eating across a spectrum of severity, a broad-based community sample was targeted. Recruitment occurred through postings to online community sites (e.g., “volunteers” section of Craigslist.org), not targeted toward overweight, obese or eating disordered populations. An example of one of our ads reads as follows: “Please consider visiting the site below to take part in an online study of eating patterns and feelings.” Questionnaires were administered through a secure website accessed by an encrypted link that allowed the data to be collected anonymously. An opportunity to be included in a raffle drawing for one of seven $100 gift cards was provided as incentive for providing data. Participants were asked to complete the questionnaire battery one time only.

Analytic plan

Data were analyzed using SPSS [30]. Participants who responded incorrectly to more than one random response pattern detection item were excluded from analyses. Data were also excluded from two individuals following a review of outliers due to the likelihood of an invalid response pattern. Mean imputation was performed for questionnaires that were at least 80 % complete.

Results

Descriptive analyses

Approximately 62 % of participants were women, 38 % were men, and three individuals (<1 % of sample) identified as transgendered. Over half (54 %) of the sample worked full-time or part-time, 20 % were full-time undergraduate or graduate students, and 25 % were unemployed, full-time homemakers, on disability, or retired. Participants’ age ranged from 18 to 72 years (M = 33.4, SD = 13.5). Race and ethnicity are as follows: 64.7 % White, 15.1 % African American/Black, 7.4 % Asian American, 6.5 % Hispanic/Latino, 4.9 % Biracial or Multiracial, 1.2 % American Indian/Alaska Native, 0.2 % Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander. Participants’ average BMI was 27.5 kg/m2 (SD = 7.7), and ranged from 16.46 to 68.41 kg/m2, based upon self-reported weight and height. Although the full sample included 436 participants, 261 participants provided analyzable data for all of the measures of interest.

Women had significantly higher anxiety (M = 8.79, SD = 8.56), and binge eating symptoms (M = 8.33, SD = 5.78), than men (M = 6.64, SD = 7.58; M = 7.02, SD = 5.59, respectively), although overall anxiety was in the mild range. We also found that cognitive avoidance was also significantly greater among women (t=-3.43, p < 0.01, M = 50.06, SD = 15.06) than men (M = 42.62, SD = 17.56). The cognitive avoidance reported in this sample was similar to levels reported among a normative comparison group of undergraduates (M men = 43.4, M women = 47.6, SD = 10.18) [25]. Approximately 6.8 % of participants who responded to the EDDS reported symptoms of binge eating that were consistent with DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria for BED, and approximately 6.5 % endorsed symptoms at a subclinical level.

Primary analyses

Cognitive avoidance was significantly positively correlated with binge eating symptoms (r = 0.30, p = 0.000) and anxiety (r = 0.46, p = 0.000), with correlation coefficients consistent with medium effect size. Mediation analyses were performed using Hayes’ Process Macro [31]. The overall model was significant F (1262) = 17.43, p < 0.001. Anxiety was significantly associated with cognitive avoidance (B = 1.82, SE = 0.23, p < 0.001). Cognitive avoidance was significantly associated with binge eating symptoms (B = 0.07, SE = 0.02, p < 0.01). The total effect of anxiety on binge eating symptoms was significant (B = 0.44, SE = 0.11, p < 0.001), and there was a significant direct effect of anxiety on binge eating (B = 0.32, SE = 0.12, p < 0.01). The indirect effect was tested using bootstrapping with 5000 resamples. There was a significant indirect effect via cognitive avoidance (B = 0.12, SE = 0.05, 95 % CI 0.03–0.22), with evidence of mediation from a significant Sobel test (test statistic = 2.24, p = 0.03). Cognitive avoidance partially mediated the relation of anxiety and binge eating symptoms.

A set of secondary analyses was conducted to contextualize and better understand the possibility of a gender effect on these results and study variables. The mediation analysis was performed separately for men and women to examine the possibility of a gender effect. For the subsample of men (n = 89), the overall model was significant F (1, 87) = 21.60, p < 0.001. Anxiety was significantly associated with cognitive avoidance (B = 2.14, SE = 0.46, p < 0.001); however, cognitive avoidance was not significantly associated with binge eating symptoms (B = −0.004, SE = 0.04, p = 0.92). The confidence interval for the indirect effect (B = −0.01, SE = 0.02, 95 % CI −0.03 to 0. 03) crossed zero, indicating that there was not a significant result for this analysis. There was no support for mediation for the subsample of men.

For the subsample of women (n = 172), a robust mediation effect was observed. The overall model was significant F(1, 170) = 34.88, p < 0.001. Anxiety was significantly associated with cognitive avoidance (B = 1.58, SE = 0.27, p < 0.001). Cognitive avoidance was significantly associated with binge eating symptoms (B = 0.11, SE = 0.03, p < 0.001). The total effect of anxiety on binge eating symptoms was significant (B = 0.30, SE = 0.13, p = 0.03); however, there was not a significant direct of anxiety on binge eating (B = 0.13, SE = 0.15, p = 0.37). The indirect effect was tested using bootstrapping with 5000 resamples. There was a significant indirect effect via cognitive avoidance (B = 0.16, SE = 0.06, 95 % CI 0.07–0.31), and a significant Sobel test (test statistic = 3.14, p = 0.002). These analyses supported full mediation.

Discussion

Our study evaluated whether avoidance of distressing thoughts mediated the relation between anxiety and binge eating symptoms, consistent with the escape from awareness model of binge eating [7]. Moreover, we aimed to evaluate whether the importance of cognitive avoidance, in relation to anxiety and binge eating symptoms, differs for men and women.

Consistent with previous empirical evidence that supports the conceptualization of binge eating as a method of coping with negative emotional states [3234]; our study found that for the sample overall, there was both a direct effect of anxiety and an indirect effect of cognitive avoidance, supporting an interpretation of partial mediation. The results from this study are novel, given that to date, many of the studies supporting this theory have extrapolated support for the role of avoidance in their examination of negative emotionality and binge eating behavior [10, 27, 35, 36]; direct tests of the association of cognitive avoidance and binge eating have been less common [37, 38].

Our finding supports a cognitive avoidance model of binge eating [7], and suggests that binge eating may be more likely to occur in as part of a maladaptive coping process when individuals are faced with distressing thoughts. Further, if the above-mentioned process is in fact occurring, this suggests that individuals may have difficulty appreciating the negative proximal and distal consequences of this behavior as they are focused on the perceived positive consequences of binge eating (i.e., temporary blunting of anxiety).

The current study furthers the understanding of the role of cognitive avoidance in the relation of anxiety and binge eating in the context of BED. Although the association between anxiety, cognitive avoidance, and binge eating has been previously studied, these evaluations have not occurred within the context of BED [39, 40]. Our data also extend these findings to a broader clinical context of individuals with subclinical binge eating disorder symptoms, and may be more generalizable than previous studies given that we utilized a diverse community-based sample.

This study examined the novel question of whether an individual’s gender impacted the relations between their emotional experiences and binge eating. Our results showed men and women differed in their degree of cognitive avoidance—and, for men and women, cognitive avoidance functioned differently in the anxiety–binge eating relationship. This study was also the first of which we are aware to explore gender effects to further the understanding of the relation of cognitive avoidance, anxiety, and binge eating behavior. We conducted separate analyses for men and women, given that women reported higher levels of cognitive avoidance than men, and found evidence that the role of cognitive avoidance differed by gender in relation to anxiety and binge eating. Specifically, cognitive avoidance completely mediated the relation between anxiety and binge eating for women, but was not significantly associated with binge eating for men. The results found in the overall sample should be interpreted in light of the differing follow-up analyses by gender.

There are several potential factors that may help to understand why cognitive avoidance was shown to be a mediator of the relation of anxiety and binge eating for women, but not for men. First, women may be more likely to experience a perceived need to push away uncomfortable thoughts due to a greater exposure to stressors, including gender-salient stressors such as sexual harassment, assault and discrimination. Similarly, men may have fewer anxious thoughts given the higher rates of anxiety disorders among women than men [41]. Second, women may be more likely to endorse cognitive avoidance although the true level of this construct may be similar across genders. Third, men may be more likely to engage in another form of experiential avoidance that we did not evaluate in the current study. For instance, they may have responded differently to items assessing avoidance of emotions rather than avoidance of thoughts. Finally, it may be possible that using an experimental technique to evaluate avoidance may have yielded different results than self-report, particularly given social norms emphasizing masculinity among men and the impact this may have on endorsement of psychological concerns [42].

There are some limitations of this study. The study design precludes causal conclusions among the study variables, and future work will be required to draw temporal conclusions among these constructs. These data were collected among a community-based sample and, therefore, may not fully generalize to clinical populations. The results found within this study warrant replication and extension in other samples, in particular, in patients with clinician-diagnosed disorders. Future research employing a longitudinal and experimental design may yield additional understanding of the relation among the factors we examined. As previously mentioned, our measures focused on one domain of psychological avoidance, specifically, cognitive avoidance. It may be possible that a broader assessment of experiential avoidance may yield different results.

While this study provides some empirical evidence to support the “escape from awareness” model, continued investigation of the process through which facets of negative affect trigger binge eating episodes is needed. Some have suggested that a dual pathway model, which posits negative affect and dietary restraint may serve as precipitants to binge eating episodes, may better explain the link between binge eating episodes and triggers such as body dissatisfaction [43]. One potential avenue to increase understanding of the moment-to-moment changes in affect and psychological processes that occur before, during, and after binge eating episodes is increased use of ecological momentary assessment and objective measurements collected through laboratory studies. While outside the scope of this national community-based study, future research utilizing these tools may provide compelling evidence to inform conceptual models of binge eating and potentially improve clinical interventions. Additionally, future study examining the differences between men and women regarding the function of binge eating and cognitive avoidance is needed.

This study has several clinical implications. Although we focused on binge eating symptoms through the lens of BED, the current study utilized dimensional measurement that included individuals at varying levels of clinical severity. As such, the results found in this study have relevance for newer clinical research perspectives that embrace the full range of functioning and behavior and minimize reliance on diagnostic cut points, such as transdiagnostic models and the NIMH Research Domain Criteria Initiative. Our finding that cognitive avoidance may be a more important factor for women than men suggests that interventions that utilize mindfulness and address avoidance of internal experiences, such as Dialectical Behavior Therapy and acceptance-based treatments, may be particularly useful for women with binge eating concerns. There is some empirical support for mindfulness as an effective strategy for reducing binge eating symptoms, as highlighted by studies that have found mindfulness training is associated with a decrease in binge eating episodes [4446]. Our data also suggest that mindfulness and acceptance-based intervention strategies may potentially have less utility for men, and men may have differential responses to other treatment modalities. Alternatively, it may be important to consider individualized treatment matching or tailoring for men and women with binge eating concerns depending on the importance of cognitive avoidance in their presentations.

In conclusion, this study supports the importance of cognitive avoidance as a mediating factor in the relation between anxiety and binge eating symptoms, particularly among women. Additional research is needed to further explore gender differences in conceptual models of binge eating, including the “escape from awareness” model [7] and whether these gender differences indicate different treatment approaches for men and women.