Introduction

Protein prenylation is an important post-translational modification that adds lipid molecules, isoprenoids, to target proteins and governs a variety of cellular signaling pathways [1]. In the nervous system, protein prenylation mediates neurite outgrowth, dendritic morphology, and synaptic plasticity, and has been implicated in the pathogenesis of neurodegenerative diseases [2,3,4]. The substrates of protein prenylation, isoprenoid diphosphates, are products of the mevalonate/cholesterol synthesis pathway, including farnesyl pyrophosphate (FPP) and geranylgeranyl pyrophosphate (GGPP) [5]. The farnesyl and geranylgeranyl groups from FPP and GGPP, respectively, are attached to a cysteine residue in the CAAX-box motifs at the C-terminus of target proteins via an irreversible covalent bond, catalyzed by farnesyltransferase (FT) and geranylgeranyltransferase type I (GGT-I, designated as GGT throughout the paper), respectively [6]. Depending on the X amino acid of the CAAX motif, proteins are preferably or exclusively prenylated by FT or GGT, or both [7]. Another type of geranylgeranylation occurs on a family of Rab proteins catalyzed by geranylgeranyltransferase type II (GGT-II or RabGGT) and its partner Rab escort protein (REP), dually appending geranylgeranyl moieties on two proximal cysteine residues near the C-terminus [8]. Recently, another geranylgeranyltransferase type III (GGT-III) was discovered with limited substrates [9, 10].

More than a hundred proteins undergo protein prenylation, including the largest and most well-studied group, small GTP binding proteins (GTPases) [6]. Small GTPases function as molecular switches in cells and control diverse cellular activities, including receptor signaling, dendritic spine morphogenesis, cell cycle regulation, and vesicle trafficking [11, 12]. In the nervous system, the importance of small GTPases, especially the Ras family, has been underscored in neurodevelopment, synaptic plasticity, and memory formation [13, 14]. Anchoring of small GTPases to membranes is required for their normal signaling functions and protein prenylation is necessary for this process [15]. The importance of protein prenylation is further highlighted by the findings that germline homozygous deletion of FT or GGT leads to embryonic lethality and dysregulation of prenylation is involved in the pathogenesis of several diseases including Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [16].

Previous work has shown that haplodeficiency of FT but not GGT rescues cognitive deficits in a transgenic mouse model of AD, suggesting that FT and GGT may have distinct roles in regulating neuronal functions [17]. A follow-up study showed that systemic GGT haplodeficiency or forebrain neuron-specific GGT deletion is detrimental, which leads to impaired learning and memory, reduced synaptic plasticity, and decreased dendritic spine density [18]. However, it remains unclear whether FT regulates synaptic functions and cognition differentially from GGT under normal physiological conditions. The current study was undertaken to determine the impact of FT deficiency on synaptic plasticity, dendritic spine density, and cognitive function using genetically modified mouse models. The results show that systemic/germline haplodeficiency of FT does not affect hippocampal synaptic plasticity, learning and memory formation, or cortical synaptic dendritic spine density. However, forebrain neuron-specific FT deletion leads to reduced magnitude of hippocampal long-term potentiation, impaired memory retention, and reduction of dendritic spine density in the hippocampus. To explore the potential molecular mechanisms underlying different roles of FT and GGT in the brain, a novel prenylomic approach was employed to uncover changes in downstream prenylated proteins. The results show that a number of prenylated proteins, including several small GTPases, are involved. These findings suggest that certain physiological levels of FT and GGT in neurons are required for normal synaptic function and cognition, and that the prenylation status of specific signaling molecules regulates neuronal function. This study presents an innovative approach to profile prenylated proteins and downstream signaling pathways in post-mortem tissue samples as well as providing novel insights into the physiological functions of FT in the brain.

Methods

Animals

Systemic/germline FT-haplodeficient (FT+/-) mice, GGT-haplodeficient (GGT+/-) mice, and forebrain neuron-specific GGT deficient (GGTf/fCre+) mice have been described previously [17,18,19,20]. The forebrain neuron-specific FT deficiency (FTf/fCre+) was achieved by crossing the FT floxed mice (FTf/f) with the mice expressing the Cre recombinase (Cre+) under the CaMKIIα promoter [19,20,21]. All these lines of mice are on a mixed genetic background of 129/OlaHsd and C57BL/6. Specifically, FT+/- and GGT+/- were backcrossed with C57BL/6J for two generations, whereas FTf/fCre+ and GGTf/fCre+ were generated by interbreeding of offspring from crossing FTf/f or GGTf/f (129/OlaHsd/C57BL/6) and Cre+ (congenic C57BL/6) mice. Therefore, littermate controls were used whenever possible to minimize potential effects of genetic backgrounds. The mice were genotyped using DNA extracted from ear biopsies and PCR with gene-specific primers. The age of the mice ranged 9–14 months, and both males and females were included in each experiment of this study. No statistically significant sex differences were observed; therefore, data from male and female mice were combined. Experiments were conducted blinded to genotypes of the mice and all animal procedures were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) at the University of Minnesota.

Electrophysiology

Electrophysiology experiments were conducted as previously described [18, 22]. Briefly, mice were deeply anesthetized using isoflurane, followed by decapitation. Brains were dissected out and sectioned in ice-cold sucrose cutting solution containing 2.5 mM KCl, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, 25 mM NaHCO3, 0.5 mM CaCl2, 7 mM MgCl2, 7 mM dextrose, and 240 mM sucrose. Transverse hippocampal slices were cut at 400 μm using a vibratome (Leica). Slices were then transferred and incubated for 15 min at 28 °C in artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) containing 125 mM NaCl, 2.5 mM KCl, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, 25 mM NaHCO3, 2 mM CaCl2, 1 mM MgCl2, and 25 mM dextrose (pH 7.4) with constant bubbling of 95%O2/5% CO2. Slices were then allowed to recover for at least 1 h in aCSF at room temperature. After recovery, slices were placed into the recording chamber (Automate Scientific, Berkeley) with aCSF flowing at approximately 1.5 ml/min at 27–29 °C. Electrical stimulations were delivered to the Schaffer Collateral axon bundles of CA3 to CA1 of the hippocampus using a bipolar tungsten electrode (FHC) and evoked field excitatory post-synaptic potentials (fEPSPs) were recorded from the stratum radiatum of CA1. Stimulation was delivered at 10 s interval and the stimulation intensity was adjusted to 40–50% of each slice’s maximum input/output curve using a constant-current stimulus isolator (World Precision Instruments, Sarasota).

Following a stable baseline (at least 20 min), long-term potentiation (LTP) was induced by three trains of theta-burst stimulation (TBS) with a 20-s interval and monitored for 40 min post-induction. The fEPSP slopes were normalized to the baseline for each slice and last 5 min of response were averaged to represent the magnitude of LTP. Input/output (I/O) curves and paired-pulse facilitation (PPF) were assessed using separate slices. I/O curves were generated by delivering five stimulations per intensity ranging from 0 up to 150 μA in 10-μA intervals. Stimulation intensity was then adjusted to 40–50% of each slice’s maximum response in the I/O curve to assess PPF. Two continuous stimuli were delivered to slices with an interval of 30 ms at every 10 s. Five minutes of stable PPFs (120 PPFs) were recorded and averaged for each genotype.

Golgi Staining and Dendritic Spine Density Quantification

Golgi staining was conducted using the FD Rapid GolgiStain™ kit (Cat. #: PK401A, FD NeuroTechnologies, Inc.) as previously described [18, 23, 24]. Briefly, mice were deeply anesthetized and perfused with PBS and brains were dissected and processed following the manufacturer’s user manual. Coronal brain sections (200 μm) were prepared using a vibratome (Leica). Bright-field microscopy was done under the 100× objective using the Nikon Eclipse 55i upright microscope to capture images of dendrites in the single-plane focal field using a CCD camera (QImaging) controlled by the Image-Pro plus software. Clearly identifiable dendritic branches from randomly chosen fully stained pyramidal neurons in the layer II/III of the posterior 2/3 of the cerebral cortex and in the hippocampal CA1 region were selected. For each mouse, fifteen neurons in each area were randomly imaged and five apical and five basal dendrites were chosen from tertiary or above dendritic segments from each neuron. Imaging and spine density quantifications were achieved using Image-Pro Plus software. On average, 19 ± 0.25 μm of dendritic segment length was quantified.

Behavioral Assessment

Three behavioral functions, including locomotive response to environmental stimulus, anxiety, and spatial learning and memory, were tested as previously described [17, 18]. The open-field test and the elevated plus maze test were employed to evaluate the locomotive response and anxiety of mice. The Morris water maze was employed to evaluate the spatial learning and memory of mice.

Briefly, all behavioral functions were assessed using setups and the ANYMAZE system from San Diego Instruments. For the open field test, mice were placed into a square open box to explore freely for 5 min daily for 3 days with their path lengths recorded. The elevated plus maze test was conducted next, in which mice were placed onto a plus shape maze with open and closed arms to explore freely for 5 min daily for 2 days. In the Morris water maze test, mice were put into a water basin to find a hidden escape platform 1 cm under the water in a visual cue-rich room. The escape latency was recorded for four trials a day for five continuous days. A probe trial was carried out on the sixth day, in which the hidden platform was removed and the number of times crossing the platform location was recorded in a single 60-s trial. After a 2-h rest, a visible trial was conducted to test for visual acuity and swimming speed.

In Vitro Prenylation and Fluorescence Labeling

Brain tissue collection followed protocols previously reported [17, 18]. Right forebrain (containing cortex and hippocampus) tissue samples were homogenized in the prenylation buffer (50 mM HEPES, 50 mM NaCl, 5 mM MgCl2, 10 μM ZnCl2, 5 mM DTT (GoldBio), 2 μM phenylmethane sulfonyl fluoride (PMSF, Sigma-Aldrich), 1.5% v/v protease inhibitor cocktail, 0.02% octylglucopyranoside (OGP, Sigma-Aldrich), pH 7.2) using a Bullet Blender® homogenizer (Next Advance, Averill Park, NY, USA). Tissue lysates were centrifuged at 1500×g in 4 °C for 15 min to clear cell debris, and further centrifuged at 100,000×g in 4 °C for 60 min to pull down cell membrane fractions. Proteins from the resulting supernatants (cytosolic fractions) were quantified using BCA assay (Thermo Scientific) and 2 mg in 1 mL solutions was prepared in the prenylation buffer. Rat FT or GGT (250 to 1500 nM) and C15AlkOPP (25 μM) were added and in vitro reactions were carried out for 4 h at 37 °C. Proteins were precipitated out through chloroform precipitation (1 mL chloroform, 4 mL methanol, and 3 mL water) and washed with methanol (3 mL) twice. Protein pellets were re-dissolved in PBS + 1% SDS and concentrations were determined using BCA. Protein lysates were diluted to 100 μg/μL for click reaction. Click reaction was carried out using TAMRA-N3 by adding TAMRA-PEG4-N3 (25 μM, BroadPharm), tris(2-carboxyethyl)phosphine (TCEP, 1 mM, Sigma-Aldrich), tris[(1-benzyl-1H-1,2,3-triazol-4-yl-)methylamine (TBTA, 100 μM, Sigma-Aldrich), and CuSO4 (1 mM, Sigma-Aldrich) at room temperature for 1 h.

After the click reaction, proteins were precipitated out using ProteoExtract® protein extraction kit (Millipore Sigma) following the manufacturer’s protocol. Protein pellets were dissolved in 1x sample loading buffer containing 2% SDS, 10% glycerol, 125 mM DTT, and 0.02% bromophenol blue in 50 mM Tris-HCl pH 6.8 and resolved in a 12% SDS-PAGE gel. TAMRA fluorescence (542 nm excitation/568 nm emission) was detected and visualized on Typhoon FLA 9500 (GE Healthcare). Gel images were processed on ImageJ for visualization by adjusting contrast and brightness.

Prenylated Protein Enrichment and Sample Preparation for Proteomics

Enrichment of prenylated proteins and sample preparation was reported previously [25, 26]. Briefly, in vitro prenylated proteins (2 mg in 1 mL) were subjected to click reaction with biotin-N3 by adding biotin-PEG4-N3, TCEP, TBTA, and CuSO4. Proteins were precipitated out and dissolved in 1x PBS + 1% SDS. Protein concentrations were normalized across all samples and incubated with pre-washed Neutravidin® beads for 1.5 h, followed by washing with PBS + 1%SDS, 1X PBS, 8 M urea, and 50 mM tetraethylammonium bicarbonate (TEAB). Beads were suspended in 50 mM TEAB and digested overnight at 37 °C with trypsin (Promega). Peptides were collected from beads using 0.5% formic acid and washings (twice) were pooled and dried. Samples were dissolved in 100 mM TEAB and spiked with yeast ADH1 standard (Waters) and TMT-labeled using TMT 6plex (Thermo Fisher Scientific). Labeled samples were pooled, dried, and dissolved in 200 mM NH4HCOO pH 10, and subsequently fractionated under high pH reversed-phase conditions. The samples were dried and dissolved in 0.1% formic acid in water.

LC-MS Data Acquisition

The TMT-labeled peptides were resolved in RSLC Ultimate 3000 nano-UHPLC (Dionex) at 300 nL/min in a reversed-phase column packed in-house [25]. Each fraction ran at varying gradients of solvent B (0.1% formic acid in acetonitrile) in solvent A (0.1% formic acid in water) with amounts ranging between 7 and 34% for 80 min and sprayed directly on Orbitrap Fusion Trihybrid (Thermo Fisher Scientific) using Nanospray Flex source (Thermo Fisher Scientific). For the multinotch SPS-MS3 approach [27], MS scans were collected at 120,000 resolution at 320–2000 m/z range with max injection time (IT) of 100 ms and automatic gain control (AGC) target of 200,000. To perform SPS-MS3, subsequent data-dependent (top speed at 3 s) MS/MS scans were acquired using collision-induced dissociation (CID) at normalized collision energy (NCE) of 35% with 1.3 m/z isolation window, max IT of 100 ms, and AGC target of 5000. Dynamic exclusion was allowed for 60 s. Acquisition at MS3 was done by synchronously selecting the top 10 precursors (SPS) for fragmentation by high-collisional energy dissociation (HCD) in the orbitrap with NCE of 55% and 2.5 m/z isolation window, 120 ms max IT, and 50,000 AGC target.

Proteomic Data Analysis

The raw files were searched using Andromeda embedded in MaxQuant (version 1.6.2.10) against the non-redundant mouse (UP0000000589) database from Uniprot (EMBL-EBI, April 2018 release). The peptides identified were based on full tryptic digestion with allowed 3 maximum missed cleavages. Fixed modifications were set for the TMT labels on both the N-terminal and lysine and variable modifications on methionine oxidation and N-term acetylation. The default settings in the software for other parameters were retained.

Statistical Analysis

GraphPad Prism 8 and Perseus (version 1.6.0.7) were used to perform statistical analyses. All values are expressed as mean ± SEM. To compare differences between two genotypes, a two-tailed unpaired Student’s t test was used. For comparisons of genotype effect on mice performance over constitutive days, two-way repeated measures ANOVA was conducted. Both males and females were included in this study and no sex difference was observed; thus, male and female data were combined for analysis. For prenylomic data, the .txt file containing the list of identified proteins was imported in Perseus (version 1.6.0.7) for statistical analysis. Proteins that were identified only by site, as potential contaminant, or reversed were removed. The reporter ion intensities were log2-transformed and proteins having 3 quantified values out of 6 across TMT channels were processed. Missing values in each channel were imputed based on normal distribution. Statistical analysis was performed using two-sample t tests with FDR = 0.01 and s0 = 0.5. Data were exported and processed on Microsoft Excel to generate and format volcano plots.

Results

Systemic FT Haplodeficiency Does Not Affect Hippocampal Synaptic Function, in Contrast to the Detrimental Effect of GGT Haplodeficiency

Hippocampal LTP of CA1 is important for shaping spatial learning and memory and defined as increased signal transmission in response to an intense stimulus [28,29,30,31]. To determine whether systemic FT haplodeficiency has any effect on synaptic function, we conducted electrophysiological experiments with hippocampal slices from FT+/- mice and WT littermates at 10 to 11 months of age. The axon bundles of the Schaffer collateral from CA3 to the CA1 were stimulated and recorded from the stratum radiatum of CA1. The results show that FT haplodeficiency has no effect on the magnitude of hippocampal LTP compared with their WT littermate controls (Fig. 1a, b). The basal synaptic transmission and short-term plasticity were also assessed in FT+/- mice by constructing the I/O curve and measuring the PPF ratio, respectively. The I/O curve shows increasing fEPSPs in response to increasing stimulus intensities. No statistical difference was observed between the I/O curves from FT+/- and WT brain slices (Fig. 1c), indicating comparable basal neuronal transmission in FT+/- and WT brain slices. PPF is a form of short-term presynaptic plasticity that is defined as an increased post-synaptic response to the second stimuli of two close successive stimuli. PPF is caused by increased presynaptic Ca2+ building up when the second stimulus arrives [32]. Similarly, there was no statistically significant difference in the PPF ratio of two stimuli at a 30-ms interval between brain slices from FT+/- and WT littermates (Fig. 1d).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Systemic FT haplodeficiency does not affect hippocampal LTP, basal neuronal transmission, and PPF. a Induction of LTP. Following a 20-min stable baseline, the LTP was induced by three trains of theta-burst stimulation (TBS) protocol with a 20-s interval. The slopes of the fEPSP rising phase were calculated and normalized to baseline in hippocampal slices from WT and FT+/- mice (n = 24–26 slices/9–10 mice per genotype, 10 months of age). b The magnitude of LTP of the last 5 min (35–40 min post-induction) was averaged for each genotype. c Input/output (I/O) curves. Five stimuli were given every 10 μA ranging from 0 to 150 μA and fEPSPs were recorded and averaged at each stimulation intensity from hippocampal slices of WT and FT+/- mice (n = 12–17 slices/5–6 mice per genotype, 11 months of age). d Paired-pulse facilitation (PPF) ratios. Two continuous stimuli with an interval of 30 ms were given to WT and FT+/- hippocampal slices at every 10 s and the ratios of the second to the first fEPSP response were plotted. Five minutes of PPF (120 ratios) were recorded and averaged for each genotype (n = 12–17 slices/5 mice per genotype, 11 months of age)

Previous work has demonstrated that systemic GGT haplodeficiency leads to a significant reduction in synaptic function, including impaired LTP [18]. To compare the effects of FT and GGT haplodeficiency on synaptic plasticity side-by-side, we conducted LTP experiments on hippocampal slices from GGT+/- mice along with FT+/- mice and their respective WT littermate controls. Consistent with the results previously reported, GGT haplodeficiency leads to reduced hippocampal LTP compared with their WT controls (Fig. 2a, b), confirming the detrimental effects of GGT haplodeficiency on synaptic function. These findings demonstrate that unlike GGT haplodeficiency, FT haplodeficiency has no effects on hippocampal synaptic transmission and plasticity.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Systemic GGT haplodeficiency impairs hippocampal LTP. a Induction of LTP. Following a 20-min stable baseline, the LTP was induced by three trains of TBS protocol with a 20-s interval. The slopes of the fEPSP rising phase were calculated and normalized to baseline in hippocampal slices from WT and GGT+/- mice (n = 8–10 slices/4 mice per genotype, 13 months of age). b The magnitude of LTP of the last 5 min (35–40 min post-induction) was averaged for each genotype. **p < 0.01, Student’s t test

FT Haplodeficiency Has No Effect on Dendritic Spine Density in Cortical Neurons but Modestly Reduces Dendritic Spine Density in Hippocampal Neurons

GGT haplodeficiency has been shown to lead to remarkably reduced dendritic spine density in the cortical layer II/III pyramidal neurons [18]. Since GGT and FT differently affect synaptic plasticity, these two enzymes and downstream pathways may also differently shape synaptic morphology. To evaluate synapse morphology in FT+/- mice, we performed Golgi staining and quantified the dendritic spine density in pyramidal neurons of the layer II/III of the cortex. No statistical difference in apical or basal dendritic spine counts was observed in FT+/- mice compared with their WT littermate controls (Fig. 3a, b). This result indicates that in contrast to GGT haplodeficiency, FT haplodeficiency does not have any negative effects on dendritic spine density in cortical neurons. Since electrophysiological properties of neurons were evaluated in CA1 of the hippocampus, the dendritic spine density in pyramidal neurons of the hippocampal CA1 region was also quantified. Unexpectedly, we observed a small (~ 10%) but statistically significant decrease in the spine density on both apical and basal CA1 dendrites in FT+/- mice compared to their WT littermate controls (Fig. 3c, d). These results suggest that pyramidal neurons in hippocampus and cortex have subtle differences in tolerance towards reduction of FT.

Fig. 3
figure 3

FT haplodeficiency has no effect on dendritic spine density in cortical neurons but modestly reduces dendritic spine density in hippocampal neurons. a Representative image of apical and basal dendritic segments of cortical layer II/III pyramidal neurons in Golgi-stained brain sections from FT+/- and WT mice. b Quantification of cortical spine densities (n = 30–44 neurons/2–3 mice per genotype, 10 months of age). c Representative images of apical and basal dendrites of CA1 pyramidal neurons in Golgi-stained brain sections from FT+/- and WT mice. d Quantification of hippocampal spine densities in FT+/- and WT mice (n = 42–45 neurons/3 mice per genotype). Student’s t test, *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01

FT Haplodeficiency Does Not Affect Spatial Learning and Memory Function

Hippocampus governs spatial learning and memory and previous work has shown that GGT haplodeficiency leads to impaired spatial learning and memory formation [18]. To determine the effects of FT haplodeficiency on cognitive function, FT+/- mice and WT littermates were subjected to the Morris water maze test. As shown in Fig. 4a and b, there were no differences in the learning curve during the acquisition phase or in the platform crossover during the probe trial between FT+/- and WT mice. This result indicates FT haplodeficiency does not affect spatial memory acquisition and retention. Notably, although there was a 10% reduction of dendritic spine density in the hippocampus of FT+/- mice compared to their WT controls, there were no changes in hippocampal LTP and learning and memory performance in FT+/- mice. These findings suggest that the modest effect of FT haplodeficiency on dendritic spine density in hippocampus is not sufficient to affect synaptic plasticity and cognitive function. Furthermore, FT haplodeficiency had no significant effects on exploratory activities except for an increased entry to the center zone in the open field test, suggesting a reduced anxiety level; however, there were no differences between FT+/- and WT mice in anxiety levels assessed by the elevated plus maze test (Supplementary Table S1).

Fig. 4
figure 4

FT haplodeficiency does not affect spatial learning and memory function. a Escape latencies during the acquisition phase of the Morris water maze test (n = 7–11 mice/genotype, 12 months of age). b Platform crossover in the probe trial (n = 7–11 mice/genotype, 12 months of age)

Forebrain Neuron-specific FT Deficiency Leads to Reduced Hippocampal LTP

Germline FT or GGT homozygous deletion leads to embryonic lethality, suggesting the importance of FT and GGT during development [20, 33]. To circumvent potential compensatory effects of germline FT haplodeficiency during early development and evaluate the impact of neuron-specific FT, we generated forebrain neuron-specific FT knockout (FTf/fCre+) mice by crossbreeding the FTf/f line [19] with the CAMKIIα-Cre line [21]. In these mice, Cre recombinase is expressed under the Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II alpha (CAMKIIα) promotor, which starts to become active late in development and drives Cre activity to increase as animals age [30, 34]. To assess the effects of neuron-specific FT deletion on synaptic function, we conducted the same sets of electrophysiological experiments on the hippocampus of these mice and their FTf/fCre− (WT) littermate controls at 9 to 10 months of age. Importantly, when we conducted hippocampal LTP experiments, FTf/fCre+ mice showed a significant decrease in the magnitude of LTP (Fig. 5a, b). Comparable I/O responses were observed in FTf/fCre+ mice and FTf/fCre− mice controls (Fig. 5c), indicating that forebrain neuron-specific FT deletion does not affect neuronal basal transmission. We next conducted PPF experiments to assess their short-term presynaptic plasticity. The PPF ratios showed a trend but not statistically significant decrease in FTf/fCre+ mice compared to their FTf/fCre− littermate controls (Fig. 5d). These results indicate that in contrast to systemic/germline FT haplodeficiency, forebrain neuronal-specific FT deletion impairs post-synaptic plasticity without affecting basal transmission and short-term presynaptic plasticity.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Forebrain neuron-specific FT deficiency leads to reduced hippocampal LTP with no significant effect on basal synaptic transmission and PPF. a Induction of LTP. Following a 20-min stable baseline, the LTP was induced by three trains of TBS protocol with a 20-s interval. The slopes of the fEPSP rising phase were calculated and normalized to baseline in hippocampal slices from FTf/fCre− and FTf/fCre+ mice (n = 14 slices/5 mice per genotype, 9 months of age). b The magnitude of LTP of the last 5 min (35–40 min post-induction) was averaged for each genotype. Student’s t test, **p < 0.01. c Input/output (I/O) curves. Five stimuli were given every 10 μA ranging from 0 to 150 μA and the fEPSPs were recorded and averaged at each stimulation intensity from hippocampal slices of FTf/fCre− and FTf/fCre+ mice (n = 16–20 slices/3 mice per genotype). d Paired-pulse facilitation (PPF) ratios. Two continuous stimuli with an interval of 30 ms were given to FTf/fCre− and FTf/fCre+ hippocampal slices at every 10 s and the ratios of the second to the first fEPSP response were plotted. Five minutes of PPF (120 ratios) were recorded and averaged for each genotype (n = 18–19 slices/3 mice per genotype, 10 months of age). Student’s t test, p = 0.051

Forebrain Neuron-specific FT Deficiency Does Not Affect Dendritic Spine Density in Cortical Neurons but Reduces Dendritic Spine Density in Hippocampal Neurons

Changes in synaptic plasticity raised the question whether FT deficiency affects neuronal structure, which may contribute to the role of FT in regulating synaptic function. We next conducted Golgi staining on brain sections of these mice and quantified the dendritic spine density in the pyramidal neurons of cortical layer II/III and hippocampus. Firstly, we found comparable cortical neuron spine density between FTf/fCre+ mice and FTf/fCre− mice (Fig. 6a, b), indicating that neuronal FT is not required for the cortex to maintain normal dendritic spine density. However, it is possible that neurons in different brain areas have different sensitivities to loss of FT. Since we evaluated electrophysiological properties in CA1 of the hippocampus, we next assessed dendritic spine density in CA1 of the hippocampus. As expected, spine density was significantly decreased on both apical (16%) and basal (13%) dendrites of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons from FTf/fCre+ mice compared to their FTf/fCre− littermate controls (Fig. 6c, d). Taken together, these results indicate that neuronal FT is required to retain normal spine density in the hippocampal CA1 area but not in the cortical area. This brain regional difference suggests that FT serves distinct roles in maintaining dendritic spine density in the cortex and hippocampus.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Forebrain neuron-specific FT deficiency does not affect dendritic spine density in cortical neurons but reduces dendritic spine density in hippocampal neurons. a Representative image of apical and basal dendrites of cortical pyramidal neurons in Golgi-stained brain sections from FTf/fCre− and FTf/fCre+ mice. b Quantification of cortical spine densities (n = 45 neurons/3 mice per genotype). c Representative images of apical and basal dendrites of CA1 pyramidal neurons in Golgi-stained brain sections from FTf/fCre+ and FTf/fCre− mice. d Quantification of hippocampal spine densities (n = 25–40 neurons/3 mice per genotype, 12.5 months of age). Student’s t test, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001

Forebrain Neuron-specific FT Deficiency Does Not Affect Learning Acquisition but Leads to Impaired Memory Retention in the Morris Water Maze

Hippocampal LTP is required for normal learning and memory [31] and an apparent reduction in LTP and hippocampal spine density predicts potential learning and memory dysfunction in FTf/fCre+ mice. Therefore, we next conducted a battery of behavioral tests including the Morris water maze test to evaluate their cognitive functions. As shown in Fig. 7, FTf/fCre+ mice had a similar learning curve as their FTf/fCre− controls. However, in the probe trial, FTf/fCre+ mice displayed significantly fewer platform crossovers compared to FTf/fCre− mice. This result suggests that forebrain neuron-specific FT is required for precise memory retention but not for normal learning acquisition. No visual acuity or swimming speed difference was observed between FTf/fCre+ and FTf/fCre− mice. In addition, FTf/fCre+ mice showed similar exploratory activities and anxiety levels as FTf/fCre− mice in the open field test and the elevated plus maze test, respectively (Supplementary Table S2).

Fig. 7
figure 7

Forebrain neuron-specific FT deficiency does not affect learning acquisition but leads to impaired memory retention in the Morris water maze. a Escape latencies during the acquisition phase of the Morris water maze test (n = 11–12 mice/genotype, 9 months of age). b Platform crossover in the probe trial (n = 11–12 mice/genotype, 9 months of age). Student’s t test, *p < 0.05

Novel Prenylomic Analysis of Post-mortem Brain Tissue Reveals Distinct Unprenylated Protein Pools in Forebrain Neuron-specific FT and GGT Deficient Mice

It has been shown previously that forebrain neuron-specific GGT deletion leads to a reduced magnitude of hippocampal LTP and a reduced dendritic spine density in cortical neurons [18]. Here we showed that forebrain neuron-specific FT deletion also leads to a reduced magnitude of hippocampal LTP and a reduced dendritic spine density in hippocampal neurons but not in cortical neurons. In addition, neuron-specific FT deficiency only impairs memory retention but not learning acquisition. The difference between FT and GGT in regulating neuronal functions, morphology, and cognitive functions indicates that unprenylated protein pools might be differently affected by FT or GGT deletion. To further investigate this idea, we developed a novel prenylomic approach to profile downstream prenylated proteins in post-mortem brain tissues from FTf/fCre+ and GGTf/fCre+ mice in comparison with their respective Cre− controls. We extracted proteins from forebrain tissue lysates, which were then subjected to in vitro prenylation with an alkyne-modified isoprenoid probe analog at different concentrations of recombinant FT or GGT enzymes [35]. This single isoprenoid analogue can be efficiently incorporated by both FT and GGT in lieu of FPP or GGPP as shown in vitro and in metabolic labeling experiments [26, 36]. Subsequent “click” reaction with an azide-modified fluorophore or biotin enables visualization or enrichment, respectively, of proteins labeled by the probe.

Forebrain neuron-specific FT or GGT deletion/knockout is expected to give rise to enlarged unprenylated protein pools compared to their Cre− controls. Supplementing brain lysates with the isoprenoid probe and exogenous FT or GGT allows the incorporation of the probe into unprenylated proteins for further identification. Since Cre− controls have endogenous FT or GGT, their unprenylated protein pools should be smaller and have reduced in vitro labeling. To assess the levels of unprenylated proteins in the brain lysates, we firstly conducted fluorescence labeling using TAMRA-N3 with the knockouts and their corresponding controls. Labeled lysates were resolved by SDS-PAGE and TAMRA fluorescence was detected (Fig. 8a and b). Although background labeling was apparent in both knockouts and their corresponding controls across all concentrations of the enzyme tested, FTf/fCre+ and GGTf/fCre+ displayed differences in the banding pattern compared with their controls. For FTf/fCre+, an intense band was observed near 50 kDa region, and bands near the 20 and 25 kDa regions were present, which were not detected in the FTf/fCre− control (Fig. 8a). On the other hand, minor differences were observed between GGTf/fCre+ and GGTf/fCre−, manifested by the extra bands observed above and below the 20 kDa region in GGTf/fCre+ (Fig. 8b). These data suggest that the deletion of prenyltransferases results in enlarged pools of unprenylated proteins.

Fig. 8
figure 8

Novel prenylomic analysis reveals distinct unprenylated protein pools in forebrain neuron-specific FT and GGT deficient mice. In-gel fluorescence analysis of protein lysates labeled with C15AlkOPP probe from FTf/fCre+ and FTf/fCre− (a) or GGTf/fCre+ and GGTf/fCre− (b) at varying concentrations of supplemented FT and GGT, respectively. Arrows indicate bands observed in Cre+ but not in Cre−, indicating in vitro probe incorporation to residual unprenylated proteins from Cre+ tissues. Volcano plots (FDR = 0.01, s0 = 0.5) generated from prenylomic analysis of labeled and enriched prenylated proteins from FTf/fCre+ and FTf/fCre− (c) or GGTf/fCre+ and GGTf/fCre− (d). Fold-changes in the x-axis reflect abundance of proteins from Cre+ against their Cre− after enrichment. Known farnesylated (Far, red circles) and geranylgeranylated (GG, blue circles) proteins known to be substrates of FT and GGT are noted (n = 3 mice/genotype, 14 months of age)

After successfully labeling natively unprenylated proteins in the brain lysates with our probe, we next investigated the molecular identities of these proteins. Proteins from the knockouts and their controls that were in vitro prenylated with the isoprenoid probe in the presence of exogenous FT or GGT were subjected to “click” reaction with biotin-N3. Since there were no significant differences in labeling observed across the enzyme concentrations tested, we selected 250 nM FT or 500 nM GGT as optimal enzyme concentrations for in vitro prenylation. Biotinylated lysates were subjected to enrichment using avidin beads and the isolated proteins were digested into peptides and prepared for proteomic analysis. A multinotch SPS-MS3 approach was employed in the LC-MS analysis to accurately quantify the extent of enrichment of the prenylated proteins identified [27]. A volcano plot (FDR = 0.01, s0 = 0.5) was generated to visualize enriched proteins from FTf/fCre+ (Fig. 8c) or GGTf/fCre+ (Fig. 8d) versus their corresponding Cre− controls. Eleven and five statistically enriched known farnesylated and geranylgeranylated proteins, including several well-known small GTPases, were identified from the FTf/fCre+ and GGTf/fCre+ samples, respectively. The identities and functions of those proteins are summarized in Table 1 and Table 2. Other known prenylated proteins such as Uchl1, DiRas, Cnp, and Cdc42 were identified but were not significantly enriched (Supplementary Table S3 and S4). Most of the enriched farnesylated proteins in FTf/fCre+ have molecular weights close to 50 and 20 kDa while those from GGTf/fCre+ have 20 kDa or lower, corroborating the gel-based labeling observed in Fig. 8a, b. Moreover, the most enriched farnesylated proteins in FTf/fCre+ have molecular weights close to 50 kDa, further validating the intense labeling clearly seen in the 50 kDa region in Fig. 8a. These proteins perhaps represent the most significantly affected prenylated proteins detectable on this LC-MS-based proteomic approach upon FT or GGT deletion. As shown in the volcano plot, several proteins have been identified and there is little overlap between FTf/fCre+ and GGTf/fCre+ enriched unprenylated protein pools. These results indicate that forebrain neuron-specific FT and GGT act on different downstream prenylated protein pools, which may explain functional differences observed in these mice.

Table 1 Functions of the enriched prenylated proteins in the brain of FTf/fCre+ mice
Table 2 Functions of the enriched prenylated proteins in the brain of GGTf/fCre+ mice

Discussion

In this study, the impact of systemic/germline haplodeficiency and forebrain neuron-specific deficiency of FT on synaptic and cognitive function has been examined. We found that FT haplodeficiency had minimal to no effects on synaptic plasticity, dendritic spine density, and learning and memory in the Morris water maze. These results are in stark contrast with those from GGT-haplodeficient mice, in which both synaptic/cognitive function and spine structure are impaired, as shown previously [18] and in the present study. These findings indicate that the physiological level of GGT is more critical than that of FT for normal brain function and suggest that farnesylated and geranylgeranylated proteins play distinct roles in shaping neuronal and cognitive function. We further showed that forebrain neuron-specific FT deficiency led to impaired hippocampal LTP, similar to the effect of forebrain neuron-specific GGT deficiency indicating that certain levels of FT and GGT in neurons are required for normal synaptic function. In addition, forebrain neuron-specific FT deficiency reduced dendritic spine density in CA1 but not in cortex, and it impaired memory retention but not learning acquisition. These results indicate that reduction of neuronal FT exerts differential effects on different areas of the brain and on different aspects of cognitive function. Alternatively, actual FT levels could be differently affected in the cortical and CA1 pyramidal neurons in FTf/fCre+ mice. Despite this possibility, the likelihood of this scenario is small, as CaMKIIα is highly expressed similarly in the pyramidal neurons of cortical layer II/III and hippocampus CA1 [74]. In addition, CaMKIIα-Cre mice have been well characterized and Cre recombinase-mediated gene deletion in the cortex and hippocampus follows the pattern of CaMKIIα expression in adult brains and displays age-dependent activity [34, 75, 76]. Therefore, FT levels are expected to reduce similarly in cortical and hippocampal neurons.

Further investigation is required to gain more insights into the molecular mechanisms underlying the region- and function-specific effects. Interestingly, although both FT-haplodeficient and forebrain neuron-specific FT deficient mice displayed reduced dendritic spine density in CA1, hippocampal LTP and memory retention were only impaired in neuron-specific FT deficient mice. It is possible that a 10% reduction of dendritic spine density in FT-haplodeficient mice was not sufficient to cause measurable functional consequence although 13–16% reduction in neuron-specific FT knockout was detrimental. It is also possible that hippocampal neurons in systemic/germline haplodeficient FT mice have developed some compensatory mechanisms to maintain synaptic plasticity and cognitive function. Additionally, it is worth noting that all cell types besides neurons have partial loss of FT in FT+/- mice, whereas FT deletion was specific to CaMKIIα expressing forebrain excitatory neurons in the FTf/fCre+ mice. Therefore, the difference in cell types affected by FT reduction and the level of FT reduction may both contribute to differential functional outcomes observed in FT+/- mice and FTf/fCre+ mice. Future studies employing other conditional cell type–specific FT deletion strategies should provide more insights into the gene dosage effect of FT and the role of FT in other brain cell types.

Functions of FT and GGT in the nervous system have been explored using pharmacological approaches, which are prone to off-target effects and limitations of not separating FT and GGT pathways [2, 77,78,79,80]. To parse out roles of FT and GGT in the brain, we utilized genetic approaches by utilizing FT and GGT haplodeficient and forebrain neuron-specific knockout mice, respectively, to better understand the role of these enzymes in shaping neuronal functions and regulating cognition. Intriguingly, GGT haplodeficiency but not FT haplodeficiency leads to a reduced magnitude of hippocampal LTP, whereas both forebrain neuron-specific GGT and FT knockouts cause hippocampal LTP impairment. These data indicate that neurons may have different levels of tolerance to the suppression of FT compared to that of GGT under the condition that the same levels of FT and GGT are reduced in neurons in these mice. Since a pool of proteins can be prenylated by both GGT and FT, it is possible that in the absence of one prenylation enzyme, proteins can be processed via the other prenylation pathway [81]. The differential effects were likely caused by the suppression of proteins that are exclusively prenylated by one enzyme. In addition, enzyme kinetics studies have shown that FT manifests higher catalytic efficiency than GGT [82, 83]; thus, it is possible that stronger suppression of FT is needed to affect signaling cascades of the farnesylated protein pool. Notably, we have shown that the levels of FT and GGT are reduced similarly in the brain of FT+/- and GGT+/- mice, and FT haplodeficiency, but not GGT haplodeficiency, is sufficient to rescue cognitive function as well as AD pathology in a mouse model of AD [17]. These results suggest that FT is dysregulated in the presence of AD pathology and reducing FT levels to the normal range but not to detrimental levels in the brain ameliorates AD pathology. Future studies are warranted to investigate the dysregulation of FT and its downstream signaling cascades that contribute to AD pathogenesis, which may lead to new treatment avenues.

To investigate prenylated protein pools and signaling cascades affected by forebrain neuron-specific FT or GGT knockout, we developed a novel prenylomic approach—an in vitro prenylation assay for use with post-mortem tissue samples. To the best of our knowledge, no prenylomic analysis on post-mortem brain tissue has ever been reported prior to this study. By extracting proteins from brain lysates and subjecting them to in vitro labeling and mass spectrometric proteomics analysis, it was possible to identify a pool of unprenylated proteins in forebrain neuron-specific FT or GGT knockouts. There was little overlap between unprenylated protein pools from these two groups of mice, consistent with the substrate specificities of FT and GGT. The results also suggest that different downstream signaling pathways may be affected by the loss of FT or GGT in neurons. Previous efforts employing in vitro prenylation of FT, GGT, and GGT-II substrates used cellular models and relied on the use of a biotinylated analogue of the isoprenoid (BGPP) [84]. Such a method works well for detecting GGT-II substrates but not for proteins prenylated by FT and GGT [85]. While that method, which employs a biotinylated analogue, circumvents the need for the subsequent derivatization step employed here (after enzymatic labeling), engineered mutants of FT and GGT were required to efficiently incorporate the bulky BGPP; the effects of those mutations on enzyme specificity remain unclear. In addition, in those studies, Western blot analysis for the biotin tag on FT and GGT substrates was used to provide qualitative assessment of in vitro labeling efficiency in cell lysates, and the molecular identities of the FT or GGT substrates were not pursued. Therefore, the prenylomic analysis reported here is the first investigation of profiling FT and GGT substrates in brain tissue facilitated by genetic deletion of these prenylation enzymes in mice [84, 86].

More than 100 proteins are known to undergo the three classes of protein prenylation [3] but a recent study reported 32 known and novel farnesylated proteins and 19 GGT substrates in a human endothelial cell line via probe metabolic labeling [81]. In the present study, it was found that subsets of these proteins (11 farnesylated and 5 geranylgeranylated) were statistically enriched as unprenylated in forebrain neuron-specific FT or GGT knockout mice. As summarized in Tables 1 and 2, some of those proteins play important roles pertinent to neuronal functions, which may contribute to the structural and functional differences observed between neuron-specific FT or GGT knockout mice and their controls. For instance, Rheb, an exclusively farnesylated small GTPase, is significantly affected by forebrain neuron-specific FT deletion. Rheb has shown to regulate neuronal morphology, synaptic plasticity, and memory functions [56,57,58, 87, 88]. It is likely that abrogation of Rheb prenylation results in reduced Rheb signaling, which may have contributed to reduced synaptic and cognitive function observed in FTf/fCre+ mice. In addition, Rac1 and RhoA are major geranylgeranylated proteins and their signaling cascades are important for shaping the neuronal cytoskeleton that determines neuronal morphology and functions [89]. Importantly, using both genetic and pharmacological approaches, recent studies have clearly demonstrated the critical roles of RhoA signaling in regulating dendritic spine structure and function in the brain [60,61,62]. Perturbation of signaling pathways mediated by these small GTPases via reduction in prenylation could contribute to structural and functional deficits in GGT knockout mice. Future studies are needed to dissect out the underlying molecular pathways of FT and GGT that regulate neuronal functions.

In this prenylomic analysis, other known prenylated proteins that did not reach statistical significance for enrichment were also identified. In particular, Uchl1, a membrane-associated protein that has been implicated in Parkinson’s disease was identified [90]. Interestingly, while a previous study supported the farnesylation of Uchl1 using mutational experiments or treatment with a farnesyltransferase inhibitor, a more recent study suggests that its C-terminal CAAX-box is not farnesylated in vitro [81]. In the current study, Uchl1 may have not been enriched due to its inability to be farnesylated in vitro or it is equally farnesylated in both FTf/fCre+ and FTf/fCre− mice. Further investigations are required to elucidate the farnesylation status of Uchl1 and its role in health and disease.

Compared to the metabolic labeling approach, which leverages the cellular machinery for probe incorporation and allows the tagging and enrichment of multiple proteins in a single experiment [25, 26, 91], the in vitro labeling used in the present study relies on the integrity of the unprenylated and soluble forms of the protein substrates in the lysate. In particular, since brain tissues with only neuron-specific FT or GGT knockouts were used, the prenylated form of proteins from other cell types may suppress the apparent abundance of the unprenylated form in soluble lysate. This limits the ability to enrich those unprenylated proteins with inherently low abundance in the lysate. Gene expression of proteins is also controlled with some having more stable expression than others [92]. The prenylation substrates detected here that remained unprenylated may have persisted at sufficient levels under steady-state conditions post-mortem compared to others. Furthermore, a significant fraction of GGT substrates belongs to the family of small GTPases, which are prenylated as complexes with chaperone inside cells [93]. While it was possible to detect some of these small GTPases using the in vitro approach, others might require these chaperone-complexed forms to be efficiently prenylated in vitro for detection. It is important to note that despite its limitations, the in vitro prenylomic approach reported here identifies differentially prenylated proteins in the brain, reflecting the effect of neuronal FT and GGT deletion in vivo. Here, we describe a tool that enables studying protein prenylation in post-mortem tissues.

In summary, the present study provides novel insights into the role of systemic and neuronal FT in regulating synaptic plasticity and cognitive function. It also offers an innovative approach for future studies to unravel physiological and pathological roles of prenylated proteins.