Introduction

Behavioural responses often occur rapidly after exposure to environmental pollutants and represent a sensitive indicator of the influence of pollutants on non target organisms, this being the basis for the rather new branch of behavioural ecotoxicology (Gerhardt 2007). Moreover, pollutant-induced alterations in behaviour affect not only individuals, but also the viability of populations and consequential the structure of ecosystems (Dell`Omo 2002). Up to now, the majority of ecotoxicological studies have focused on the direct effect of pollutants on organisms; although indirect effects, such as an impairment of inter- and intraspecific interactions, are also likely consequences of an exposure event. An ecologically important example of interactions in this context is the predator–prey relationship, as food web interactions influence not only the structure of the populations of both directly involved species but might also affect ecosystem functioning (Townsend et al. 2003). According to the optimal foraging theory (Begon et al. 2006), predators exert specific behaviours to detect (searching behaviour) and hunt (handling behaviour) prey organisms, whereas the prey develop predator avoidance behaviours in co-evolution.

Since the predator prey relationship represents an important biotic interaction it therefore may be susceptible to pollutant exposure. To date, studies of predator–prey interactions in aquatic ecosystems have concentrated mainly on either the predator or the prey. This topic has been addressed in a number of studies up to now, with most investigations aiming at the prey (e.g. Baker and Ball 1995; Brown 2003; Goyke and Hershey 1992; Hershey 1987; Hölker and Stief 2005; Macchiusi and Baker 1992; Schulz and Dabrowski 2001; Sih 1982; Tseng 2003). Only a few studies are available which focus on the predator (e.g. Hamers and Krogh 1997; Power 1990). Grippo and Heath (2003) detected the effects of mercury on the foraging efficiency and capture speed of fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas) exposed to 13 and 57 μg/l HgCl2. The prey capture rate of mummichogs (Fundulus heteroclitus) in the laboratory was closely related to the diet of the fish in the field, thus representing a biomarker with high ecological relevance. However, due to great variability at the different test sites it was not especially sensitive (Weis et al. 2001).

However, as predator and prey live in the same biocoenosis it is quite likely, that both groups of organisms will be affected by pollution either directly or indirectly. As proposed by Lima (2002), important conclusions about ecological consequences can only be drawn if predators and prey are regarded both equally in the investigation of predator–prey interactions. This approach has been applied in only a few field and laboratory studies with aquatic invertebrates, amphibians and fish (Bridges 1999; Gómez et al. 1997, Rahel and Stein 1988; Taylor et al. 1995; Thorp and Bergey 1981).

In our study we included both, predator (Danio rerio) and prey (Chironomus riparius), to investigate the indirect effect of a pollution scenario (pulse exposure of chlorpyrifos) on the predator prey relationship. To differentiate between effects on predator or prey we used 3 different exposure scenarios: Predator exposed, prey exposed and both predator and prey exposed. A similar exposure design was conducted by Gómez et al. (1997), using different rotifer species. An important difference to this setup is that in our experiment the prey organisms had the opportunity to avoid the predators via burrowing, providing a higher ecological relevance.

As “model” predator we chose the zebrafish (Danio rerio), which naturally occurs in stream habitats rich in macrophytes in South East Asia (Börries 2006). Ecologically, fish represent a very important group of secondary consumers or even top predators.

Our “model” prey organisms were 4th instar larvae of the non-biting midge Chironomus riparius. This organism was chosen because of its ecological importance as food item for fish (Pinder 1986). Chironomids have been used as prey objects for D. rerio in many studies and husbandry instructions (Béchard et al. 2008; Lawrence 2007; Nyholm et al. 2008). Additionally as sediment-dwelling organisms, they are particularly susceptible to sediment bound pollutants. There is evidence that chironomids are reacting actively to the presence of predators. For example, studies showed that larvae of C. riparius burrowed significantly deeper when exposed to fish kairomones, simulating increasing predator density by Rutilus rutilus (Hölker and Stief 2005). A predatory damselfly, showing visual orientation like fish, fed mostly on chironomids which spent more time out of the tube, i.e. on the sediment surface (Hershey 1987).

As a “model” for an environmentally relevant pollutant we chose chlorpyrifos (CHP), a broad-spectrum organophosphorus insecticide (Richardson 1995). It is one of the most common active compounds in pest control products worldwide (Dow AgroSciences 2010) and is applied in high amounts to agricultural areas of corn, cotton, apples and other orchard crops (Gilliom et al. 2006). In 1990, approx. 1.4 million pounds of this insecticide were applied in the Central Valley of California (Sheipline 1993). In urban streams in the United States, chlorpyrifos concentration exceeded water quality benchmarks in 37% of the sites (2nd highest exceedance rate after diazinon) and in 21% of the sites in agricultural streams (highest exceedance rate) during 1992–2001 (Gilliom et al. 2006). Environmental concentrations of 0.19–0.3 μg/l were detected in urban waterways in California and in several surface waters in the USA (Bailey et al. 2000; Gilliom et al. 2006).

Studies so far mainly investigated the effects of chlorpyrifos alone and in mixtures on the acute toxicity to Chironomus tentans and on early-life stage toxicity to zebrafish with different parameters. These included abnormal swimming movements and mortality, among others. However, studies regarding predator–prey interactions with this widely used insecticide are lacking for both invertebrates and for fish.

In the present study the following hypotheses were tested:

  1. 1.

    Exposed chironomids burrow less than control animals, and are therefore more susceptible to predation by fish.

  2. 2.

    Predation by fish stimulates increased burrowing behaviour in exposed as well as control chironomids.

  3. 3.

    When exposing predator and prey, the decreased success of the predator to prey and of the prey to burrow are outweighed, resulting in insignificant differences in feeding rate compared to the control.

Materials and methods

In the following experiment zebrafish Danio rerio were used as predators, and larvae of the non biting midge, Chironomus riparius as the prey. The animal maintenance and the experiments were conducted in an acclimatized chamber at 25 ± 0.5°C.

Animal culture and maintenance

Chironomus riparius

Egg ropes of C. riparius have been collected from a breeding stock at the University of Tübingen, and kept at 21 ± 0.5°C. After hatching, chironomids in the first larval stage (L1) were reared in plastic containers containing dechlorinated tap water and a two centimetre thick layer of quartz sand (particle size 0.1–0.3 mm, burned for 3 h at 500°C; Dehner, Germany) under constant aeration. Every day the chironomid larvae of each stock vessel were fed ad libitum with fine powderized ground fish flakes (50% Tetramin, 50% Tetraphyll, Tetra, Germany). Dechlorinated tap water was exchanged once a week. For acclimation to the final test conditions, C. riparius larvae (L1) were kept in a climate chamber at 25 ± 0.5°C for ten days until they reached the L4 stage. After 10 days larvae reached the L4 stage and were used for the predator–prey experiment.

Danio rerio

The 4–6 month old Danio rerio (total length: 27.93 ± 3.95 mm) used in our experiments were partly the offspring of wild-type zebrafish from the strain WIK (ZFIN ID: ZDB-GENO-010531-2) and wild-type zebrafish from the strain Tue.G14 (generously provided by the Max-Planck-Institute for Developmental Biology in Tübingen). The fish were kept in aerated and filtered aquaria with a minimum of 1 litre of water per fish. Culture conditions were 25 ± 0.5°C at a 12:12 h light:dark cycle. The adult fish were fed twice per day with dry flake food and frozen small crustaceans, Tubifex or midge larvae, respectively. Fish had up to 1 month time for acclimatisation to the new environment. To become acquainted with the prey objects, D. rerio was fed during that time with living C. riparius larvae several times before the start of the experiment.

Test substance

Chlorpyrifos (Pestanal, analytical standard, Sigma-Aldrich, Germany) was dissolved in reconstituted water (OECD 1992). In order to prepare a stock solution it was constantly stirred for a minimum of 4 h at a water temperature of about 45°C and a pH of 8.0. Subsequently, the solution was kept at 35°C overnight until use with constant stirring. From this stock test solutions were prepared directly before use with dechlorinated tap water. In order to simulate possible pulse pollution concentrations, nominal test concentrations for exposure experiments were 1 and 6 μg CHP/l. The retrieval rate for chlorpyrifos in an earlier study with the same experimental setup for stock solution preparation was 51.8 % in analytical measurements (Kienle et al. 2009).

Experimental design

Preliminary tests

First, the burrowing behaviour of C. riparius in the L4 stage has been observed (unpublished data) as follows: The natural burrowing behaviour of C. riparius L4 larvae was assessed in three replicate treatments with 50 chironomids each. The numbers of totally visible and partly visible larvae were counted manually every 20 min. Due to those experiments, a two hour period was determined as the adequate time for healthy C. riparius to dig entirely into the sediment and to show natural behaviour. Second, the recapture rate of 100 C. riparius L4 larvae burrowed in quartz sediment was observed (replicated nine times). The recapture rate was 97.2%. Third, the feeding rate of D. rerio with 100 introduced C. riparius larvae was determined. After 2 h, the average number of surviving chironomids was between 50 and 60 individuals. With this medial number of surviving chironomids in the control treatment it is possible to detect both, an increase or decrease in feeding rate. Therefore in the main experiment we chose 100 chironomids as an adequate number for the predator–prey experiments.

Main experiments

In this study, three different treatments and one negative control were investigated:

  1. 1.

    Predator pre-exposed (Dc D. rerio contaminated)

  2. 2.

    Prey pre-exposed (Cc C. riparius contaminated)

  3. 3.

    Predator and prey pre-exposed (Bc Both (fish and chironomids) contaminated)

All treatments were replicated three times. In every replicate five D. rerio as predators and 100 C. riparius as prey were introduced. A total of 100 chironomids per replicate were collected randomly and transferred for exposure into large glass dishes (15 cm diameter, depth 8 cm) containing 50 g of quartz sediment and the corresponding chlorpyrifos solution made from dechlorinated tap water. After 2 h, the chironomids were transferred into 10 l aquaria containing 400 g quartz sediment (corresponding to a 1–2 cm thick layer) and 8 l of a mixture of dechlorinated tap water and distilled water to obtain a conductivity of 400–450 μs/cm, optimal for D. rerio cultivation. During the transfer of the test organisms into the feeding aquaria special attention was paid to ensure that neither contaminated sediment nor water were transferred. In the 2 h following the transfer the chironomids had the opportunity to burrow into the sediment.

Meanwhile, five D. rerio per replicate were transferred into 4 l aquaria containing 3 l of the respective chlorpyrifos solution or control water. All aquaria were wrapped with a black cover to avoid disturbances from human presence. The fish were exposed for 2 h to the CHP contaminant.

Before the transfer of D. rerio into the 10 l aquaria with the C. riparius larvae, the numbers of chironomids completely visible at the surface and those partly visible were counted. After the transfer, the fish had 2 h to forage and feed on C. riparius.

After these 2 h periods, the number of chironomids completely at the surface and those partly visible was re-counted. Then, the fish were removed and anaesthetised with benzocain (3–5 ml of 40 mg Bencocain/ml Aceton). The total length of each fish was measured with a sliding calliper (powerfix EMC, model number Z11155, resolution 0.01 mm). Subsequently, surviving chironomids were searched and counted in the 10 l aquaria and the sediment.

Data analysis

Nonparametric methods were chosen for the analysis because the data were only partially normally distributed (Shapiro–Wilk Test, JMP 4.0, SAS systems, USA). The data from all tests were analysed for significance using Friedman’s ANOVA (Statistica 5.0, StatSoft, USA), followed by a Wilcoxon two group test (equivalent to Mann–Whitney test, JMP 4.0, SAS systems, USA) to examine differences between control and exposure treatments.

Results

The average total length of the zebrafish was 27.93 ± 3.95 mm (see Table 1). There were no significant size differences between the various treatments (Friedman`s Anova n.s.). At a nominal concentration of 1 μg/l of CHP no significant difference between the treatments was observed (Fig. 1). No changes in the numbers of burrowed chironomids, of chironomids partly at the surface and of chironomids remaining at the sediment surface occurred neither before the introduction of the fish into the feeding aquaria nor after the removal of the fish (Friedman`s Anova n.s., respectively; data not shown). Also no significant difference was found in the feeding rate of D. rerio preying on C. riparius exposed to 1 μg/l CHP in neither of the treatments (Fig. 1).

Table 1 Total length of D. rerio (mm) (mean ± SD)
Fig. 1
figure 1

Feeding rate [%] of D. rerio on larval chironomids after 2 h. Fish and/or chironomids were exposed to 1 μg/l CHP for 2 h prior to the feeding trials. Treatments: Cc C. riparius contaminated, Dc D. rerio contaminated, Bc Both (fish and chironomids) contaminated, n = 3, bars represent means ± SD

At a nominal concentration of 6 μg/l CHP the burrowing behaviour of exposed C. riparius was significantly changed compared to the burrowing behaviour of nonexposed chironomids before the introduction of the fish. Here the number of C. riparius remaining completely at the sediment surface was significantly increased (Wilcoxon Cc P = 0.0495, Bc P = 0.0495) (Fig. 2). Consequently, a significantly decreased number of larvae were partly and fully burrowed compared to the number of unexposed chironomids. No significant difference occurred between the burrowing behaviour of unexposed (Control vs Dc) or exposed chironomids (Cc vs Bc), respectively. After the introduction of zebrafish, a majority of the surviving C. riparius in the control and the Bc treatment were burrowed in the sediment (Fig. 3). Compared to that, the number of exposed C. riparius (Cc and Bc) at the surface was significantly increased (Fig. 3) (Wilcoxon Cc P = 0.037, Bc P = 0.037).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Percentage of chironomids at the sediment surface, partly burrowed and totally burrowed before introducing the zebrafish. Fish and/or chironomids were exposed to 6 μg/l CHP for 2 h prior to the feeding trials. Treatments: Cc C. riparius contaminated, Dc D. rerio contaminated, Bc Both (fish and chironomids) contaminated. * Significantly different to the control, P < 0.05, n = 3, bars represent means ± SD

Fig. 3
figure 3

Percentage of chironomids at the surface, partly burrowed and totally burrowed after introducing zebrafish. Fish and/or chironomids were exposed to 6 μg/l CHP prior to the feeding trials. Treatments: Cc C. riparius contaminated, Dc D. rerio contaminated, Bc Both (fish and chironomids) contaminated. * Significantly different to the control, P < 0.05, n = 3, bars represent means ± SD

Comparing the number of burrowed chironomids before and after the introduction of the fish, significantly more animals were completely burrowed in the control treatment, as well as in the treatments where only the zebrafish were contaminated (Wilcoxon, P = 0.046 and P = 0.046, respectively) (Figs. 2 and 3). However, in the treatment where only the chironomids were exposed a significantly less number of animals was burrowed (Wilcoxon, P = 0.037) and when both, predator and prey, were exposed, no significant difference in the number of burrowed chironomids before and after the introduction of the fish was observable.

At the 6 μg/l CHP level the feeding rate of nonexposed D. rerio on exposed C. riparius was significantly increased compared towards the control (Wilcoxon P = 0,0495) (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Feeding rate [%] of Danio rerio on larval chironomids after 2 h. Fish and/or chironomids were exposed to 6 μg/l CHP for 2 h prior to the feeding trials. Treatments: Cc C. riparius contaminated, Dc D. rerio contaminated, Bc Both (fish and chironomids) contaminated. * Significantly different to the control, P < 0.05, n = 3, bars represent means ± SD

Discussion

The integrity of ecosystems can be influenced by stressors on many different levels. Most studies have focused on the direct effects of contaminants on single species. For example it is known that CHP acts on the nervous system as an inhibitor of the enzyme acetylcholinesterase (Kamrin 1997). The toxicity of chlorpyrifos has been mainly assessed during the early life stages of zebrafish (e.g. Kienle et al. 2009; Levin et al. 2003, 2004; Roex et al. 2002; Scheil and Köhler 2009), where effectively imparing concentrations were 10 μg/l for locomotor activity and 250 μg/l for morphological abnormalities (Kienle et al. 2009). For adult freshwater fish, 96 h LC50 values ranged from 9 μg/l for adult rainbow trout to 331 μg/l for fathead minnow (Kamrin 1997; U.S.-EPA 1986). The effects of chlorpyrifos on chironomids have been assessed in various studies (Ankley et al. 1994; Belden and Lydy 2000; Callaghan et al. 2001; Fisher et al. 2000; Jin-Clark et al. 2002; Lydy et al. 1999; Moore et al. 1998; Schuler et al. 2005). For Chironomus tentans effective concentrations, for the single substance, were found to be at 0.3 μg/l (48 h LC50) (Moore et al. 1998) and 0.07 μg/l (10 d LC50) (Ankley et al. 1994), The EC50 for abnormal swimming movements was 0.39–0.49 μg/l for chlorpyrifos (Belden and Lydy 2000; Jin-Clark et al. 2002). In the present study, the observed effects on predator prey relationship between zebrafish and chironomids can presumably be attributed to the neurotoxic mode of action of chlorpyrifos resulting in behavioural changes.

In the present study interactions between representatives of two trophic levels and different habitats, chironomids as benthic detritus feeders and fish as pelagic secondary consumers, were investigated. The main exposure route for aquatic ecosystems is pesticide spray drift or runoff after a rain event following pesticide application. Therefore in stream systems, mainly short-time pollutant pulses occur. Regarding sediment exposure, chlorpyrifos exhibits a high affinity to sediments and a potential adsorption to sediment particles should not be omitted (Gilliom et al. 2006; Kamrin 1997). In such a situation, chironomids might be exposed even longer. Our study simulated pulse exposures both at low and high doses. Concentrations of up to 0.3 μg CHP/l water have been measured in aquatic systems (Gilliom et al. 2006). Schulz (2001) detected maximum chlorpyrifos concentrations of 924 μg/kg CHP in the sediment after a single rainstorm event in the Lourens River, South Africa, whereas concentrations in the water were only 0.2 μg/l CHP, which indicates, that CHP rapidly binds to sediment and therefore might pose a high risk for sediment inhabiting organisms such as chironomids as well as for organisms feeding on them.

When examining the burrowing behaviour of chironomids and the foraging behaviour of zebrafish exposed to 1 μg/l CHP, neither the natural behaviour of C. riparius nor the feeding rate of the fish seemed to be impaired by the pollutant in our study. This might result from the low concentration and short exposure time of these organisms to CHP. The highest tested CHP concentration of 6 μg/l could occur in water after a rain event following pesticide application, as high concentrations of CHP can be expected over a short period of time (pulse pollution). At this concentration (6 μg/l), CHP impaired the ability of the exposed chironomids to show natural burrowing behaviour. In these treatments a major part of the chironomids stayed at the sediment surface instead of burrowing. Therefore, they seemed to be better detectable and more easily preyed upon by the unexposed D. rerio (Cc) (Fig. 4). Accordingly, our first hypothesis (‘Exposed chironomids are burrowing less than control animals, and are therefore more susceptible to predation by fish.’) was verified. In choice-experiments, Hershey (1987) found that predators consistently selected chironomids which spent more time out of their tube.

In the treatments with non-exposed chironomids (control and Dc), the introduced fish seemed to trigger an increase in burrowing behaviour. The proportion of chironomids at the sediment surface was almost reduced to zero (Fig. 3). Such a behaviour has been observed with chironomid larvae exposed to fish-borne chemical cues (kairomones) simulating increasing predator densities (Hölker and Stief 2005). It can be assumed that those chironomids which had burrowed escaped from the presence of fish and survived. In the treatment with only zebrafish exposed, the feeding rate as well as the number of burrowed chironomids resembled that in the control treatment (Fig. 3). Thus, our second hypothesis (‘Predation by fish leads to increased burrowing behaviour of chironomids in exposed as well as control animals.’) was, in part, proven true. This is due to the fact that significantly more chironomids were burrowed in the control and the Dc treatment after having been exposed to fish, compared to the situation without fish (Figs. 2 and 3). The significantly reduced number of animals burrowed in the Cc treatment, after fish predation, indicates an easier capture of those animals by fish. This might be interpreted as a result of a reduced ability to burrow presumably due to increased convulsions caused by the effects of chlorpyrifos on the nervous system. In the Bc treatment the chironomids did not (or were not able to) change burrowing behaviour due to fish predation as no significant difference in burrowed animals could be observed. Therefore finally, our third hypothesis (‘When exposing predator and prey, the decreased ability of the predator to recognize the prey and of the prey to burrow are outweighed, resulting in no significant differences in feeding rate compared to the control’) could be proven. This was confirmed by the similar feeding rate of zebrafish in control treaments and in the Bc treatment. Similar results were obtained when investigating predator–prey relationships between two amphibian species under insecticide exposure (Bridges 1999). Here predation rates did not differ from those under natural conditions when pre-exposing both, predator and prey, simultaneously.

In the literature, chironomids have been found to be an important prey object to several fish species (Forsyth and James 1988). It is known that the densities of chironomids can respond to fish predation (Gilinsky 1984). In conclusion, the results from our study imply that the biocoenosis of aquatic ecosystems might be indirectly affected due to pollutant exposure.

The effect concentration for Chironomids in our study is an order of magnitude higher compared to earlier studies with C. tentans exposed to chlorpyrifos, where effective concentrations of 0.3 μg/l (48 h LC50), 0.07 μg/l (10 d LC50) and 0.39–0.49 μg/l (96 h EC50 for abnormal swimming movements) were observed (Ankley et al. 1994; Belden and Lydy 2000; Jin-Clark et al. 2002; Moore et al. 1998). However, our results can be considered as even more relevant due to the short exposure time and the integrative parameters observed. The same is true for chlorpyrifos exposure to zebrafish, where subchronic effects on locomotor activity were visible at 10 μg/l (LOEC after 5 d exposure, Kienle et al. 2009) and chronic effects on response latency and spatial discrimination of adult zebrafish occurred after early life stage exposure to 0.1 μg/l chlorpyrifos for 5 days (Levin et al. 2003). Our effective concentration is again one order of magnitude higher than the one mentioned above, but with a much shorter exposure time, which might explain the deviation of the effect concentrations. Another possible reason might be differences in the experimental setup and the addressed endpoints (response latency and spatial discrimination vs feeding rate). In a previous study, the predator avoidance behaviour of chironomids in reaction to kairomones of predatory fish (Rutilus rutilus) did influence mineralization processes of organic matter (Stief and Hölker 2006). This indicates that predator–prey interactions have an impact even on ecosystem function. Our results suggest that simple single species ecotoxicity tests do not reflect adequately potential effects of a toxin on ecosystem structure and function. Up to now the relevance of predator–prey interactions has not been considered in chemical risk assessment, with the exception of conducted mesocosm studies. Our study shows the relevance of the mentioned problem and also proposes a simple method to quantify the effects of a toxic compound, CHP, on interactions between predator and prey organisms.