Background

Stigma is defined as the devalued status that society attaches to a condition or attribute [1]. Stigma also refers to the socially constructed meanings associated with an attribute (labeled “perceived” or “felt” stigma). Thus, HIV stigma can be thought of as socially-shared knowledge about the devalued status of people living with HIV (PLHIV). Particularly among people not infected with HIV, stigma is manifest via prejudice, rejection, discrediting, stereotyping and discrimination mechanisms aimed toward people perceived to have HIV and the individuals and groups with whom they are associated, which may result in distancing oneself from PLHIV [25]. Although stigma toward PLHIV may be held by individuals, collectively, stigma is developed through a shared social process of labeling, stereotyping, lowering status, and discriminating [6] against PLHIV; this process occurs within the social power structure that negatively associates HIV infection with gender, class, race, and sexuality-based inequalities [7, 8].

UNAIDS identifies HIV stigma as one of the key social factors driving the spread of HIV and hindering the treatment of PLHIV [9]. The negative impact of stigma on HIV prevention and treatment for PLHIV is well known [7, 10] and serves to isolate PLHIV socially and physically [11]. The effect of stigma on HIV prevention and treatment is particularly salient in China, where in 2011 it was estimated that 57 % of those with HIV were unaware of their serostatus even though efforts to improve testing rates are a national priority [12]. Of those who do know their status, PLHIV who perceive and encounter stigmatizing attitudes avoid treatment services [13] and hide their serostatus from sexual partners and medical staff [1416], both because they fear discrimination at the hands of health staff and because they fear the social consequences for themselves and their families if their status becomes known or suspected [17]. Similarly, HIV-related stigma may act as a barrier to participation in HIV prevention programming, even among individuals who are not HIV positive [18, 19].

Numerous studies have documented the general public’s negative attitudes toward PLHIV in both rural and urban areas of China [2023]. Commonly reported stigmatizing attitudes in China include, for example, that it is not safe for someone with HIV to take care of other people’s children, that PLHIV persons should be isolated, that PLHIV bring shame to their families, and that seropositive persons deserve to be punished [21, 22, 24]. These beliefs persist even among (presumably better informed) Chinese health care providers, who exhibit generally low levels of support for PLHIV [2527]. Stigma contributes to these attitudes, as some providers reported feeling stigma and shame as a result of being associated HIV patients [26], although providers with increased empathy had reduced avoidance of PLHIV, which improved the quality of their care [28].

According to Herek [29], there are two main components of stigma: (1) the instrumental stigma which results from fear of contracting HIV; and (2) the symbolic stigma which results from the socially derived values that form the moral judgment against behaviors and people associated with HIV infection. With regards to instrumental stigma, most of the Chinese stigma studies show that accurate understanding of the main routes of HIV transmission is generally widespread; however misconceptions persist regarding the rarity of casual transmission and people use these beliefs as reasons to fear PLHIV [24]. Many people tend to overestimate the contagiousness of HIV [17], and this concern about HIV transmission translates into avoiding interactions with PLHIV [30]. Importantly, individuals who believe that casual contact can transmit HIV are much more likely to hold stigmatizing attitudes [20, 23]. For symbolic stigma, evidence that individuals blame PLHIV for their infection continues, primarily as a moral judgment against the behaviors associated HIV infection. Some studies in China have found that blame and rejection of PLHIV is distinct from any fears of infection, and this stigma results from social and cultural values [31, 32]. Chinese PLHIV who were infected through blood plasma donation are not “blamed” for having acted in a risky way that led to their infection; instead, fear of infection from, but not judgment of, the PLHIV acts as the main driver of stigma [11].

Efforts to assess social- and individual-level HIV stigma, both perceived by PLHIV and enacted by HIV negative individuals, have proliferated in recent years (see [7] for a systematic review of this literature). We undertook this study to explore the beliefs and attitudes of the general population in Liuzhou in order to reveal the stigmatizing perceptions held toward PLHIV and the HIV epidemic. Liuzhou is a city located in Guangxi province in southwest China, and given its proximity to the drug trafficking routes in Vietnam and Southeast Asia, it was among the first regions to experience the HIV epidemic. The first HIV case was identified in Liuzhou in 1996. Between 1996 and 2007, injection drug use (IDU) accounted for the majority of transmission events. By 2012, the cumulative number of HIV cases in Liuzhou was 11,323 (unpublished data from China’s information system for disease control and prevention). While drug use continues to be a significant problem in Liuzhou, heterosexual transmission is now the main route of infection. The Liuzhou CDC conducts targeted surveillance of high-risk groups each year. In 2008, their surveillance data reported HIV prevalence as 15.6 % among IDU, 2.9 % among men who have sex with men, and 0.5 % among female sex workers [33]. In the following year, their surveillance data reported 21 % prevalence among IDU [34] and 2.3 % among female sex workers [35]. By 2012, heterosexual contact accounted for the vast majority (89 %) of new infections in Liuzhou, compared to injection drug use (6 %) and men who have sex with men (1 %) (unpublished data from China’s information system for disease control and prevention). In response, Liuzhou has established multi-sectorial working groups to address prevention, education, and treatment interventions for high-risk groups, funded in part by national and international agencies and grants.

As China continues to commit to its “Four Frees and One Care” program to identify PLHIV and provide comprehensive care and treatment services [12, 36], it is vital to characterize HIV stigma in locations like Liuzhou, which has the potential to compromise the impact of these programs. This is particularly important as the risk of HIV infection becomes more prevalent among the general population where otherwise little is known about how people not infected with HIV perceive and react to PLHIV. We report on stigma findings from a population survey conducted in Luizhou. The purpose of the study is to gain greater understanding of factors associated with the stigma attitudes and beliefs held by Liuzhou residents in order to develop better targets for stigma reduction efforts. This research specifically addresses two extreme stigmatizing beliefs in the Liuzhou general population related to symbolic and instrumental stigma and explores the factors associated with greater stigmatizing attitudes.

Methods

Study Design

The work presented in this manuscript was a sub-study of the R24 Partnership for Social Science Research on HIV/STI in China. We conducted this secondary analysis using survey data collected in urban Liuzhou in June and July 2008. The overall purpose of the survey was to provide background information for other HIV-related studies sponsored by the R24 collaboration, which focused on Liuzhou’s high HIV risk populations. The region is hard-hit by the HIV epidemic and was selected as a demonstration site for the collaborative R24 research and training grant between the University of North Carolina-Chapel Hill, Renmin University in Beijing, and the National Center for STD Control in Nanjing, China.

The 2008 Liuzhou sampling design and survey were adapted from the 2006 Chinese Health and Family Life Survey (CHFLS), a nationally representative survey of Chinese adults focused on sexual partnerships, behaviors, and attitudes, which are linked to STI and HIV risk [37]. The primary and secondary sampling units were selected with probability proportional to size. Eight urban neighborhoods (jiedao) in Liuzhou were randomly selected as the primary sampling unit, followed by random selection of three residence committees (juweihui) in each of the eight neighborhoods as the secondary sampling unit, amounting to 24 residence committees. We randomly selected households using the household registration lists in each residence committee. Then, individual respondents (n = 1209) who appeared on the residence committee list as permanent residents or as migrants registered as temporary residents, and who were at least 18 years old, were invited to participate. The audio computer assisted self-interview (ACASI) survey was completed by 852 respondents (70.5 % response rate) ages 18–61 at a private venue to preserve the confidentiality of the respondents. The Renmin University of China IRB approved the study.

Study Setting

Liuzhou is an industrial city that serves as a transportation hub for southwest China. The city has several industrial and high-tech development zones and is a major tourist center. While the survey was conducted in Liuzhou’s four urban districts, the city also includes six surrounding rural counties. The total urban population is approximately 1.3 million, about 300,000 of whom are migrants.

Measures

Main Outcome Measures

With permission of Lee and colleagues [22], we used two stigma questions from their study conducted with market stall employees and owners in an Eastern coastal city in China. The two items were answered on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = not sure, 4 = agree, 5 = strongly agree)Footnote 1: (1) people with HIV must have done something bad and should be punished, and (2) people with HIV should be quarantined. Throughout the analysis, these outcomes are referred to as “punishment” and “isolation” stigma, respectively. We chose these items because they capture severe aspects of stigma, which would most likely impede HIV testing and disclosure of serostatus. Further, these items have the potential to represent two key components of the stigma construct [29]: (1) the punishment item representing symbolic stigma—the expression of moral judgment and blame against the behaviors associated with HIV infection (e.g., promiscuity, homosexuality, drugs, and commercial sex); and (2) the isolation item representing instrumental stigma—the expression of fear of casual HIV infection.

Protective and Risk Factors

Based on the literature, we identified factors potentially related to HIV stigma, including demographic and socioeconomic characteristics, HIV knowledge and risk behaviors, and media measures. The following variables were tested as factors associated with stigmatizing attitudes: number of lifetime sexual partners (0, 1, 2, 3 or more), tested for HIV (yes, no), number of days of reading the newspaper in a week, number of hours of TV watching in a day, and internet use (never use, sometimes use, and often use). We created a measure of HIV transmission knowledge by asking respondents if sharing utensils and sneezing can transmit HIV; respondents were categorized as those who believe that both sneezing and sharing utensils cannot transmit HIV (know all), those who believe that either sneezing or sharing utensils can transmit HIV (know some), and those who believe that both sneezing and sharing utensils can transmit HIV (know none). A history of a previous sexually transmitted infection (yes, no) was determined for respondents who reported ever having syphilis, gonorrhea, chlamydia, herpes, genital warts, or trichomoniasis. We hypothesized that more HIV knowledge and media use would be protective factors associated with less stigma, while indicators of greater sexual risk behavior, such as more sexual partners or history of an STI diagnosis, would be risk factors associated with more stigma. We included the socio-demographic variables gender, age, location of permanent household registration (urban, rural), education level (less than high school, high school graduate, more than high school), employment status (full-time worker, not employed, retired, temporary worker), has children (yes, no), and has a current partner (yes, no) as control variables.

Analysis

First, we calculated descriptive statistics of the sample by gender. We conducted simple ordinal logistic regression analysis to identify bivariate correlations of factors associated with punishment and isolation stigma. Adjusted odds ratios (AORs) and their 95 % confidence intervals (CIs) were calculated; variables in the multivariate ordinal logistic regression model included those associated with stigma in the bivariate analysis or hypothesized a priori. All regression analyses controlled for clustering and were weighted to be representative of the age and gender distribution of Liuzhou [38]. The analyses were performed using SAS 9.2 (SAS Institute Inc.) and findings were presented using unweighted counts and weighted percentages.

Results

Characteristics of the population sample are summarized in Table 1, by gender, to document differences between females and males. The weighted sample includes slightly more males (52 %) than females. The average age was 38 years. Over 20 % of the respondents for this survey, which was conducted in urban districts, held a rural permanent household registration, which implies that they were rural-to-urban migrants, and appeared on the residence committee list as “temporary residents”. More than a third (38 %) of the sample had not completed high school, while 39 % had graduated from high school and 24 % had continued their studies beyond high school. Forty-six percent of respondents knew that neither sneezing nor sharing utensils can transmit HIV, 20 % thought either sneezing or sharing utensils can transmit HIV, and 34 % thought that both can transmit HIV. All but 7 % of the sample had at least one sexual partner in their lifetime. Twelve percent reported a history of an STI and 8 % had been tested for HIV. Eighty-four percent reported having a current partner and 33 % did not have children. In general, media use was high. The median number of days reading the newspaper in a week was 4.0, the median number of hours of TV watched in a day was 2.9, and 39 % reported using the internet often. There were few significant differences between females and males. Males (55 %) were more likely to have full-time employment than females (39 %), while females were more likely to be retired (16 versus 7 %) or not employed (10 versus 5 %). Females had fewer lifetime sexual partners than males. Males had greater internet use than females.

Table 1 Characteristics of a random population sample of adults interviewed in Guangxi Province, China

Survey respondents held higher isolation stigmatizing attitudes than punishment stigmatizing attitudes (Table 2). Forty percent of the sample reported high or very high isolation stigma, while 18 % of the sample reported high or very high punishment stigma. Punishment stigma was positively correlated with isolation stigma (r = 0.46, p < 0.01).

Table 2 Distribution of isolation and punishment stigma responses toward people living with HIV (PLHIV) in Guangxi province, China

We conducted simple ordinal logistic regression analyses to determine the HIV risk factors and protective behaviors associated with HIV punishment and isolation stigma. In the bivariate analysis, greater age, less education, non-full time work, less HIV transmission knowledge, more lifetime sexual partners, having a history of STI, less newspaper reading and TV watching, and less internet use were significantly associated with an increased odds of punishment stigma (Table 3). Older age, rural residence, less education, temporary employment, less HIV transmission knowledge, having a history of STI, never testing for HIV, less newspaper reading, and less internet use was significantly associated with increased odds of isolation stigma (Table 4).

Table 3 Bivariate and multivariate ordinal logistic regression models predicting punishment stigma among adults ages 18–61 in Guangxi Province, China
Table 4 Bivariate and multivariate ordinal logistic regression models predicting isolation stigma among adults ages 18–61 in Guangxi Province, China

The results of multivariate ordinal logistic regression models testing the associations with the punishment (Table 3) and isolation stigma (Table 4) outcomes were controlled for age, gender, education, urban registration, and employment. When fit into the multivariate model, many of the factors that were significant in the simple model were no longer significant when controlling for other predictors. Overall, having the knowledge to reject HIV transmission myths served as the adjusted covariate with the strongest and most consistent negative association with both punishment and isolation stigma. The effect of HIV transmission knowledge was incremental; the odds ratio of punishment stigma were attenuated to 0.67 (95 % CI = 0.46–0.96) for those with some and 0.52 (95 % CI = 0.38–0.71) for those with all HIV knowledge, and the odds ratio of isolation stigma were attenuated to 0.63 (95 % CI = 0.44–0.90) for respondents with some and 0.38 (95 % CI = 0.28–0.51) for respondents with all HIV knowledge. Older age increased the odds of punishment (AOR 1.03; 95 % CI = 1.01–1.04) and isolation stigma (AOR 1.02; 95 % CI = 1.01–104).

A higher number of lifetime sexual partners was significantly associated with punishment stigma; for respondents with three or more sexual partners in their lifetime, the odds of very high punishment stigma are 3.24 times (95 % CI = 1.41–7.43) as likely as the odds of responding to a lesser category of punishment stigma. Full-time work status was protective for punishment stigma; those who did not work (AOR 2.34; 95 % CI = 1.33–4.12) and those who worked on temporary contracts (AOR 1.74; 95 % CI = 1.28–2.38) were more likely to endorse higher punishment stigma. A history of STI was a risk for greater isolation stigma (AOR 1.65; 95 % CI = 1.11–2.43) but not for punishment stigma (AOR 1.48; 95 % CI = 0.99–2.20). Notably, having a history of testing for HIV was not significantly associated with punishment or isolation stigma in the multivariate models.

Discussion

The levels of endorsement for the punishment and isolation stigma items are quite high in this representative sample of the general population of Liuzhou, with 18 % of the general population sample supporting the belief that PLHIV deserved to be punished for their infection and 40 % agreeing that PLHIV should be isolated from others through quarantine. PLHIV living in Liuzhou feel the effects of both the symbolic and instrumental stigmatizing attitudes, which have a substantial impact on their lives. For example, stigma can lead to the loss of social and emotional support of their families, friends and significant others who are afraid of HIV infection and associating with someone who is living with HIV; to the loss of employment despite the desire to work, which affects their income and in turn leads to the inability to afford their treatment; and to self-isolation and negative mental health consequences [24]. The levels of stigma we found were similar to general population samples in other settings in China. For example, in their 2002 article, Derlega and colleagues [39] found that 53 % of a sample of the general population in a province in Southwestern China endorsed isolation stigma.

The higher prevalence of isolation stigma than punishment stigma suggests that more of the stigma against PLHIV is driven by fear of infection, rather than the moral judgment reflected by the punishment item. Further, although the two stigma items are correlated, the associated risk behaviors do not entirely overlap, so there are likely slight differences in the processes that drive how these stigmatizing attitudes are held. While a history of STI was associated with isolation stigma, having more sexual partners was associated with punishment stigma. However, what is not clear is the nature of the relationship between different risk behaviors and punishment and isolation stigma. Because of the social processes that drive stigma, it is likely that navigating sexual relationships and dealing with the potential consequences of risk behaviors associated with contracting HIV influence the stigmatizing attitudes toward PLHIV. Respondents with a previous STI may report more isolation stigma as way to label and distance themselves from “others” who have the potential to put them at risk for another STI or HIV. The endorsement of punishment stigma could reflect distancing from the stigma targeted inwardly on themselves, i.e., internalized stigma, and the self-blame that they may feel as a result of having multiple sexual partners. Regardless of the mechanism, risk behaviors in this setting are associated with greater stigmatizing attitudes. This confirms other findings that risk behavior is associated with high stigma in China [14, 20]. In a study among market vendors in eastern China that used the same stigma items [22], individuals who had more lifetime sexual partners were more likely to believe that PLHIV should be punished for their infection. Not all evidence points in the same direction, however. Another study found greater HIV risk behaviors among female migrants in Shanghai, such as premarital sex and multiple sexual partners, but less stigma measured through prejudicial attitudes [40]. The equivocation in the evidence describing the associations between risk behavior and stigma lends further strength to the argument that stigma is developed through a complex social process informed by the power structure that exists in a particular social and cultural context [7, 8].

The majority of participants were misinformed about how HIV is spread, believing that HIV is readily transmitted through casual contact. Those with the most knowledge about HIV transmission were at decreased risk of both isolation and punishment stigmatizing attitudes, and even participants with some HIV knowledge held less stigma than those with no HIV knowledge. This finding underscores the link that fear of infection has in driving the stigma process, assuming that our indicators of knowledge signifies a better understanding of the mechanisms of HIV transmission. However, we found that HIV knowledge is not only related with the isolation item, potentially representing instrumental stigma or the fear of infection, but also with the punishment item, potentially representing the symbolic stigma or moral judgment of people affected by HIV. Further research is necessary to understand the underlying social process that is driving the relationship between knowledge and different components of stigma.

While the relationship between HIV stigma and transmission knowledge is well-documented [20, 40], interventions to improve individual-level knowledge alone have not been effective at decreasing HIV stigma [41]. More recent efficacious stigma interventions utilize a multi-faceted approach that broaden HIV knowledge along with other targets, such as providing stigma reduction messages to health care providers using popular opinion leaders [42] or distributing community-level HIV and prevention information to market workers to change their social norms [43]. Further, exposure to media messaging may be one mechanism to decrease misconceptions about how HIV is spread, thereby reducing stigma in the general population. Although marginally significant in our study findings, media use, like the internet, TV, and newspaper, seems to have a slight protective effect on HIV stigma. We did not measure the content of the media messages to which people were exposed. Certainly, this information would be necessary to determine how the media and type of messages shape stigmatizing attitudes in order to tailor the contents better. Media interventions for HIV stigma may prove effective at simultaneously improving HIV knowledge and changing the social norms necessary for communities to support reduced stigma [43, 44].

Our study has limitations. A cross-sectional analysis cannot support causal inferences regarding the nature of the relationship between risk behaviors and isolation and punishment stigma. Asking about more, different types of stigma attitudes toward PLHIV would have captured additional complexity of how stigma is manifest in the study population. Isolation and punishment items do not measure other aspects of the stigmatizing process, such as labeling, stereotyping, and distancing. And, while we argue that the punishment and isolation items capture the symbolic and instrumental stigma constructs, we cannot be sure that is the case. As the HIV epidemic matures and HIV knowledge increases, people may have become more aware of the problems of expressing overt forms of HIV stigma, creating a social desirability bias resulting in the underreporting of stigmatizing beliefs. It is difficult to know how much of this affected our survey results. Like others calling for improved stigma measurement [45, 46], we recommend developing valid and reliable measures in the Chinese context to capture the nuances of these mutable stigmatizing attitudes in general population samples as the HIV epidemic evolves over time. Finally, there were factors that we consider important, but that we were not able to measure, such as the sources of participants’ HIV-related knowledge, the contents of the media to which participants were exposed, and the potential associations between different types of media-messaging and HIV-related knowledge.

Despite these limitations, this study provides evidence that high levels of stigma are held within the general population of Liuzhou, in a sample that is generalizable to the permanent and temporary residents in Liuzhou who are recorded on the residence committee list. The effects of these stigmatizing attitudes have palpable consequences for the physical and mental health of PLHIV living in Liuzhou [24, 47]. Failure to address this stigma in the general population has the potential to undermine China’s commitment to reducing new infections and HIV-related mortality [48]. In developing interventions to reduce stigma among the residents of Liuzhou, it is critical that we not only intervene on the social aspects of stigma that drive moralization against PLHIV, but we must also continue to address how fear of casual contact fuels stigma.