Introduction

Snakes are potential predators of primates, even of those that are primarily arboreal (Cisneros-Heredia et al. 2005; Quintino and Bicca-Marques 2013; Teixeira et al. 2015; Ribeiro-Júnior et al. 2016). Primates’ fear of snakes is acquired by a fast, strong and persistent conditioned learning process (Cook and Mineka 1989; Vitale et al. 1991; Emile and Barros 2009; Kawai and Koda 2016). One hypothesis explaining such quick learning is that recognition of snakes (and the elicitation of adequate responses) was so important for survival during primate evolution that learning to fear snakes is a facilitated response mechanism in primates. Snakes are a stimulus more strongly and persistently conditioned as aversive stimulus than are neutral ones, and snakes appear to have attentional priority in visual search tasks (Öhman 2009). Studies have shown that experimental lesions in early life of specific neural circuits, such as the superior colliculus, impair fast detection of threat responses by primates, as revealed by the lack of fear of a rubber snake in a threat-reward conflict task by young capuchin monkeys (Maior et al. 2011). In Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) an experiment showed an attentional bias toward snakes, with quick detection and rapid fear learning limited to snake visual stimulus (Kawai and Koda 2016). Wild vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus pygerythrus) can easily detect snake skin patterns, even when only a couple of centimeters are visible (Isbell and Etting 2017). In wild white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus) subjects of all ages show anti-predatory behavior towards snake models, but infants show less discrimination between predatory and non-predatory snake models (Meno et al. 2013). In humans (Homo sapiens) the visual system is capable of detecting and reacting to snake visual stimuli quicker than to other fear-related stimuli (Soares et al. 2017). Humans identify and react to a snake visual stimulus more readily than to a spider visual stimulus, even when distracting neutral stimuli are presented (Soares et al. 2014).

According to Isbell’s snake detection theory (Isbell 2006, 2009), snakes were an important evolutionary pressure for the evolution of primates’ visual and perceptual adaptations that increased their ability to detect and quickly react to snakes. Mobbing on snakes by primates usually involves approaching, gathering around, intently observing and harassing the snake, and sometimes vocalizing and attacking it (Crofoot 2012). Vervet monkeys react to snakes with distinct vocalizations and looking to the ground (Seyfarth et al. 1980), and white-faced capuchins usually present mobbing behavior (Perry et al. 2003; Meno et al. 2013), and calls directed at dangerous snakes (Digweed et al. 2005).

Isbell (2006, 2009) also argues that the pressure of venomous snakes was different during the evolution of platyrrhine and catarrhine primates. Since the various genera of platyrrhines had already differentiated by the time venomous snakes arrived in America, platyrrhines present more variability in the ability to detect and respond to snakes than catarrhines. In addition, since platyrrhines had less time to evolve under the pressure of venomous snakes, their visual system may not be as capable of detecting snakes; this might explain why platyrrhines are less terrestrial than catarrhines (Isbell 2006, 2009). However, studies concerning wild primates’ encounters with snakes are still limited (McGrew 2015).

Although several primate species frequently hunt and eat vertebrates (Butynski 1982), they almost never eat snakes, possibly because of the life-threatening risk involved. In fact, we found only three reports of primate snake predation: one by wild Tarsius bancanus preying on Calliophis intestinalis (Niemitz 1973), and two by lion tamarins (Leontopithecus sp.), one captive Leontopithecus chrysomelas and one reintroduced Leontopithecus rosalia, that preyed on coral snakes (Pissinatti et al., in press). Both tamarins died as a consequence of envenomation. To our knowledge, humans are the only primate that regularly reverse this predator–prey relationship, and include snakes in their diet (Headland and Greene 2011).

We report here a compendium of 6 years of fieldwork, with several observations of snake interactions, including predation and consumption, with different species of snakes, by four groups of bearded capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) from two wild populations living in northeastern Brazil. Our hypothesis is that Sapajus monkeys, due to their cognitive capabilities, omnivorous diet, opportunistic foraging, high behavioral plasticity (Fragaszy et al. 2004) and having the most catarrhine-like vision (Isbell 2009), would be able to discriminate dangerous (capable of causing great harm or death) from non-dangerous snakes (capable of causing no or little harm). We predicted that capuchin monkeys would:

  1. 1.

    Mob dangerous snakes more often than non-dangerous snakes.

  2. 2.

    Kill and consume non-dangerous snakes more often than dangerous ones.

  3. 3.

    Present a sex/age difference, with adult capuchins, especially males, being more involved than juveniles in snake consumption since it is a potentially dangerous activity, with youngsters being more involved in scrounging pieces of snake killed by older individuals.

Methods

Study sites

We collected data on four bearded capuchin monkey groups living in two distinct study sites. Both sites are located in the south of Piauí State, Brazil, and are 320 km apart. Serra da Capivara National Park (SCNP; 8°50′S, 42°33′W) is the location of the Pedra Furada (PF) and Bocão (BC) neighboring groups. The climate is semi-arid, with dry bush vegetation (Caatinga biome), and a long annual dry season from May to October/November (Falótico and Ottoni 2013).

Fazenda Boa Vista (FBV; 9°39′36″S, 45°25′10″W) is the location of the Chicão (CH) and Zangado (ZA) groups. FBV is a semiarid Cerrado-Caatinga ecotone, where rainfall is seasonally distributed, with a clear dry season from May to September and a wet season from October to April (Spagnoletti et al. 2012).

There are reports from both sites on the presence of snakes, some of them posing a threat to capuchin monkeys by predation or accidental envenomation (Rodrigues and Prudente 2011; Cavalcanti et al. 2014; Pessis et al. 2014; see Supplementary material 1). Details on studied groups, locality, and hours of observation are reported in Table 1.

Table 1 Group, study site, observation time, research period, group size range, number and sex ratio of adults and main data source

Data collection

We recorded all occurrences of encounters between snakes and capuchin monkeys (Martin and Bateson 2007) during the data collection phase of several different behavioral studies (see Table 1) (Falótico and Ottoni 2016; Verderane et al. 2013; Mendonça-Furtado et al. 2014; Spagnoletti et al. 2011). For each event, we recorded time and day, subject(s) involved (and/or age and sex), type of snake (visually identifying species when possible), type of interaction (capuchins killing and eating snakes or behaving defensively), and the monkeys’ behavioral responses (prey sharing with group members, in the predation case, or mobbing behavior toward a snake, in the defense case). We used Fisher’s exact tests (two-tailed, α-value 0.05) to compare events by sex and age.

Results

Snakes as prey

Three out of the four studied groups were observed preying on snakes, and altogether there were 23 recorded events (Table 2; Fig. 1; Supplementary material 2). In 11 of them we identified the snake by sight, before the monkeys ate or took it away. All 11 were non-venomous colubrids.

Table 2 Capuchin monkey groups, events and rates of predation on snakes, sex [M, F, unknown sex (U)] and age-class [adult (A), juvenile (J)] of the monkeys involved, and percentage of scrounging observed after predation on snakes for each group at both study sites
Fig. 1
figure 1

a Snake (unknown species) consumption by an adult male (Teimoso), observed by an adult female with her infant. b An adult female eating a snake (unknown species). Photos taken at Fazenda Boa Vista by Noemi Spagnoletti (a) and Luciano Candisani (b)

Snake predation was performed more by adult males (16 of 23 events for all groups, 70%), but the difference was only significant for the ZA group (p = 0.007) after controlling the expected values by group sex ratio. Females and juveniles were also recorded killing and eating snakes (Table 2; Supplementary material 3).

In nine of 23 of the predation events (39%), we also observed scrounging from group members. Eleven out of the 15 scroungers (73%) were infants and juveniles (Supplementary material 3). However, this age difference (adults/subadults vs. juveniles/infants) in scrounging events was not statistically significant (p = 0.449).

Snakes as danger

We recorded 35 snake encounters in which capuchin monkeys were alarmed and did not prey on the snake (Table 3; Supplementary material 4, 5).

Table 3 Snake mobbing events observed in the four studied groups of the two populations (SCNP and FBV), and the snake encounter rate

We could identify 25 of 35 snakes (71%) that were attended to but not eaten, and 20 of those identified were potentially dangerous (Table 3; Fig. 2). The monkeys responded mainly to dangerous snakes (96%; 24 of 25 events) by vocalizing, shaking and dropping branches, threatening, and even poking them with sticks [poking with sticks occurred only in the PF group in two events out of 13 (Falótico and Ottoni 2014); see Supplementary material 4]. Threat displays were mostly collective: 91% (n = 35) of the observed events involved mobbing by an average of 7.9 individuals, of all ages (SCNP, 11.3, range 1–40; FBV, 4.6, range 1–16).

Fig. 2a–e
figure 2

Some of the dangerous or mimetic-dangerous snakes encountered by the capuchin monkeys at Serra da Capivara National Park. a Boa constrictor, b coral snake (Micrurus sp.), c Thamnodynastes sp. (a Bothrops mimic), d Spilotes pullatus, e Crotalus durissus. Photos by Tiago Falótico

Mobbing events (Supplementary material 5) were variable in duration (median 17 min, range 1–365 min). Most defensive responses were of short duration. However, two events lasted at least 1 h and involved the entire group, and another, directed at an injured snake, lasted several hours.

Although we did not measure snake length, the snakes that were eaten appeared to be shorter than those that were mobbed. Exceptions were the coral snakes (Micrurus sp.) we observed, which were both small (10–20 cm in length) and dangerous, that the monkeys mobbed and did not eat (see Supplementary material 4). We never witnessed a predation event by any predator on the monkeys.

Discussion

Although events of encounters with dangerous and non-dangerous snakes were rare, bearded capuchin monkeys presented different responses when interacting with these types of snake. When dealing with predatory and potentially dangerous non-predatory snakes (i.e., venomous snakes, such as coral snakes), or with mimics of venomous snakes (i.e., Xenodon merremi and Thamnodynastes sp., genus Bothrops mimics), capuchins displayed mobbing behavior, including threat vocalizations and postures, looking for other monkeys nearby to join the threat (recruiting), and even using sticks to poke the snakes [15% events in the PF group (Falótico and Ottoni 2014)]. In contrast, non-dangerous snakes were readily killed and eaten, with no other behavioral display other than the predatory one.

Predation on snakes

The rate of predation on snakes by PF was double that of the CH and ZA groups. This is probably due by the PF group being the largest group studied. If the PF group had the same group size as the FBV groups, then its estimated snake predation rate would have been 0.26 events/100 h; this value is close to the average values observed for snake predation in FBV groups (0.28 events/100 h). Other factors, such as variation in snake population density, not sampled at this time, could also affect the snake encounter and predation rate. We did not observe snake predation by a BC group that was equally habituated but observed for less time.

Snakes that were killed were often not identifiable because the monkeys took the carcass with them or consumed it before we could identify it. However, in the cases where examination of the remains and/or visual assessment of the snake while it was being consumed was possible, they were clearly non-venomous snakes.

Adult males killed snakes more often (70%) than adult females and juveniles. In other primate species, e.g., chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and baboons (Papio spp.) a bias toward adult males in predatory behavior has also been reported (Butynski 1982; Watts and Mitani 2002; Pruetz et al. 2015). These sex-biased predation rates could be due to males’ risk-prone behavior to obtain high-risk, energetically valuable food, independently of abundance of food. This behavior is predicted by the energetic costs and fitness potential from males’ and females’ different sexual roles, as females have more costs—pregnancy, lactation, and carrying infants—and are risk averse (Fedigan 1990). We did not have enough data to relate rates of predation on snakes to food abundance, thus future research should examine this issue.

Defensive behavior toward snakes

Most defensive behaviors involved the participation of several individuals and were directed largely at venomous snakes. Nevertheless, capuchins also behaved defensively toward two non-venomous snakes, the vine snake (Chironius sp.) in one instance and the chicken snake (Spilotes pullatus) in two instances. Although these snakes are non-venomous, both of them predate small vertebrates (usually rodents); moreover, S. pullatus is large (up to 2.7 m), aggressive (Freitas 2003) and has black and yellow aposematic colors (Fig. 1d) to warn predators.

The use of tools as weapons in mobbing contexts has been rarely observed in capuchins, e.g., using a club to attack a Bothrops snake [wild C. capucinus (Boinski 1988)], or to explore the response of the snake [captive Sapajus sp. (Vitale et al. 1991)]. The SCNP population exhibited the snake poking behavior probably because they regularly use stick tools (Mannu and Ottoni 2009; Falótico and Ottoni 2014), and individuals were capable of generalizing the use of sticks from the usual foraging context to defensive behavior. The FBV population did not use poke sticks, possibly because they do not use stick tools (Cardoso and Ottoni 2016).

Learning

Our data indicate that adult capuchin monkeys respond differently to snakes that can be preyed upon and snakes that can kill them. Several cues may account for this (Isbell 2006). The size of the snake is a good predictor, especially of constrictors. However, small snakes, e.g., coral snakes, can also be dangerous (Pissinati et al., in press). For coral snakes, color (for trichromatic individuals), or contrast (for dichromatic individuals), could be a potential cue (Fig. 1b). Both types of information are available for the detection of S. pullatus, a large and aposematic snake. A third stimulus is the sound that some venomous snakes produce, e.g., the rattling sound of rattlesnakes (Crotalus spp.).

The learning of this discrimination must be fast, especially when dealing with dangerous snakes. Primates have a bias to learn a fear of snakes (Cook and Mineka 1989; Öhman 2009; Maior et al. 2011; Kawai and Koda 2016), and it has been argued that their visual system has evolved in response to the pressure to detect snakes (Isbell 2006). The social and conspicuous nature of capuchins’ defensive behavior toward snakes may provide a good learning context for a young monkey. Our data show that most (91%) of the threatening events were collective. Observing older individuals dealing with dangerous snakes creates the opportunity to associate some characteristics, such as size, skin pattern, coloration and sound of these snakes to the threatening behavior exhibited by expert group members.

Similarly, scrounging and allowing others to share killed snakes, which occurred in at least one-third of predation events, provides information about which snakes to eat, and food reward. Immatures experiencing these opportunities may learn faster and in a less hazardous way than those that do not. An ontogenetic study examining how capuchins learn to distinguish between dangerous versus non-dangerous snakes is crucial to fully appreciate this phenomenon.

Evolution

The capacity to detect snakes appears to be especially developed in primates that have a long evolutionary history of snake predation pressure (Isbell 2006). Isbell’s snake detection theory proposes that (1) different taxonomic groups of primates had different amounts of evolutionary time coexisting with venomous snakes; and that (2) differences in time would affect the behavioral response, making Catarrhini more uniformly “wired” to detect and avoid snakes than Platyrrhini. Our data support Isbell’s view, showing that a New World monkey, although fearful of snakes, learns to discriminate non-dangerous snakes as potential prey.

Isbell also argued that the habitual terrestriality reported in some catarrhines (e.g., baboons, vervets, and patas monkeys) may not occur in platyrrhines because their visual systems in general are not as sensitive as those of catarrhines to snakes. Snakes can be extremely difficult to see, especially on the ground where they may be camouflaged by leaf litter. Thus, platyrrhines may be more hesitant to go onto the ground because they may not be able to detect snakes as well as catarrhines. Interestingly, among the platyrrhines studied to date, capuchins have the most catarrhine-like vision (Isbell 2009), and the groups studied here are among the most terrestrial of known populations of capuchin monkeys (Spagnoletti et al. 2009; Verderane 2010; Falótico 2011). Therefore, the high level of terrestriality could also be a consequence of the ability to detect snakes, although ecological factors can also affect ground use by platyrrhines (Tabacow et al. 2009). Further studies should investigate the level of terrestriality in the genus Sapajus and the ability to prey on terrestrial vertebrates, as well as to discriminate dangerous from non-dangerous snakes.

Overall, our findings support the hypothesis that these populations of S. libidinosus can discriminate between dangerous and non-dangerous snakes and behave accordingly, protecting themselves when necessary, but taking the opportunity to prey on snakes when it is safe to do so. This study shows that capuchin monkeys are, like humans, both predators and prey of snakes. As such, capuchins can offer a non-human primate model for further investigation into the evolution of complex predator–prey relationships in the human lineage.